TARGET SEARCH TECHNIQUES

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP96-00787R000300030001-7
Release Decision: 
RIFPUB
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
32
Document Creation Date: 
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date: 
June 11, 1998
Sequence Number: 
1
Case Number: 
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP96-00787R000300030001-7.pdf1.14 MB
Body: 
For Release 2000/08/10 : CI -RDP96-0078780003 0030001-7 Final Report December 1984 Covering the Period 15 November 1983 to 15 December 1984 ^ C~ TARGET SEARCH TECHNIQUES (U) ~y Approved by: .~ SRI Project 6600 1 ~. roved For Release 2000/08/10 : C14-RDP96-00787R00~300030001-7 Approved For Release 2080/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-00787~t000300030001-7 CONTENTS (U) (U) LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . - - - - - - - . . - iv (U) LIST OF TABLES - . . iv I (U) OBJECTIVE . - - - _ - - - 1 II (U) INTRODUCTION . 2 A. (U) General - . 2 B. (U) Search Categories - - - _ - - - - - ~ - - - - ~ - - - 3 III (U) ;METHOD OF APPROACH q A. (U) Continuum Search--Statistical Approach q B. (U) Discrete Search-Statistical Approach . - - , - - _ 5 V (U) EXPERIMENTAL . - - _ - - - - - - - - - - - - - g A (U) General . - - - 9 B? ~~-~~~~. Simulation of "Bug" Search (Continuum)- , - _ . g 1. (U) Condition I 2. (U) Condition II , - - - , - - - - - - 10 C?~ i' Simulation of "Agent" Search, Facility Level ~Conunuum~ - - ~ - - - - ~ D? `, ) Simulation of "Agent"/Facility Search (Continuum)- 12 E. (U) Binary Search (Discrete/Continuum) - - - - - - - - - - 13 F? (U) Computer Assisted Search (CAS} - - - - - - - , - 21 1. (U) Basic Investigation (Simulation). - - - - - - - - 21 2. (U) Location of Real-~'Vorld Targets (Known) - - - - - - - - 22 - ii - Approved For Release 2 0/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0 7878000300030001-7 t A roved For Release 20~ 0i'08i'10 : CIA-RDP96-007 78000300030001-7 pp 3. (U) CAS Against Real Targets (Application) . G? Controlled Long-Distance Test of "Agent/ Building Search, Facility Level (Continuum). 1. (U) Long-Distance "Agent" Search 2. (U) Long-Distance "Building" Search - V (U) Summary . A. (U) Overview B. (U) Focus of Investigation , _ _ C. (U) Recommendations for Follow-On Actions - , - ? 23 - 24 ? - - 24 - 26 ? - 28 ? - 28 - - - 28 ? - 29 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0078~7R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2p00/08/10:CIA-RDP96-007~7R000300030001-7 I OBJECTIVE (U) The objective of this effort at SRI International is to investigate a particular aspect of psychoenergetic phenomena called Target Search. This search technique is designed to determine the location of objects, individuals, and facilities where the potential target area can range from room- to global-sized dimensions. -I- Approved For Release 20 0/08/10 : CIA-RDP~6-007878000300030001-7 Approved For Release'~2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787~R000300030001-7 II INTRODUCTION (U) ;.., ' ~ -~" ~ a claimed ability in the broad field of psychoenergetic functioning; namely, the ability to search for and locate water, oil, minerals, objects, individuals, sites of archaeological significance, and so forth. This ability can be contrasted to the related psychoenergetic ability "remote viewing," in the following manner. In remote viewing, the RVer is given location information (coordinates, "beacon" agent, picture), and (RV) asked to provide data- on target content in "search," the RVer is given information on target content, then asked to provide location' data (e.g., position on a map). The two [unctions are thus complimentary to each (U) The ability to locate targets is most often referred to as "dowsing" in the Western literature, and "biophysical effect (BPE)" in the Soviet/East Bloc literature. In this report, we shall refer to such techniques simply as "search." Although much of the literature is anecdotal,' attempts to quantify the ability and to determine its mechanisms have been pursued.t `(U) For the most comprehensive and authoritative survey of the claims for dowsing, see Christopher Bird, The Divining Nand, E. P. Dutton, New York, NY (1979). t(U) .See, for example, papers published by Z. V. Harvalik, beginning 1970, in The American Dowser, the journal of the American Society of Dowsers, (i-iarvalik is the Approved For Release ~t000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-0078 8000300030001-7 Approved For Rele se 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0~787R000300030001-7 r r r r r r r r r The goal of the present effort is to research the literature, then perform laboratory experimentation to determine whether, and to what degree, such functioning is a viable candidate for applicatiorf~ t This includes determining the best methods and efficiencies of various search techniques, and the appropriate statistical analyses for evaluating results. B. (U) Search Categories .Search tasks fall into two broad categories of effort--continuum and discrete. In the "continuum" search category, a target of interest is typically to be located on a continuum area map,. such as a topographical map or navigational charN ` For this category, the target/response distances and circular error probabilities (CEPS) constitute the statistics of interest in evaluation. i~-~- ,~ In the "discrete" search category, a target of interest is associated with a discrete number of possibilities For this category, the appropriate statistic of interest in the evaluation of a series of location attempts is a comparison against the simple binomial statistic of the probability of obtaining an observed R hits in N trials, by chance. -3- Approved For Release~2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-0 7878000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007878000300030001-7 UNCLASSIFIED III METHOD OF APPROACH (U) A. (U) Continuum Search--Statistical Approach (U) The first-order requirement in carrying out acontinuum-search effort is to determine an appropriate method of evaluation. The approach chosen here is a modification of a procedure developed by Dean Radin at Bell Laboratories for evaluating geometric-distance scores in a perceptual task. * In this approach, one begins by assuming that the target area of interest is in the form of a square (a useful, but not necessary, requirement). A grid system is then laid down over the square in the form of an n X n matrix (20 X 20, say), to yield n2 separate grid elements (400 fora 20 X 20 grid, for example) . Using this grid as an approximation to a continuum, one can then calculate exactly the a priori chance distribution that any given search response would lie at any given distance from a particular target location. From this, an evaluation can be made on the quality of response (see Figure 1). (U) In the 20 X 20 case, for a target square not on the edge, there is one chance in 400 of the searcher landing directly on the target square by chance, four chances in 400 of being one unit away (above, below, and to the sides), four chances of being ~ units away, and so forth. Counting out from any particular target square, the inhibition of responses beyond the boundary of the overall square (at certain distances in certain directions) is easily taken into account in the counting process. Thus, an exact calculation can be made on the basis of straightforward counting (taking into account edge effects) for the probabilities of all possible target/response pair distances. One result of these calculations for the 20 X 20 case (taken as standard) is Table 1, in which the mean distance from each given target square to all possible response squares in the grid is displayed. As a reference, the grand mean chance expectation (MCE) target response pair distance in the 20 X 20 grid is 10.41 units. * (U) Radin, D., "Evaluating Geometric Distance Scores in a Perceptual Task," Research in Parapsychology 1981, pp. 163-164, Scarecrow Press, Inc., Metuchen, NJ (1982). -4- Approved For Release 200~~~`~i~g~~~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20QQ/Q$/1Q :~1~4;~P'~6~Q0787R000300030001-7 Prob (d) Q.r, ::4 : }::: n ? :. .}9: n}:v .Y;4 S{p ... 1?:4i?:{:~?:4Y?$ii:'::~:: .:; :,: :.,~f':.,:k:~:~%:i;'!~::'::}: :}c:??.::.'~{:?`::~?'~,~~~~#f?^~Eh:..::.2`;~:Y3?>::`2:rc:.r~>TO lE~ 64tLBL 1. 6` RL'L 11 6F Ltd 67 RCL 1~? f~$ + F ~ E1`" n ~~ T+ 81 71 RCL 01 72 TOtdE O 73 PSE 74 1 75 'iT+ 9F 76 RCL BF_. r7 1 76 - 79 RCS -? 