ADVANCES IN REMOTE-VIEWING ANALYSIS
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
K
Document Page Count:
37
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
May 17, 2000
Sequence Number:
1
Case Number:
Publication Date:
September 1, 1990
Content Type:
HW
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5.pdf | 1.52 MB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Journal of Parapsychology, Vol. 54, September 1990
ADVANCES IN REMOTE-VIEWING
ANALYSIS
BY EDWIN C. MAY, JESSICA M. UTTS,. BEVERLY S. HUMPHREY,
WANDA L. W. LUKE, THANE J. FRIVOLD,. AND VIRGINIA V. TRASK
ABSTRACT: Fuzzy set technology is applied to the ongoing research question of
how to automate the analysis of remote-viewing data. Fuzzy sets were invented to
describe, in a formal way, the. subjectivity inherent in human reasoning. Applied
to remote-viewing analysis, the technique involves a quantitative encoding of target
and response material and provides a formal comparison. In this progress report,
the accuracy of a response is defined as the percent of the intended target material
that is described correctly. The reliability is defined as the percent of the response
that was correct. The assessment of the remote-viewing quality is defined as the
product of accuracy and reliability, called the figure of merit. The procedure is
applied to a test set of six remote-viewing trials. A comparison of the figures of
merit with the subjective assessments of 37 independent analysts shows good
agreement. The fuzzy set technology is also used to provide a quantitative defini-
tion of target orthogonality.
Human analysts are commonly used to evaluate free-response
data. Although there are many variations, the basic idea is that an
analyst, who is blind to the actual result, is presented with a re-
sponse and a number of target possibilities, one of which is the in-
tended target. The analyst's task is to decide what is the best re-
sponse/target match, and frequently includes rank-ordering the
targets from best to worst correspondence with the response. It is
beyond the scope of this report to provide a critical review of the
extensive literature on this topic.
One aspect, however, of this type of evaluation is that analysts
are required to make global judgments about the overall match be-
tween a complex target (e.g., a photograph of a natural scene) and
an equally complex response (e.g., written words and drawings). In
a recent book, Dawes (1988) has discussed various decision algo-
rithms in general and the difficulty with global techniques, such as
those used in rank-order evaluation, in particular.' According to
Dawes, the research results suggest that global decisions of this type
are not as good as those based on smaller subelements that are later
' We are indebted to Professor D. Bem, Cornell University, for directing us to
this valuable source of information.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
194 The Journal of Parapsychology
combined. (See Dawes, 1988, chap. 10, for references to the re-
search.) Humans appear to be capable of deciding what the appro-
priate variables should be in complex decision processes, but they
have proved to be unreliable at combining these variables to arrive
at a single decision. Linear algorithms are consistently better at this
latter task. Therefore, it seems prudent to develop evaluation tech-
niques that are less sensitive to global decision processes and rely on
combinations of more restrictive decisions.
Honorton (1975) has pointed out an additional difficulty inher-
ent in a global rank-order approach. Asking an analyst to rank-
order a small set of target possibilities converts the free-response
experiment into a forced-choice one, at least on the part of the an-
alyst. It is obvious that in doing so, much quantitative information
is lost. For example, a near perfect correspondence between re-
sponse and target will receive only as much "credit" as one that just
barely allowed an analyst to discriminate among the possibilities.
If multiple analysts are used, addition problems arise concerning
interanalyst reliability. If an individual analyst judges a number of
responses in a series, within-analyst consistency becomes an individ-
ual problem.
To address these difficulties, various computer-automated pro-
cedures have been suggested in an attempt to reduce the inter-
analyst reliability while increasing within-analyst consistency. For ex-
amples, see Honorton (1975), Humphrey, May, Trask, and
Thomson (1986), Humphrey, May, and Utts (1988), Jahn, Dunne,
and Jahn (1980), May (1983), May, Humphrey, and Mathews
(1985), and Targ, Puthoff, and May (1977).
In this paper we present the current status of an ongoing re-
search topic. We are not yet ready to propose that the techniques
described here be used for free-response analysis; however, we hope
to inspire the community to develop a proper set of subvariables so
that the problems inherent in global decision processes can be
avoided.
Finally, we present a successful application of the mathematical
techniques for quantifying target orthogonality for a complex target
pool.
Background
Substantial progress has been made in methods for evaluating
remote-viewing experiments since the publication of the initial re-
mote-viewing (RV) effort at SRI International (Puthoff & Targ,
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 195
1976). This paper outlines some of the progress and presents the
details for one particular method.2
Two basic questions are inherent in the analysis of any remote-
viewing data, namely, how is the target defined, and how is the re-
sponse defined.
In a typical outbound RV experiment, definitions of target and
response are particularly difficult to achieve. The protocol for such
an experiment dictates that an experimenter travel to some ran-
domly chosen location at a prearranged time; a viewer's task is to
describe that location. One method of trying to assess the quality of
the RV descriptions in a series of trials is to require that an analyst
visit each of the sites and attempt to match responses to them. While
standing at a site, the analyst has to determine not only the bounds
of the site, but also the site details that are to be included in the
analysis. For example, if the target location was the Golden Gate
Bridge, the analyst would have to determine whether the buildings
of downtown San Francisco, which are clearly and prominently vis-
ible from the bridge, were to be considered part of the target. The
RV response to the Golden Gate Bridge target could be equally
troublesome, because responses of this sort are typically 15 pages of
dream-like free associations. A reasonable description of the bridge
might be contained in the response; it might be obfuscated, how-
ever, by a large amount of unrelated material. How is an analyst to
approach this problem of response definition?
The first attempt at SRI at quantitatively defining an RV re-
sponse involved reducing the raw transcript to a series of declarative
statements called concepts (Targ et al., 1977). Initially, it was de-
cided that a coherent concept should not be reduced to its compo-
nent parts. For example, a small red VW car would be considered a
single concept rather than four separate concepts, small, red, VW,
and car. Once a transcript had been "conceptualized," the list of
concepts constituted, by definition, the RV response. The analyst
rated the concept lists against the sites. Although the response was
well defined by this method, no attempt was made to define the tar-
get site.
