SHANNON ENTROPY: A POSSIBLE INTRINSIC TARGET PROPERTY
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00791R000200100004-2
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
U
Document Page Count:
18
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
October 27, 1998
Sequence Number:
4
Case Number:
Publication Date:
December 1, 1994
Content Type:
MAGAZINE
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00791R000200100004-2.pdf | 780.14 KB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
SHANNON ENTROPY: A POSSIBLE
INTRINSIC TARGET PROPERTY
BY EDWIN C. MAY, S. JAMES P. SPOTTISWOODE,
AND CHRISTINE L. JAMES
ABSTRACT: We propose that the average total change of Shannon's entropy is a candidate for
an intrinsic target property. An intrinsic target property is one that is completely independent of
psychological factors and can be associated solely with a physical property of the target. We
analyzed the results of two lengthy experiments that were conducted from 1992 through 1993
and found a significant correlation (r, = 0.337, df = 31, t = 1.99, p 5 0.028) with an absolute
measure of the quality of the anomalous cognition (AC). In addition, we found that the quality
of the AC was significantly better for dynamic targets than for static targets (t = 1.71, df = 36, p
5 0.048). The 1993 correlation with the change of entropy replicated a similar finding from our
1992 study. Using monte carlo techniques, we demonstrate that the observed correlations were
not due to some unforeseen artifact with the entropy calculation, but perhaps the correlation
can be accounted for because of the difference in some other measure between static and
dynamic targets. The monte carlo results and the significant correlations with static targets in
the 1992 study, however, suggest otherwise. We describe the methodology, the calculations, and
correlations in detail and provide guidelines for those who may wish to conduct similar studies.
The psychophysical properties of the five known senses are well known
(Reichert, 1992). At the "front end," they share similar properties. For
example, each system possesses receptor cells that convert some form of
energy (e.g., photons for the visual system, sound waves for the audio sys-
tem) into electrochemical signals. The transfer functions are sigmoidal;
that is, there is a threshold for physical excitation, a linear region, and a
saturation level above which more input produces the same output. How
these psychophysical reactions translate to sensational experience is not
well understood, but all the systems do possess an awareness threshold
similar to the subliminal threshold for the visual system.
Since all the known senses appear to share these common properties, it
is reasonable to expect that if anomalous cognition (AC) l is mediated
1The Cognitive Sciences Laboratory has adopted the term anomalous mental phenomena
instead of the more widely known psi. Likewise, we use the terms anomalous cognition and
anomalous perturbation for ESP and PK, respectively. We have done so because we believe
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
2 The Journal of Parapsychology
through some additional "sensory" system, then it, too, should share similar
properties at the preperceptual cellular front end. For example, a putative
AC sensory system should possess receptor cells that have a sigmoidal trans-
fer function and exhibit threshold and saturation phenomena. As far as we
know, there are no candidate neurons in the peripheral systems whose
functions are currently not understood. So, if receptor cells exist, it is likely
that they will be found in the central nervous system. Since 1989, our labo-
ratory has been conducting a search for such receptor sites (May, Luke,
Trask, & Frivold, 1990); that activity continues.
There is a second way in which receptor-like behavior might be seen in
lieu of a neurophysiology study. If either an energy carrier for AC or some-
thing that correlated with it were known, then it might be possible to infer
sigmoidal functioning at the behavioral level as opposed to the cellular
level. Suppose we could identify an intrinsic target property that correlated
with AC behavior. Then, by manipulating this variable, we might expect to
see a threshold at low magnitudes and saturation at high magnitudes.
To construct such an experiment, it is mandatory that we eliminate, as
much as possible, all extraneous sources of variance and adopt an absolute
measure for the AC behavior (Lantz, Luke, & May, 1994). We can reduce
one source of variance by considering what constitutes a good target in an
AC experiment. Delanoy (1988) reported a survey of the literature for
successful AC experiments and categorized the target material according to
perceptual, psychological, and physical characteristics. Except for trends
related to dynamic, multisensory targets, she was unable to observe system-
atic correlations of AC quality with her target categories.
Watt (1988) examined the target question from a theoretical perspective.
She concluded that the "best" AC targets are those that are meaningful,
have emotional impact, and contain human interest. Those targets that
have physical features that stand out from their backgrounds or contain
movement, novelty, and incongruity are also good targets.