8A kY 8i GTsa OB 82 RCL O1 83 "P, R OR MORE=" 84 ARCL 85 A4' I Ebi 86 BEEP 67 END -8- Approved For Release 2b~~PJ4~IRU~6~00787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release) 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007~87R000300030001-7 IV EXPERIMENTAL EFFORT (U) 1 (U) In pursuing the search task, SRI engaged several remote viewers (RVers) ranging from (1) volunteer subjects, (2) experienced SRI RVers, through (3) well-known professional "dowsers" (who were contacted through the American Society of Dowsers). In somewhat extensive work with the latter, every effort was made to determine whether whatever skills could be demonstrated- might be of a transferable nature. B. ~ ~ Simulation of "Bug" Search (Continuum) 1 `Described here is a test of whether a search procedure involving attempts to locate small objects in a roor.., 'would be successful. The target location was a large conference room in which a >1400 sq. ft. area (37.5 X 37.5 ft) was designated as the potential target area. For each trial, a small hand-size object was chosen (e.g., a calculator) then placed somewhere in the conference room-the location was determined by entry into a random number generator for x-y coordinates on a 20 X 20 unit grid. (U) A total of 50 trials, 25 in each of two conditions (labeled I and II), was carried out with an experienced SRI RVer (#688) as search percipient. The RVer was in the RV chamber on the third floor of the Radio Physics Laboratory (RPL); the target area was a locked and guarded, nonoccupied conference room on the ground floor of the RPL. r (U) In Condition I, for each trial, an experimenter (EI) places an object at a location in the target room (determined by random number generator), then remains outside the target room as a guard. A few minutes later, at apreviously-agreed-upon time, Experimenter E2, who is kept blind as to the object's location, has the RVer indicate his assessment of the object's location. The RVer places a mark on a piece of paper containing a single blank square to represent the target room. At the end of the trial, the RVer turns I Approved For Relea a 2000f08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0~ 7878000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20~/~~~I~~Q~~C~~787R000300030001-7 (u) his response over to E2, the two of them proceed to the target room to meet E1, and they all enter the room to obtain feedback. Following feedback, the response coordinates, obtained by use of a 20 X 20 grid square overlay on the response sheet, are determined, and (along with the target coordinates) are entered into a computer to compare the probability of the observed result against chance. (U) In the 37.5-ft-square target area, the mean chance expectation (MCE) target/response pair distance is 19.5 ft. In the 25 trials taken under Condition I, the target/response pair distances ranged from a maximum of 15.6 ft to a minimum of 2.0 ft, with a mean of 13.5 ft. This result (a 31 percent reduction from MCE) is statistically significant at p = 1.7 X 10_3. As a second measure of performance, the target/response pair distances were ordered such that there was a trend from larger to smaller distances as the series progressed, providing some evidence of improvement in task performance over time, but the trend was not statistically significant. (U) The Condition I and Condition II series were carried out in an identical fashion, except that in Condition II, only one experimenter (E1) was involved. Thus, E1 "hid" the object for each trial. In the 25-trial sequence, the target/response pair distances ranged from a maximum of 37.5 ft to a minimum of 3.75 ft, with a mean of 18.8 ft. This result (a 4 percent reduction MCE) is statistically nonsignificant. In the second measure of perform- ance, however, the target/response pair distances were again ordered from larger to smaller as the series progressed, but, in this case, the trend was statistically significant (p < 0.05). Thus, evidence of improvement in task performance over time was established. (U) With regard to the difference in RVer performance between Conditions I and II (which, aone-way analysis of variance shows to be significant`), one can simply note the difference in the two conditions. Technically speaking, the RVer's task was identical in the two cases--namely, to determine the location of arandomly-placed object in a target room. The psychology differed somewhat, however, in that during the less successful series (Condition II), one experimenter hiding an object could be said to have more of a "game" "(U) dfl = 1, df2 = 48, F = 4.94; p= 0.031. -10- Approved For Release 2~0~81'~t~:'C11X ~~~9~A~00787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release~1000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-007~7R000300030001-7 (U) aspect to it than in Condition I, where the task is structured more along application lines of an experimenter and- RVer searching for an object placed by a third party. Another difference is that the second series followed the first; the trend may simply reflect the "decline" effect that is typical in the psychoenergetics field when experimentation becomes a repetitive routine. Further work along these lines would be required to clarify this particular facet. Nonetheless, with both sets of data combined, the overall result, a mean target/response pair distance of 16.2 ft (17 percent reduction of MCE), is statistically significant at p = 0.01. Thus, the experimental series as a whole indicates that the application of psychoenergetic search techniques, although not yet developed to high accuracy, can augment other techniques in locating small target objects in a remote, otherwise inaccessible, space. C. ~ Simulation of "Agent" Search, Facility Level (Continuum) ~_...: (U) The RVer who participated in the above experiment was asked to take part in a second experiment of a similar nature. In the second case, the target was to be a person, located somewhere on the grounds of the SRI 70-acre complex. The RVer entered the RV chamber on the third floor of the RPL, then an experimenter was sent to a random location, determined by entry into a random number generator for x-y coordinates on a 20 X 20 unit grid. (U) Forty trials were carried out-20 in each of two conditions; the second condition differed from the first only in that the RVer had in his possession a sample of hair from the individual to be targeted (to test the so-called "witness" concept, part of the lore in dowsing studies) . (U) The outcome of this experiment was that neither series yielded results differing from chance expectation, nor was there any significant difference between the two conditions. Although the RVer expressed subjective differences in attempting to locate a person (as opposed to an object), no conclusions as to the difference between the two Experiments B and C' could be drawn. ' (U) Defined in Subsections B and C of this chapter and hereafter referred to as Experiments B and C. Approved For Releas 2,000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007 78000300030001-7 i 3 Approved For Release 000/08%10 :CIA-RDP96-007~7R000300030001-7 -._.~- Simulation of "Agent"/Facil?ity Search (Continuum) i A series of trials was undertaken with a special RVer, recommended by the American Society of Dowsers, who responded to an invitation to participate in the SRI search program (#198). An initial-exploratory series. of .eight trials was carried out as in Experiment B, Condition I, The results were not beyond chance expectation. Debriefing of the RVer revealed a preference for tasks that involved people, as in locating people, or places inhabited by people. Therefore, a second exploratory series of fifteen trials was performed, the first ten of which repeated the same experiment, but with an individual replacing the object. For the remaining five trials, the target area in which the individual was located was expanded to the SRI complex as in Experiment C above. The overall result in this series also was not beyond chance expectation, but the target/response pair distances were ordered such that there was a trend from larger to smaller distances as the series progressed, which began to approach statistical significance (p = 0.07). Therefore, a more extensive series was planned in which "peopled" locations were to be the targets. (ll) For this series, the target area was a > 20 sq. km area (4.8 km X 4.8 km). The viewer was provided a satellite photograph of the area with a 20 X 20 unit grid overlay. In order to investigate potential differences in targeting strategies, a targeting protocol was prepared for the series in which, on an intermixed basis, the RVer was targeted on being (1) given a photograph of the site, e.g., of a house, (2) told that an experimenter known to the RVer was at a site, (3) told the name of the site (e.g., Stacey's Bookstore). Twenty-one trials were carried out under this protocol: six under targeting method (1), six under targeting method (2), and nine under targeting method (3). ._ (U) In the 4.8-km-square area, the mean chance expectation