In 1982, a procedure was developed to define both the target
and response material (May, 1983). It became evident that before a
site can be qualified, the overall remote-viewing goal must be clearly
defined. If the goal is simply to demonstrate the existence of the
2 Although the term remote viewing is used throughout this paper, the analysis
techniques can easily be applied to any free-response data.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Inn mr I r r i i
RV phenomenon, then anything that is perceived at the site is im-
portant. But if the goal is to gain specific information about the RV
process, then possibly specific items at the site are important
whereas others remain insignificant. -
In 1984, work began on a computerized evaluation procedure
(May et at., 1985), which underwent significant expansion and re-
finement during 1986 (Humphrey et at., 1986). The mathematical
formalism underlying this procedure is known as the "figure of
merit" (FM) analysis. This method is predicated on descriptor list
technology, which represented a significant improvement over ear-
lier "conceptual analysis" techniques, both in terms of "objectifying"
the analysis of RV data and in increasing the speed and efficiency
with which evaluation can be accomplished. Humphrey's technique,
which was based on the pioneering work of Honorton (1975) and
its expansion by Jahn, Dunne, and Jahn (1980), was to encode tar-
get and response material in accordance with the presence or ab-
sence of specific elements.
It became increasingly evident, however, that this particular ap-
plication of descriptor lists was inadequate in providing discrimina-
tors that were "fine" enough to describe a complex target accurately,
and unable to exploit fully the more subtle or abstract information
content of the RV response. To decrease the granularity of the RV
evaluation system, therefore, a new technology would have to allow
the analyst a gradation of judgment about target and response fea-
tures rather than the hard-edged (and rather imprecise) all-or-noth-
ing binary determinations. Requiring an analyst to restrict subjective
judgment to single elements rather than to complete responses is
consistent with the research reported by Dawes (1988).
A preliminary survey of various disciplines and their evaluation
methods (spanning such diverse fields as artificial intelligence, lin-
guistics, and environmental psychology) revealed a branch of math-
ematics, known as "fuzzy set theory."'
Fuzzy Set Concepts
Fuzzy set theory was chosen as the focal point of the RV analyt-
ical techniques because it provides a mathematical framework for
modeling situations that are inherently imprecise. Because- it is such
an important component in the analysis, a brief tutorial will be pre-
sented to highlight its major concepts.
' We wish to thank S. James P. Spottiswoode and D. Graff, CE, for directing us
to the fuzzy set literature and for many helpful discussions.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 197
Membership Value
1 2 3 4 5 6 . 7 8 9 10 ?00 15 09? 30
Population in 100 Thousands
Figure 1. The fuzzy set "kind-of-small" cities.
In traditional set theory (i.e., crisp sets), an element either is or
is not a member of a set. For example, the crisp set of cities with
population equal to or greater than 1,000,000 includes New York
City, but not San Francisco. This set would also not include a city
with a population of 999,999. The problem is obvious. There is no
real difference between cities with populations of 1,000,000 and
999,999, yet one is in the set and the other is not. Humans do not
reason this way; therefore, something other than crisp sets is re-
quired to capture the subjectivity inherent in RV analysis.
Fuzzy set theory introduces the concept of degree of membership.
Herein lies the essence of its applicability to the modeling of impre-
cise concepts. For example, if we consider the size of a city, we
might define certain fuzzy sets, such as very small cities or kind-of-small
cities. Using kind-of-small cities as a fuzzy set example, we might sub-
jectively assert that a city with a population of 100,000 is definitely
such a city, but a city with a population of 400,000 is only a little bit
like a kind-of-small city. As depicted in Figure 1, fuzzy set theory al-
lows us to assign a membership value between 0 and 1 that repre-
sents our best subjective estimate as to how much each of the pos-
sible city populations embodies the concept kind-of-small. In this
example, a population of 700,000 assigned a membership value of
0.3.
Clearly, a different set of membership values would be assigned
to the populations for the fuzzy sets very small cities, medium cities,
large cities, and so forth; a population of 100,000 might receive a
value of 0.2 for very small cities, but a value for 1.0 for kind-of-
small cities, depending on context, consensus, and the particular
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
198 The Journal of Parapsychology
application. These membership values can be obtained through con-
sensus opinion, a mathematical formula, or by several other means.
Crisp sets are special cases of fuzzy sets, in which all membership
values are either zero or one. By using membership values, we are
able to provide manipulatable numerical values for imprecise natu-
ral language expressions; in addition, we are no longer forced into
making inaccurate binary decisions such as, "Is the city of San Fran-
cisco large-yes or no?"
In this example, the crisp set of all cities defines the universal set
of elements (USE). The crisp set of cities with populations of one
million or more is a subset of USE. The fuzzy sets very small, kind-
of-small, medium, and large cities are fuzzy subsets of USE.
Universal Set of Elements
Since targets and the responses will be defined as fuzzy sets, we
must specify a USE. The universal set of elements can be quite gen-
eral and include all aspects of a given target pool, or it can be tai-
lored to a specific experiment to test a given concept (e.g., include
only geometric shapes). Since the method of fuzzy set analysis crit-
ically depends on the choice of USE, we provide one example that
was derived from a target pool used in earlier experiments. What
follows is only an example of how one might construct a USE. The
one we use is not generally applicable to other target pools or other
experiments.
We constructed our USE by including a list of features present
in photographs from the National Geographic magazine with ele-
ments obtained from the RV responses in earlier experiments. This
USE is presented in Appendix A as the actual coding forms. For
the target features, we focused on direct visual elements. (In the
general case, other perceptual dimensions can be considered.) In
the case of the RV response-derived elements, an effort was made
to preserve the vocabulary used by the viewers. Some of the ele-
ments, therefore, are either response-dependent or target-depen-
dent or both, whereas others, particularly at the more abstract lev-
els, appear to be more universal across possible USEs.