In trying to understand these findings and develop a methodology for
target selection for process-oriented research, we have constructed a meta-
phor. Figure 1 shows three conceptual domains that contribute to the vari-
ability in AC experiments. The engineering metaphor of source,
transmission, and detector allows us to assign known contributors to the
variance in specific domains. Without controlling or understanding these
sources, interpreting the results from process-oriented research is problem-
atical, if not impossible.
that these terms are more naturally descriptive of the observables and are neutral in
that they do not imply mechanisms. These new terms will be used throughout this paper.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Shannon Entropy 3
Figure 1. Information-transfer metaphor.
For example, suppose that the quality of an AC response actually de-
pended upon the physical size of a target, and that affectivity was also a
contributing factor. That is, a large target that was emotionally appealing
was reported more often more correctly. Obviously, both factors are impor-
tant in optimizing the outcome; however, suppose we were studying the
effect of target size alone. Then an "attractive" small target might register as
well as a less attractive large target, and the size dependency would be
confounded in unknown ways.
Our metaphor allows us to assign variables, such as these, to specific
elements. Clearly, an individual's psychological response to a target is not an
intrinsic property of a target; rather, it is a property of the receiver.2 Likewise,
size is a physical property of the target and is unrelated to the receiver.
Generally, this metaphor allows us to lump together the psychology, person-
ality, and physiology of the receiver and consider these important factors as
contributors to a detector "efficiency." If it is true that an emotionally ap-
pealing target is easier to sense by some individuals, we can think of them as
more efficient at those tasks. In the same way, all physical properties of a
target are intrinsic to the target and do not depend on the detector effi-
ciency. Perhaps, temporal and spatial distance between target and receiver
are intrinsic to neither the target nor the receiver, but rather to the trans-
mission mechanism, whatever that may be.
2A person's psychological reaction to a target (i.e., "detector" efficiency) is an impor-
tant contributing factor to the total response, as indicated in the references sited above;
however, it is possible to reduce this contribution by careful selection of the target pool
material.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
4 The Journal of Parapsychology
More than just nomenclature, our metaphor can guide us in designing
experiments to decrease certain variabilities in order to conduct meaning-
ful process-oriented research. Some of the methodological improvements
seem obvious. If the research objective is to understand the properties of
AC rather than how an AC ability may be distributed in the population, then
combining results across receivers should be done with great caution. To
understand how to increase high jumping ability, for example, it makes no
sense to use a random sample from the general population as high jumpers;
rather, find a good high jumper and conduct vertical studies (no pun in-
tended). So, too, is it true in the study of AC. We can easily reduce the
variance by asking given receivers to participate in a large number of trials
and not combining their results.
May, Spottiswoode, and James (1994) suggest that by limiting the num-
ber of cognitively differentiable elements within a target, the variance can
also be decreased. A further reduction of potential variance can be realized
if the target pool is such that a receiver's emotional/psychological response
is likely to be more uniform across targets (i.e., reducing the detector vari-
ance as shown in Figure 1).
Having selected experienced receivers and attended to these methodo-
logical considerations, we could then focus our attention on examining
intrinsic target properties. If we are successful at identifying one such prop-
erty, then all process-oriented AC research would be significantly improved,
because we would be able to control a source of variance that is target
specific. The remainder of this paper describes the analysis of two lengthy
studies that provide the experimental evidence to suggest that the average
of the total change of Shannon's entropy may be one such intrinsic target
property.
The methodological details for the two experiments can be found in
Lantz, Luke, and May (1994). In this section, we focus on the target calcula-
tions and the analysis techniques.
Shannon Entropy: A Short Description
Building upon the pioneering work of Leo Szilard (1925/1972,
1929/1972), Shannon and Weaver (1949) developed what is now called
information theory. This theory formalizes the intuitive idea of information
that there is more "information" in rare events, such as winning the lottery,
than in common ones, such as taking a breath. Shannon defined the en-
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Shannon Entropy 5
tropy for a given system as the weighted average of the probability of occur-
rence of all possible events in the system. Entropy, used in this sense, is
defined as a measure of our uncertainty, or lack of information, about a
system. Suppose, for example, we had an octagonal fair die (i.e., each of the
eight sides is equally likely to come up). Applying Equation 1, below, to this
system gives an entropy of three bits, which is in fact the maximum possible
for this system. If, on the other hand, the die were completely biased so that
the same side always came up, the entropy would be zero. In other words, if
each outcome is equally likely then each event has the maximum surprise.