distance is 2.5 km, In the 21-trial sequence, the target/response pair distances ranged from a maximum of 3.36 km to a minimum of 0.34 km, with a mean of 1.55 km. This result (a 38 percent reduction from MCE) is statistically significant at p = 1.1 X 10_3 . (U) With regard to the difference in RVer performance between targeting conditions, the mean target/response pair distance was least for the photo-targeting condition (1), median for the person-targeting condition (2), and maximum for the name-targeting condition (3)-1.15 km, 1.58 km, and 1.80 km respectively. One-way analysis of variance Approved For Release ~OOOl08/10 : CIA-RDP96-Op787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20Qq/a$/,'~O,:~IQr-R~R~~Qg787R000300030001-7 (u) shows, however, that the differences between the conditions were not statistically significant. * Furthermore, no statistically-significant ordering of the target/response distances was observed as the series progressed, indicating a level performance throughout the series. E. (U) Binary Search (Discrete/Continuum) (U) A "zeroing-in" approach that suggests itself is a sequential method whereby the RVer makes a series of binary decisions to close in, step-by-step, on the target. If one begins with a square map, for example, the sequence of questions would be of the form "left or right half," followed by "upper or lower half" of the remaining half, followed again by "left or right half" of the remaining square, and so forth, as indicated in Figure 2. After n binary decisions, the designated target area would be narrowed down to 1/2"`h of the original area (1/2, 1/4, 1116, 1/32, ...). Assuming a flawless sequence of binary decisions, one could pinpoint a target location relatively quickly by this means. (U) The statistics of binary (p = 1/2) sorting by psychoenergetic means are relatively well known, and on the order of a few percent above chance--this is statistically significant, but is of little use in applications. One is led naturally to consideration of the use of redundancy in one form or another (e.g., repetitive "guessing"), in order to amplify the small statistical advantage available into an overall higher-accuracy result for the basic binary process. As part of the binary-approach study, we investigated implementation of the redundancy concept, reduced to practice in the form of a hand-held calculator programmed for statistical averaging of (psi) inputs by an RVer. (U) The targets for the series described here are the outcomes (black/redt in roulette wheel spins) that are being generated in a double-blind fashion by an experimenter. The task is thus one of determining whether a roulette ball is located in a red or a black bin. As the RVer attempts to identify the ball's location, several responses are entered into a calculator for statistical averaging as described below. *(U) dfl = 2, df2 = 18, F = 1..43; p < 0.26. t (U) Green 0 and 00 are, for the purposes of this study, taken to correspond to red and black, respectively. ''~ ll ~~^^ -AA13CC- ''~CC~ nn Approved For Release 20091~'F9 ~ G?~~~96~09~187R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20QQfQ$/~~I~~~P~,6;(~0787R000300030001-7 ':?:t:.~a:.~t;,r..:is.:.::::::?::iit:?.:: ~:.:tr S:t:'':. ?;:F:t;;:;:'::;:~':?::a:.r,.~,;,fast?i;;5,:>~:t:.?::i',:;?.'.:?;:x...a:+::::?.,?:::i.;;:$?:?E>:;2?''.: ci,':` ~~E}:?>:+::.tr,.,`.?:t'~:t?:.i:::':o?.?:x.,p;.x't.;:o~:2;;r,.~.: ~:8:;a::.+:...:.,3;;:{,>.,.#:S..h. 0.5, the overall probability of error, 1 - P(corr), for the 5-bit majority-vote code is shown in Figure 3. As can be seen, the error rate decreases dramatically as higher P values are reached. (U) As indicated earlier, a critical element of the procedure is to arrange for the five subtrials to constitute independent attempts at target specification. The procedure chosen is one that is especially amenable to implementation by the use of a programmable calculator-- here one of the HP-41C series. The calculator program used is provided in Table 3. (U) Taking 0 and 1 to represent the two possible outcomes of interest, a ball located in the red bin or black bin, respectively, were one to simply enter a series of 0's and 1's into a calculator memory to represent selections, the individual entries would not likely constitute independent events. To overcome this shortcoming, the calculator is programmed such that the 0 and 1 buttons used to enter