This universal set of elements is structured in levels, ranging
from the relatively abstract, information poor (such as vertical lines),
to the relatively complex, information rich (such as churches). The
current system is structured into seven primary and three secondary
levels of elements; the main intent of this structure is to serve as a
heuristic device for guiding the analyst into making judicious con-
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
i
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 199
Crete element assignments based on rather abstract commentary.
The use of levels is advantageous in that each element level can be
weighted separately and used or not, as the case may be. This ena-
bles various combinations of levels to be deployed to identify the
optimal mix of concrete versus abstract elements. Of course, any
such weighting scheme must be determined in advance of any ex-
periment.
The determination as to which elements belonged on which level
was made after consideration of two primary factors: (1) the appar-
ent ability of the viewers to be able to resolve certain features, cou-
pled with (2) the amount of pure information thought to be con-
tained in any given element. Some of these "factor one"
determinations were based on the combined anecdotal experience
of analysts and monitors in the course of either analyzing or con-
ducting numerous RV experiments; some were determined empir-
ically from post hoc analyses of viewers' abilities to perceive various
elements in previous experiments.
The "factor two" determinations were made primarily by arrang-
ing the elements such that an element at any given level represents
the sum of its constituent elements at lower levels. For example, a
port element (Level 7) could be considered to include canal (Level 6)
and partially bounded expanse of water (Level 5). The world is not a
very crisp place and not all its elements are amenable to hierarchical
structuring. Certain violations of the "factor two" rule appear,
therefore, throughout the USE example. It should be noted, how-
ever, that some of the more glaring violations were largely driven
by the "factor one" determinations (i.e., the viewers' abilities to dis-
cern certain elements) enumerated above.
To emphasize once again, it is very important to realize that this
universal set of elements was constructed to match our particular
special targets, viewers, and requirements. They are shown here to
illustrate the procedure. Any particular application of fuzzy set tech-
nology to the analysis of free-response material requires an a priori
construction of an individualized, and improved, USE specific to the
target pool and the goals of the experiment.
Target Fuzzy Sets
Each target is defined as a fuzzy set constructed by assigning a
membership value to each of the elements in the USE (see Appen-
dix A). In general, membership values can vary continuously on the
interval [0,1]. In this application they represent human judgment
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R00220066000.1-5
and, thus, were constrained to vary in steps of 0.1. In addition, they
must represent the perceptual dimension used to construct the USE.
In our example, membership values were assigned to each element
for each of the targets, according to a consensus (on an element-by-
element basis) reached by three analysts. This approach was used to
mitigate the potential influence of any single coder's biases and idio-
syncrasies. A numerical assignment, p (0 -- ? = 1, in steps of 0.1),
was made for each element in response to the following question:
How visually important is this element to this photograph?
Encoded by this method, the fuzzy.sets served as a formal defi-
nition of the targets for the analysis. It should be noted that our
USE defined targets in terms of visual importance.' If other dimen-
sions are of interest (e.g., conceptual, functional, allegorical), the
USE would have to be revised to incorporate them.
In an actual experimental series, it is critical that the target fuzzy
sets be defined by analysts before the series begins. Because of the
potential information leakage owing to bias on the part of the ana-
lyst, it is an obvious mistake to attempt to define the target fuzzy set
on a target-by-target basis in real time or post hoc.
Response Fuzzy Sets
To define RV response fuzzy sets, membership values ? are as-
signed for each element in the USE by asking: To what degree am
I (the analyst) convinced that this element is represented in this re-
sponse? For example, if a response explicitly states "water," then the
membership value for the water-element should be 1. If, however,
the response is a rough sketch of what might be waves, then the
membership value for the water-element might be only 0.3, depend-
ing on the specificity of the drawing. This definition of membership
value is quite general and can be used in most applications.
In our example, responses were coded according to this defini-
tion (but still using the USE in Appendix A). The assigned ?'s for
the targets and responses were one-digit fuzzy numbers on the in-
terval [0,1] (e.g., 0.1, 0.2, 0.3, etc.). In some rare cases, two-digit
assignments (e.g., 0.05, 0.15, 0.25, 0.35, etc.) were made; any finer
assignments, however, were deemed to be meaningless. Thus, the
response was defined as its fuzzy subset of the USE.
' Implied visual importance was ignored. For example, in a photograph of the
Grand Canyon that did not show the Colorado River, water, river, and so on would
be scored as zero. By definition the target was only what was visible in the photo-
graph.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 201
In an actual experimental series, each response fuzzy set is cre-
ated by analysts who are blind to the intended target.
Fuzzy Set Definition of Figure of Merit
Once the fuzzy sets that define the target and the response have
been specified, the comparison between them to provide a figure of
merit (FM) is straightforward. In previous work (Humphrey et al.,
1986), we have defined accuracy as the percent of the target material
that was described correctly by a response. Likewise, we have de-
fined reliability (of the viewer) as the percent of the response that
was correct. The FM is the product of the two; to obtain a high FM,
a response must be a comprehensive description of the target and
be devoid of inaccuracies. The mathematical definitions for accuracy
and reliability for the jth target/response pair are as follows. Let
?k(R;) and ?k(T;) be the membership values for the kth element in
USE for the ith response and the jth target, respectively. Then the
accuracy and reliability for the ith response applied to thejth target
are given by:
Z Wkmin{?k(R;), ?k(Tj)}
reliabilityij = r j =
Wk?'k(Tj)
k
J l
Wkmin{?k(RJ), N'k(Tj)}
W?k(Rj)
where the sum over k is called the sigma count in fuzzy set terminol-
ogy, and is defined as the sum of the membership values. We have
allowed for the possibility of weighting the membership values with
weights Wk in order to examine various level/element contributions
to the FM. The index, k, ranges over the entire USE.