Conversely, there is no surprise if the same side always lands facing up.
In the case of images, a similar analysis can be used to calculate the
entropy. For simplicity, consider a black and white image in which the
brightness, or luminance, of each picture element, or pixel, is measured on
a scale from zero to 255, that is, with an eight-bit binary number. Equation
1 can again be used to arrive at a measure of the picture's entropy. As with
the other sensory systems where gradients are more easily detected, we shall
show that the gradient of Shannon's entropy is correlated with AC perform-
ance far better than the entropy itself.
In other sensory systems, receptor cells are sensitive to incident energy
regardless of "meaning", which is ascribed as a later cognitive function.
Shannon entropy is also devoid of meaning. The pixel analysis ignores
anything to do with cognitive features. From this point of view, a photo-
graph of a nuclear blast is, perhaps, no more Shannon-entropic than a
photograph of a kitten; it all depends on the intensities, which were used to
create the photographs. Thus, it is not possible to give a prescription on
how to chose a high change-in-entropy photograph based on its pictorial
content. Perhaps, after much experience, it may be possible to recognize
good targets from their intensity patterns; at the moment we do not know
how to accomplish this.
Target Calculations
Because of the analogy with other sensory systems, we expected that the
change of entropy would be more sensitive than the entropy alone would
be. The target variable that we considered, therefore, was the average total
change of entropy. In the case of image data, the entropy is defined as:
Nk
Sk = - I Pmk log 2 (Pmk),
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
6 The Journal of Parapsychology
where p,,k is the probability of finding image intensity m of color k. In a
standard, digitized, true color image, each pixel (i.e., picture element)
contains eight binary bits of red, green, and blue intensity, respectively. That
is, Nk is 255 (i.e., 28-1) for each k, k = r, g, b. For color, k, the total change of
the entropy in differential form is given by:
dSk = I vsk . 41 +
as,,
at
The first term corresponds to the change of the entropy spatially across
a single photograph of video frame. Imagine a hilly plane in entropy space;
this term represents the steepness of the slope of the hills (i.e., the change
between adjacent macropixels, as defined below). The second term adds
time changes to the total change. Not only does the entropy change across
a scene, but a given patch of the photograph changes from one scene to the
next. Of course, this term is zero for all static photographs.
We must specify the spatial and temporal resolution before we can com-
pute the total change of entropy for a real image. Henceforth, we drop the
color index, k, and assume that all quantities are computed for each color
and then summed.
To compute the entropy from Equation 1, we must specify empirically
the intensity probabilities, pm. In Lantz, Luke, and May's 1993 experiment,
the targets were all video clips that met the following criteria:
1. Topic homogeneity. The photographs contained outdoor scenes of
settlements (e.g., villages, towns, cities, etc.), water (e.g., coasts, rivers and
streams, waterfalls, etc.), and topography (e.g., mountains, hills, deserts,
etc.).
2. Size homogeneity. Target elements are all roughly the same size. That
is, there are no size surprises such as an ant in one photograph and the
moon in another.
3. Affectivity homogeneity. As much as possible, the targets included
materials thst invoke neutral affectivity.
For static targets, a single characteristic frame from a video segment was
digitized (i.e., 640 x 480 pixels) for eight bits of information of red, green,
and blue intensity. The video image conformed to the NTSC standard as-
pect ratio of 4 x 3, and so we arbitrarily assumed an area (i.e., macropixel)
of 16 x 12 = 192 pixels from which we calculated the pm. Since during the
feedback phase of a trial the images were displayed on a Sun Microsystems
standard 19-inch color monitor, and since they occupied an area approxi-
mately 20 x 15 cm square, the physical size of the macropixels was approxi-
mately 0.5 cm square. Since major cognitive elements were usually not
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Shannon Entropy 7
smaller than this, this choice was reasonable-192 pixels were sufficient to
provide a smooth estimate of the pm.
For this macropixel size, the target frame was divided into a 40 x 40 array.