For the above calculation to be meaningful, the ?'s for the tar-
gets must be similar in meaning to the ?'s for the responses. As we
noted above, in our definition of the membership values, this is not
the case. The target ?'s represent the visual importance of the ele-
ment relative to the scene, and the response ?'s represent the de-
gree to which an analyst is convinced that the element is repre-
sented in the response regardless of its relevance to that response.
With advanced viewers it might be possible to change the defi-
nition of the response ?'s to match the definition of the target ?'s.
In that case, the viewer must not only recognize that an element is
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
202 The Journal of Parapsychology '
present in the target, but must also provide information as to how
visually important it is. This ability is currently beyond the skill of
most novice viewers. Alternatively, we have opted to modify the tar-
get ? definition by using the fuzzy set technique of a-cuts. In our
example, an a-cut is a way to set a threshold for visual importance.
All target elements possessing that threshold value or higher are
considered to be full members of the target set. In fuzzy set par-
lance, an a-cut converts a, fuzzy set to a crisp one. The result is that
the target set is now devoid of detailed visual information: a poten-
tial target element is either present or absent in the target set, re-
gardless of its actual visual importance. Even with this conceptual
change in the target definition, the FM formalism described above
remains applicable, because a crisp set can be considered as a fuzzy
set with all membership values equal to 0 or 1. It is important to
recognize that the a-cut is only applied to the target set; the re-
sponse set remains fuzzy,
Assessment of Quality of the Remote Viewing
It is difficult to arrive at a general assessment of how well a given
response matches a specified target. The ideal situation is to obtain
some absolute measure of goodness of match. Although the FM is
an approximation to this measure, it is impossible to assess the like-
lihood of a particular FM value because it requires knowledge of the
viewer's specific response bias for the session. It is possible to deter-
mine general response biases (May et al., 1985), but that knowledge
is only useful on the average. For example, a viewer may love rock
climbing and may spend most of his free time involved in that ac-
tivity. Thus, the general response bias would probably entail aspects
of mountains, rocks, ropes, and so forth. Suppose, however, that the
viewer spent the evening previous to a given RV session on a ro-
mantic moonlight sail on San Francisco Bay. For this specific RV
session, the response bias might include romantic images of the
moonlit water, lights of the city, and bridges.
The current solution to the problem is to provide a relative as-
sessment of FM likelihood. A relative assessment addresses the fol-
lowing question: "How good is the response matched against its in-
tended target, when compared to all possible targets that could have
been chosen for the session?" This is not ideal, since the answer de-
pends on the nature of the remaining targets in the pool. An ex-
ample of the worst-case scenario illustrates the problem. Suppose
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 203
that the target pool consisted of 100 photographs of waterfalls, and
the viewer gave a near-perfect description of a waterfall. (We as-
sume that this description is not fortuitous.) An absolute assessment
of the resulting FM should be good, whereas a relative assessment
will be low. The worst-case scenario can be avoided, to a large de-
gree, by carefully selecting the target pool. (See the later section "A
Quantitative Definition of Target Orthogonality.")
To provide a relative assessment of the likelihood of a given FM,
we define the score for one session to be the number of targets, n,
out of a total, N, that have an FM equal to or higher than the FM
achieved by the correct match.5 The answer to the question: "Given
this response, what is the probability of selecting a target that would
match it as well as or better than the target selected?" is n/N.
Consecutive RV responses by the same viewer are not statistically
independent, nor can the responses be considered to be random in
any sense. The statistically independent random element in the ses-
sion is the target. Since targets are selected with replacement, under
the null hypothesis of no psi, the collection of scores derived over a
series of m trials constitutes a set of independent random variables,
each with a discrete uniform distribution. Under the null hypothe-
sis, the mean chance expectation for the score in each session is
given by (N + 1)/2 and the variance is given by (N2 - 1)/12. If K
is the sum of scores from a series of remote viewings, then the prob-
ability of K, under the null hypothesis, can be obtained from the
exact distribution for the sum of ranks given by Solfvin, Kelly, and
Burdick (1978):
(m) (a - bN1J - 1
K or less) = 1 N 1),
P( K N?~E
a=m i(-b m- (1)
If m is large, then the sum-of-ranks distribution is approximately
normal and K/m has a mean of (N + 1)/2 and a variance of
(N2 - 1)/12m. Thus, a z score can be computed from:
0.5(N+1)-K
z(K or less) = _ m 2
N21 ()
5 N must be the size of the target pool from which each target was randomly
selected, and for this theoretical discussion, we assume no ties.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
204 The Journal of Parapsychology
Ground Truth
To determine whether the new analytical approach was effective,
a standard had to be developed against which it could be measured.
It was determined that this standard-known as "ground truth"-
should consist of a "real-world" normalized consensus about the de=
gree of correspondence between RV responses and their intended
targets.
To achieve this objective, we presented analysts (chosen from the
general SRI staff) with the same test case of six remote-viewing re-
sponses and their associated targets. The test case was the data from
a single viewer (177) taken from an experimental series in a 1986
photomultiplier tube experiment (Hubbard, May, & Frivold, 1987).
The responses (i.e., two to five pages of rudimentary drawings with
some associated descriptive words) were fairly typical of novice
viewer output and represented a broad range of response quality.
The targets consisted of six photographs of outdoor scenes selected
from a National Geographic magazine target pool of 200. Thus, this
data set was ideally suited for an analysis testbed. Appendix B con-
tains the "best" and "worst" trials (Sessions 9005 and 9004, respec-
tively) from this series in the form of their responses, their intended
targets, and their fuzzy set encodings (see the next section).
Each analyst was asked individually for his subjective judgment
about the degree of correspondence betweenthe remote-viewing re-
sponses and their respective intended targets. The "degree of cor-
respondence" was purposely undefined; the analysts had to formu-
late their own criteria. The only information provided was that
responses typically begin with small bits of information and even-
tually culminate in a composite drawing at the end. Appendix C
contains the coding form that was used to obtain "ground truth."
Each analyst was instructed to examine all of the responses and
their intended targets. Then, on a session-by-session basis, he was
asked: (1) to assess the degree of correspondence between the
remote-viewing response and its intended target, and (2) to register
this correspondence assessment by making a vertical hash mark
across a 10-cm scale ranging from "none" to "complete."
To perform the ground truth analysis, distance measurements
were taken from the left end point of each scale to the vertical slash
mark for each assessment. Let the distance obtained for the kth ses-
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
A vances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 205
sion from the jth analyst be given by xi k. To account for analysts'
biases, the xi,k were normalized by a z transformation,
zi xi,k -
,k = ,
~
Q
where ?i and Qi are the mean and standard deviation of the jth an-
alyst's distance scores, x1,k. The effect of this transformation is to
convert an analyst's absolute subjective opinion to a relative one. For
the jth analyst, the largest zz,k indicates that the degree of corre-
spondence for response/target k is higher than any other pair in the
series. It does not indicate overall quality. This type of transforma-
tion was necessary since we wished to combine the assessments from
a number of different analysts.
To combine the assessments across analysts, we computed the
mean z score for each response/target pair, k, as:
Na
Zk = EZi',k,
Na i= 1
where Na is the number of analysts. The number of analysts was
determined by the data. For the best response/target pair (i.e., ses-
sion 9005, k = 5) we computed the percent change of zs for every
additional analyst. When the addition of two new analysts produced
consecutive changes of less than 2%, the process was considered
complete. For this data set, 37 analysts were required before this
condition was met. Figure 'shows the normalized mean for each
target/response pair, and represents a relative assessment of remote-
viewing quality. These means constitute the basis for the ground
truth against which the fuzzy set technique was measured. We re-
cognize that this definition of ground truth is based on global deci-
sions and may not be most optimal (Dawes, 1988).
Results of the Fuzzy Set Analysis
To effect a meaningful comparison between ground truth and
the figure of merit analysis, we also analyzed the same RV series
that served as the ground truth set by the fuzzy set figure of merit
method. The fuzzy set membership values (?'s) for the six targets
and six responses were consensus coded by five analysts ranging
from expert to novice. A typical spread of ? assignments was ? 0.1
with an occasional outlier. Some of the elements were vigorously de-
bated until a consensus was reached. Accuracies, reliabilities, and
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 CIA-RIDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Figure 2. Normalized mean for each target/response pair.
figures of merit were calculated for each target/response pair (Table
1). It should be noted that the encoding was a post hoc exercise, but
because the assignment for each element in the USE had to be de-
fended before a consensus was reached, the FMs shown in Table 1
constitute reasonable estimates of their "blind" equivalents. Appen-
dix B shows the target and response elements that were scoredfrom
the universal set (see Appendix A) for Sessions 9004 and 9005. As
an example of the fuzzy calculation, Appendix B also shows the re-
9001 9002 9003 9004 9005 9006
Session Number
Figure 3. Comparison with ground truth.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 207
TABLE I
Fuzzy SET QUANTITIES FOR "GROUND TRUTH" SERIES
Figure of Fractional
Session Accuracy Reliability merit Rank rank
9001
.317
.484
.153
80
.403
9002
.273
.477
.130
103
.515
9003
.358
.571
.205
31
.155
9004
.212
.379
.080
142
.713
9005
.573
.594
.340
3
.015
9006
.298
.555
.165
13
.068
suits of the target a-cut, the fuzzy intersection, and the accuracy,
reliability, and figure of merit for Session 9005. Table 1 also shows
the absolute and relative ranks from a target pool of 200. To deter-
mine the absolute rank for each session, we calculated figures of
merit for all 200 targets in the pool and placed them in numerical
order from the largest to the smallest. The absolute rank is just the
position (from the top) of the FM corresponding to the intended
target. Ties were resolved by choosing the next larger integer rank
number to the centroid of the ties. The fractional rank number can
be considered a p value for an individual session and is equal to the
absolute rank/200. Using Equation 1, the overall p value for the
combined six trials is .052 (N = 200, K = 372, m = 6). Using the
approximation (Equation 2), we compute z = 1.633, p _< .05, to
demonstrate that for six trials, the approximation in reasonable. For
completeness, we compute the effect size (r = 0.67).
To compare the results of the fuzzy set analysis with those of the
ground truth, we linearly renormalized the ground truth figures to
be within the interval [0,1] and to possess the same maximum and
minimum. As can be seen from Figure 3, the results from the fuzzy
set analysis system parallel those obtained by a consensus of the 37
analysts each making a subjective assessment of the matches.
These results imply that the combination of (1) the structure of
the USE (i.e., the linguistic hierarchical structure), (2) the fuzzy set
mathematics, and (3) a consensus approach to assessing the fuzzy
sets themselves provided a reasonable representation of the subjec-
tive scoring of the same data by a large number of individuals.
A Quantitative Definition of Target Orthogonality
It is often of interest to define how similar or dissimilar targets
are to each other. For example, free-response experiments like the
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
208 The Journal of Parapsychology
ganzfeld often use target packets, with the unselected targets in a
packet serving as decoys for judging. Assigning potential targets to
packets would be easier withsome measure of target orthogonality.
Target definition for the purposes of this mode of analysis is ex-
actly the same as the one described (i.e., a given target is defined by
its fuzzy subset of the USE, which has been coded to reflect the vis-
ual importance of each target element). The average number of ele-
ments, of the total of 131, that was assigned a nonzero value for the
targets in our pool of 200 was approximately 37, indicating that the
fuzzy set representation of the target pool is rich in visual infor-
mation. We used this information to determine the degree to which
the target set contains visually similar targets.