The entropy for the (ij) th macropixel was computed as:
N-1
Sij = - I pm loge (pm),
m=0
where pm is computed empirically only from the pixels in the (i, 1)
macropixel and m is the pixel intensity. For example, consider the white
square in the upper left portion of the target photograph shown in Figure
2.
Figure 2. City with a mosque.
The green probability distribution for this macropixel (3, 3) is shown in
Figure 3. The probability density and the photograph itself indicate that
most of the intensity in this macropixel is near zero (i.e., no intensity of
green in this case). In a similar fashion, the Stj are calculated for the entire'
scene. Since i and j range from 0 to 40, each frame contains a total of 1,600
macropixels.
We used a standard image processing algorithm to compute the two-di-
mensional spatial gradient for each of the 1,600 macropixels. The first term
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
8 The Journal of Parapsychology
Figure 3. Green intensity distribution for the city target (macropixel 3, 3).
in Equation 2 was approximated by its average value over the image and was
computed by the relations shown as Equations 3.
dsii Sii
IVSij dj I - dx + ndy
'i=(S=+ 1,1+i-Si,j+i)+ 2(S +i-S;,j+i)+(Si-1,j+i-Si-ij-1)
dx
dS=,
"'(Si+l,j+1-Si-l,j+l)+2(Si+l,j -Sd-1, j)+(Si+1,j-1- Se-l,j-1)
dy
The total change of entropy for the dynamic targets was calculated in
much the same way. The video segment was digitized at one frame per
second. The spatial term of Equation 2 was computed exactly as it was for
the static frames. The second term, however, was computed from differ-
ences between adjacent, 1-second frames for each macropixel. Or,
at e t
S2;(t+At)-S2;(t)
At
where At is one over the digitizing frame rate. We can see immediately that
the dynamic targets will have a larger AS than do the static ones because
Equation 4 is identically zero for all static targets.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Shannon Entropy 9
In Lantz, Luke, and May's 1992 experiment, the static targets were digit-
ized from scanned photographs. This difference and its consequence will
be discussed below.
AC Data Analysis
Rank-order analysis in Lantz, Luke, and May's (1994) experiment dem-
onstrated significant evidence for AC; however, this procedure does not
usually indicate the absolute quality of the AC. For example, a response that
is a near-perfect description of the target receives a rank of 1. But a response
that is barely matchable to the target may also receive a rank of 1. Table 1
shows the rating scale that we used to assess the quality of the AC responses,
regardless of their rank.
To apply this subjective scale to an AC trial, an analyst begins with a score
of 7 and determines if the description for that score is correct. If not, then
the analyst tries a score of 6, and so on. In this way the scale is traversed from
7 to 0 until the score-description seems reasonable for the trial.
TABLE 1
0-7 POINT ASSESSMENT SCALE
7 Excellent correspondence, including good analytical detail, with
essentially no incorrect information
6 Good correspondence with good analytical information and relatively
little incorrect information
5 Good correspondence with unambiguous unique matchable
elements, but some incorrect information
4 Good correspondence with several matchable elements intermixed
with incorrect information
3 Mixture of correct and incorrect elements, but enough of the former
to indicate receiver has made contact with the site
2 Some correct elements, but not sufficient to suggest results beyond
chance expectation
1 Little correspondence
0 No correspondence
For all analyses in the 1992 and 1993 studies, we decided a priori to use'
only the upper half of the rating scale. As the strength of the AC functioning
increases, by definition there is less incorrect information (i.e., noise). In
other words, the noise contribution to each score level decreases in some
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200100004-2
10 The Journal of Parapsychology
unknown way as the the AC increases. Thus, we limited the noise contribu-
don by using only the upper half of the scale in the analysis.
ANOMALOUS COGNITION EXPERIMENT-1992
In Lantz, Luke, and May's 1992 experiment there were no significant
interactions between target condition (i.e., static vs. dynamic) and sender
condition (i.e., sender vs. no sender); therefore, they combined the data for
static targets regardless of the sender condition (i.e., 100 trials). The sum-
of-ranks was 265 (i.e., exact sum-of-rank probability of p S 0.007, effect size
= 0.248). The total sum-of-ranks for the dynamic targets was 300 (i.e., p