It is beyond the scope of this paper to describe the extensive
work in the literature seeking to find algorithmic techniques that
mimic human assessments of visual similarity. One recent article de-
scribes techniques similar to the one we used (Zick, Carlstein, & Bu-
descu, 1987).
We begin by defining the simlarity between target i and target j
to be a normalized fuzzy set intersection between the two target sets:
l2
I Z Wkmin{?k(Ti),Wk(7'.)} 1
S. _
i' EWkN'k(Ti)EW4N'k(T;)
k k
where the index k ranges over the entire USE. We have allowed for
the possibility of weighting the membership values with weights Wk
to examine various level/element contributions to the target similar-
ities.
For N targets, there are N(N- 1)/2 unique values (19,900 for N
= 200) of S. The values i and j that correspond to the largest value
of Sig represent the two targets that "look" most similar. Suppose
another target m is chosen and S,,,,i and S,,,; are computed. If both of
these values are larger than S?,,,, (for all n not equal to i or j), then
target m is assessed to be most similar to the pair ij. The process of
grouping targets based on these similarities is called cluster analysis.
Using this process, 200 targets were grouped into 19 clusters,
such that the targets are similar within a cluster, and dissimilar be-
tween clusters. Table 2 provides an overview of the 19 clusters
found from the total analysis of the 200 targets. Some of the names
appear to be quite similar, but, in fact, these sets are visually quite
distinctive. Figure 4 shows the graphic output of a single cluster in
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 209
TABLE 2
NAMES OF THE 19 CLUSTERS
1 Flat towns
2 Waterfalls
3 Mountain towns
4 Cities with prominent structure
5 Cities on water
6 Desert/water interfaces
7 Deserts
8 Dry ruins
9 Towns on water
10 Outposts on water
11 Cities with prominent geometries
12 Snowy mountains
13 Valleys with rivers
14 Meandering rivers
15 Alpine scenes
16 Outposts in snowy mountains
17 Islands
18 Verdant ruins
19 Agricultural scenes
detail. A much more complex-and visually difficult to under-
stand-graph is generated for the full cluster analysis and is not
included here; this smaller subset, therefore, has been chosen to be
illustrative of the whole analysis. All targets in this particular sample
cluster are islands; the island in each photograph is visible in its en-
tirety. Except for one outlier (i.e., a hexagonal building covering an
island), the islands fall into two main groups (i.e., with and without
Linear Geometries
(e.g., Runways)
1198
1138
1128
Target lxxx
Number
1186
Many Structures 1179
(e.g., Town) 1177-1
Ruins ------------ > 1083
1185
Flat And Verdant 1139
1140
1088
1161 ::~~
Mountains 1.049
1038
Hexagonal Building Covering Island ------> 1003-
0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0
1 - Si,i
Figure 4. Cluster analysis of island targets.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
210 The Journal of Parapsychology
manmade elements). The natural islands include three similar
mountain islands, two sandbars, and two flat verdant islands.
Using cluster analysis in conjunction with fuzzy set analysis pro-
vides for a quantitative definition of sets of targets that are similar
to each other within a cluster, but visually different across clusters.
Orthogonal clusters can be used to provide visual decoy targets for
traditional rank-order judging.
To apply the analysis in its present form to a long RV series is
quite labor intensive and., from the results shown in Figure 3, is
most likely not justified since this fuzzy set technique approximates
human assessment. As we stated in the introduction, however, we
are providing only a progress report of ongoing research. Because
of the decision concepts described in Dawes (1988) and the obvious
benefits of an automated evaluation system, the effort to improve
what was described in this paper is certainly justified. The proce-
dure can be used "as is" to improve and quantify target orthogo-
nality.
Several future research areas are suggested to improve the tech-
niques described in this paper. The use of both inter- and intra-level
weighting factors needs to be examined systematically. In the analy-
sis described above, all levels and elements were accorded equal
weight. The ideal goal would be to determine the optimal weighted
mix of abstract versus concrete elements, as a means to achieving
the following objectives:
1. Refinement of the cluster analysis for targets, in an effort to
simulate, as closely as possible, what is meant by "visual simi-
larities" between targets.
2. Refinement of the analysis of responses, in an effort to
achieve even greater correlations between the fuzzy set figure
of merit analysis and various forms of ground truth.
Another area that requires examination in some detail is the
USE and the hierarchical nature of its structure. It is probable that
some elements are more appropriate than others; furthermore, they
might be more effectively structured in a semantic network as op-
posed to a true hierarchy. If a hierarchical structure is retained,
then some attention must be paid to the formulation of logical con-
sistency rules that govern element use. This would include numeri-
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 211
cal relationships governing the membership values (?'s) of higher-
order elements (e.g., port) vis-A-vis the combined value of their con-
stituent parts (e.g., city, river, boats, jetties, commercial).
One inadequacy of the system is that it atomizes conceptual
"units." For example, if the response element is red box, information
is lost in separating red from box. Current research in fuzzy set the-
ory indicates that fuzzy aggregates of fuzzy elements-"fuzzy sets of
fuzzy sets"-are mathematically complex but possible. Some effort
should be made to determine whether this technology could be im-
plemented as a means to capturing the information content of the
RV response with greater accuracy.
For the visual analysis, research into visual similarities between
pictures of natural scenes may serve as a potential refinement tool.
The aim here would be to enhance the visual orthogonality of rank-
order analysis decoy targets as much as possible. Experiments in
normal perception of similarities would assist in determining
whether scenes are perceived as similar because of their low-level
geometries, concrete elements, or some combination of factors. The
ultimate aim would be to refine the target cluster analysis such that
it closely simulates ground truth representations of orthogonality.
APPENDIX A
CODING FORMS FOR THE UNIVERSAL SET OF ELEMENTS
The following coding forms illustrate the use of a universal set of elements
(USE) that matched our particular special targets, viewers, and requirements.
We constructed our USE by including a list of features present in photographs
from the National Geographic with elements obtained from the remote-
viewing responses in earlier experiments.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
212 The Journal of Parapsychology
? L' L
00
W~ Q U s
~
~ '~ ~~~ d 3 t
o000000
00
O
P
o
Figure Al. Coding form.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 213
iiiliiihd
DUTTON
0
DUO
LI
A 6 Y
B
0a
w
z3>
U
_
gg
LU
U1
3Z
UU TIE
U LI1LJLI
z
0
z
W
2
Mt
8 . ~ 8 B h ~~
0
UU0
J
j
F-
O
N
Figure A2. Coding form.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001.-5
214 The Journal of Parapsychology
E t .. ??
y a w?
CL a v
a
8
i z i
m 8 3
a
m
~
z
z
p
N
s
w
Ell.
LLI
2
W
~ p5
=
S
6 L $
w
W
MOO
a
U)
J
M
T t
S
U
W
q
t SA ~~ ~~
.SAD C
0
W
Figure A3. Coding form.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 215
g,~?
uJ - Q
U
g
~yEt ~
E
w~
?O
=,n
IL
2
LU
S2
y@ Fy
}
W
U)
ca
L< =
29
II L
0
r
O
I i i
z
V Q
U U
DuLl
r:E
0 cc
E 8
R D N
WW
W
N
..,
Figure A4. Coding form.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001.-5
216 The Journal of Parapsychology
APPENDIX B
FUZZY SET ANALYSIS TESTBED
The following pages show the targets, responses, and analysis for two
remote-viewing trials.
Figure B 1. Target for Session 9004.
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 217
1_6 S.GL o ,-v j-.,4 4~
aQv
Figure B2. Page one of the response (Session 9004, Target 1094)
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 :CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001.-5
~yJ rte-' ~`-"
Figure B3. Page two of the response (Session 9004, Target 1094).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 219
P
LA7
JU_
Fm"' 7C~.Szs 's ON
Figure B4. Page three of the response (Session 9004, Target 1094).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
220 The Journal of Parapsychology
TABLE B I
TARGET-RESPONSE 9004
20
Roads
0.30
0.00
23
Agricultural fields
0.05
0.00
32
Urban
0.00
0.50
33
Rural, pastoral
0.60
0.50
44
Town, village
0.00
0.50
45
City
0.00
0.40
46
Single peak
0.70
0.00
47
Hills, slopes, bumps, mounds
0.10
0.40
48
Mountains
0.00
0.60
49
Cliffs
0.00
0.10
60
Vegetation, trees
0.30
0.00
64
Blue
0.50
0.00
65
Green
0.30
0.00
69
White
0.10
0.00
70
Grey
0.20
0.00
76
Obscured, fuzzy, dim, smoky
0.20
0.00
77
Cloudy, foggy, misty
0.20
0.00
79
Weathered, eroded, incomplete
0.00
0.10
80
Smooth
0.00
1.00
81
Fuzzy
0.20
0.00
82
Grainy, sandy, crumbly
0.20
1.00
90
Other implied movement
0.20
0.00
91
Congested, cluttered, busy
0.10
0.30
92
Serene, peaceful, unhurried
0.40
0.00
93
Closed in, claustrophobic
0.00
0.10
94
Open, spacious, vast
0.60
0.00
95
Ordered, aligned
0.00
0.40
97
Buildings, structures
0.00
1.00
98
Rise, vertical rise, slope
0.60
1.00
99
Flat
0.30
1.00
100
Light/dark areas
0.10
0.00
101
Boundaries
0.30
1.00
103
Land/sky interface
0.50
0.00
104
Single predominant feature
0.60
0.00
105
Odd juxtaposition, surprising
0.30
0.00
106
Manmade, altered
0.20
0.80
107
Natural
0.70
0.20
108
Rectangle, square, box
0.00
1.00
109
Triangle, pyramid, trapezoid
0.60
0.00
115
Cone
0.60
0.00
117
Irregular forms
0.00
0.20
118
Repeat motif
0.10
0.60
119
Stepped
0.10
0.70
120
Parallel lines
0.10
0.00
121
Vertical lines
0.10
1.00
122
Horizontal lines
0.10
0.00
123
Diagonal lines
0.40
0.00
125
Inverted V-shape
0.70
0.00
126
Other angles
0.00
0.10
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 221
Figure B5. Target for Session 9005.
~~ ,~,,, PS 3 4 S
Figure B6. Page one of response (Session 9005, Target 1005).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
222 The Journal of Parapsychology
Figure B7. Page two of response (Session 9005, Target 1005).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 223
I~C_U_~
I-
-,W r
tij
Figure B8. Page three of response (Session 9005, Target 1005).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Figure B9. Page four of response (Session 9005, Target 1005).
pproved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
dvances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 225
Figure B 10. Page five of response (Session 9005, Target 1005).
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08TABC1&-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
14
Spire, minaret, tower
0.00
0.20
0
0.00
20
Roads
0.10
0.10
0
0.00
32
Urban
0.80
0.70
1
0,70
38
Canal, manmade waterway
0.00
0.10
0
0.00
44
Town, village
0.00
0.30
0
0.00
45
City
0.90
0.70
1
0.70
46
Single peak
0.00
0.20
0
0.00
47
Hills, slopes, bumps, mounds
0.00
0.10
0
0.00
54
Unbounded large expanse water
0.00
0.40
0
0.00
56
Partially bounded water
0.30
0.30
1
0.30
58
River, stream, creek
0.00
0.40
0
0.00
59
Coastline
0.00
0.20
0
0.00
60
Vegetation, trees
0.20
0.20
1
0.20
64
Blue
0.25
0.00
1
0.00
65
Green
0.20
0.00
1
0.00
67
Brown, beige
0.50
0.00
1
0.00
69
White
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
70
Grey
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
80
Smooth
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
81
Fuzzy
0.00
1.00
0
0.00
82
Grainy, sandy, crumbly
0.00
1.00
0
0.00
83
Rocky, ragged, rubbled, rough
0.00
1.00
0
0.00
91
Congested, cluttered, busy
0.70
0.70
1
0.70
94
Open, spacious, vast
0.10
1.00
0
0.00
95
Ordered, aligned
0.00
0.30
0
0.00
96
Disordered, jumbled, unaligned
0.30
0.00
1
0.00
97
Buildings, structures
0.80
0.90
1
0.90
98
Rise, vertical rise, slope
0.00
1.00
0
0.00
99
Flat
0.50
1.00
1
1.00
100
Light/dark areas
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
10I
Boundaries
0.20
1.00
1
1.00
102
Land/water interface
0.30
1.00
1
1.00
103
Land/sky interface
0.10
0.10
0
0.00
104
Single predominant feature
0.10
0.40
0
0.00
106
Manmade, altered
0.80
0.80
1
0.80
107
Natural
0.20
0.20
1
0.20
108
Rectangle, square, box
0.70
1.00
1
1.00
111
Cross-hatch, grid
0.30
0.00
1
0.00
112
Circle, oval, sphere
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
116
Semicircle, dome, hemisphere
0.10
0.30
0
0.00
118
Repeat motif
0.40
0.80
1
0.80
119
Stepped
0.20
1.00
1
1.00
120
Parallel lines
0.30
0.30
1
0.30
121
Vertical lines
0.50
1.00
1
1.00
122
Horizontal lines
0.10
0.00
0
0.00
123
Diagonal lines
0.10
0.20
0
0.00
125
Inverted V-shape
0.00
0.20
0
0.00
127
Arc, curve
0.30
1.00
1
1.00
128
Wave form
0.00
0.10
0
0.00
Accuracy = 0.573
Reliability = 0.594
Figure of merit = 0.340
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Advances in Remote-Viewing Analysis 22
APPENDIX C
"GROUND TRUTH" INSTRUCTION AND CODING FORM
Analysts' Instructions for Remote-Viewing Series 900X
Thank you for helping us perform a post hoc assessment of a series of
remote viewings. The targets were actually 35-mm slides that were attached
to a photomultiplier, a device to measure small amounts of light. We were
searching for possible physical correlates to remote viewing.
You will find in your packet 6 remote viewing responses labeled 9001-
9006 respectively. Also shown is the target number of the intended photo-
graph. We have supplied the original, rather than the 35-mm slide.
We would like you to make a subjective judgment as to the degree of
correspondence between the remote viewing response and its associated tar-
get. Familiarize yourself with the task by first looking at all the responses
and their intended targets. Then, on a session-by-session basis, rate your
assessments. You are completely free to define what is meant by "Degree of
Correspondence." Indicate your judgment by marking one line across the
appropriate continuous scale shown below. A vertical line near the "None"
end of the scale will indicate that you feel there is very little correspondence
between that response-target pair. Likewise a vertical line near the "Com-
plete" end of the scale will indicate that you feel that there is a significant
degree of correspondence.
Many of the responses begin with a little information and build toward
a composite drawing at the end. Please assess the response in its entirety as
best you can. Thank you again.
Complete
--I 1034
9001
1042
9002
-I 1065
9003
-I 1094
9004
--I 1005
9005
1024
9006
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5
Approved For Release 20QQ/0~8/08 : JA-tP96-007898002200660001.-5
e ournal o ara syc ology
DAWES, R. M. (1988). Rational choice in an uncertain world. New York: Har-
court, Brace, Jovanovich.
HONORTON, C. (1975). Objective determination of information rate in psi
tasks with pictorial stimuli. Journal of the American Society for Psychical
Research, 69, 353-359.
HUBBARD, G. S., MAY, E. C., & FRIVOLD, T. J. (1987). Possible photon pro-
duction during a remote viewing task: A replication experiment. Final
Report, SRI Project 1291, SRI International, Menlo Park, California.
HUMPHREY, B. S., MAY, E. C., TRASK, V. V., & THOMSON, M. J. (1986).
Remote viewing evaluation techniques. Final Report, SRI Project 1291,
SRI International, Menlo Park, California.
HUMPHREY, B. S., MAY, E. C., & UTTS, J. M. (1988). Fuzzy set technology
in the analysis of remote viewing. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conven-
tion of the Parapsychological Association (pp. 378-394).
JAHN, R. G., DUNNE, B. J., & JAHN, E. G. (1980). Analytical judging pro-
cedure for remote perception experiments. Journal of Parapsychology,
44, 207-231.
MAY, E. C. (1983). A remote viewing evaluation protocol. Final Report (re-
vised), SRI Project 4028, SRI International, Menlo Park, California.
MAY, E. C., HUMPHREY, B. S., & MATHEWS, C. (1985). A figure of merit
analysis for free-response material. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Con-
vention of the Parapsychological Association, (pp. 343-354).
PUTHOFF, H. E., & TARG, R. (1976). A perceptual channel for information
transfer over kilometer distances: Historical perspective and recent re-
search. Proceedings of the IEEE, 64(3), 329-354.
SOLFVIN, G. F., KELLY, E. F., & BURDICK, D. S. (1978). Some new methods
of analysis for preferential-ranking data. Journal of the American Society
for Psychical Research, 72(2), 93-110.
TARG, R., PUTHOFF, H. E., & MAY, E. C. (1977). State of the art in remote
viewing studies at SRI. 1977 Proceedings of the International Conference of
Cybernetics and Society (pp. 519-529).
ZICK, R., CARLSTEIN, E., & BUDESCU, D. V. (1987). Measures of similarity
among fuzzy concepts: A comparative analysis. International Journal of
Approximate Reasoning, 1(2), 221-242.
SRI International
333 Ravenswood Av.
Menlo Park, CA 94025
Division of Statistics
University of California, Davis
Davis, CA 95616
Approved For Release 2000/08/08 : CIA-RDP96-00789R002200660001-5