selections do not, in fact, provide direct access to the 0 and 1 memory registers, respectively. Instead, an internal random number generator relabels the 0 and 1 buttons, subtrial to subtrial, on a random basis.. The RVer's task, therefore, reduces to one of simply pressing the "right" button, subtrial to subtrial, to accumulate entries in the "right" memory location. As a result, the five subtrials are effectively independent by virtue of the random renumbering of the buttons. Although such a procedure may seem somewhat abstract as a means to implement the binary search procedure, the * (U) Slepian, D., "A Class of Binary Signaling Alphabets," The Bell System Technical Journal, Vol. 35, pp. 203-234 (January 1956). (2) -15- Approved For Release 20~'~1~~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20q,OjQ$/~?QL~I~~~~~~Q,p787R000300030001-7 w a A PRIORI PROBABILITY, P, OF CORRECTLY PERCEIVING A SINGLE BIT FIGURE 3 (U) PROBABILITY OF ERROR, PE, WITH AND WITHOUT USE OF A SIMPLE 5-BIT MAJORITY-VOTE CODE (U) potential for increasing accuracy on the basis of multiple choices per decision, statistically averaged, is considered well worth the effort. (U) As a subtrial series is carried out, the calculator registers the inputs until three entries have been accumulated in either the 0 or 1 memory register (which takes as few as - 16 - Approved For Release 20~saV4~!QIf~~RDP9~6~6~0787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20g0~Q$/~,~p;:~I~~pe~,Qp787R000300030001-7 @1~LBL 'BLOCK H' 44 'CHOICE=' 87 ~ @2 'BLK L?' 45 RRGL 31 88 1@@ 83 PROMPT 46 AVIEW 89 84 STO @6 47 STOP 98 TONE 6 @5 2 48 'RHSWER?' 91 '~ BLK HITS=' 86 STO 29 49 PROMPT 92 RRGL X 87 TIME 58 STO 87 93 R9IEW @$ LN 51 RCL 31 94 STOP @9 RBS 52 RCL 87 95 9 1@ STO 3@ 53 X=Y? 9b STO 86 11 @ 54 GTO 'BLK HIT' 97 STO @1 12 STO @6 55 RCL 31 98 1 13 STO 81 56 1 99 ST+ 85 14 STO @2 57 + 18@ GTO 'BLK BIT' 15 STO 63 58 RCL 29 . 181 .EHD. lE STO @4 59 MUD @1~LBL 'RNG SUB' 17 1 68 STO 32 @2 9821 iS STO @5 61 RGL IHD 32 @3 RCL 38 19~LBL 'BLK BIT' 62 GTO 'END' @4 * 28 XEQ 'RHG SUB' 63~LBL 'BLK HIT' 85 .211327 21 'READY' 64 1 86 + 22 TONE 7 65 ST+ 84 87 FRG 23 TONE 6 b6 RCL IHD 31 88 5T0 36 24 PROMPT 67~LBL 'END' @9 RCL 29 p5 + 68 ST+ B3 18 26 RCL 29 69 TORE 6 I1 INT 27 MOD 7@ 'TRIAL HR=? 12 EHD 2$ STO 31 71 ARGL 85 29 1 3@ ST+ IHD 31 72 AVIEW 73 STOP 31 ST+ @? 74 FIX 1 32 RCL 86 75 RGL 93 33 1 76 RGL 82 34 + 77 ~ 35 2 78 18@ 36 ~ 37 RCL IHD 31 79 8@ TONE b 38 X=Y? 39 GTO ?BLKDONE' 4@ GTO 'BLK BIT' 41~LBL 'BLKDOHE' 81 ': BIT HITS=' 82 RRGL X 83 AYIEW 84 STOP 42 FIX @ 85 RCL @4 43 BEEP 8b RCL B5 -17- Approved For Release 20~08~~~Cfxf~l7P~~0787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20QQ/~$~~~~;~gP~,6;.Q0787R000300030001-7 (u) three, and up to five entries), at which point the calculator program announces the overall trial result. (U) To date, two RVers have completed a 100-trial series under the protocols described; their results are summarized in Table 4 and in Figures 4 and 5. In the figures, cumulative percentage hits are plotted for both the subtrial data and the majority-vote trial data. As is evident by inspection, the 5-bit majority-vote procedure amplifies the statistical advantage of the subtrial entries, as could be expected in the case of extra-chance RV performance. In particular, whereas only one of the two RVer's subtrial data displayed in Table 4 were significant [evaluated by Eq. (1)], both RVer's 5-bit majority-vote data independently reached significance. Percipient Summary Statistics (subtrials) Summary Statistics (5-bit majority-vote trials) ~k642 R = 52.6% (219/416) R = 60.0% (60/100) p 0.14 p = 0.028 ~'k730 R = 55.0% (220/400) R = 60.0% (60/100) p = 0.023 p = 0.028 Combined R = 53.8% (439/816) R = 60.0% (120/200) Data p = 0.015 p = 2.8 x 10-3 (U) The use of a calculator as a convenient on-line psi-amplification tool appears to hold promise as a first step in the development of a binary search procedure. The study shows that the accuracy of the individual bit choices can be amplified by the use of the statistical averaging procedure described. The level of accuracy reached (60 percent) although statistically significant, needs to be strengthened by further research before binary search meets minimal requirements for application efforts. - 18 - Approved For Release 200~~~OL~~~~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007878000300030001-7 UNCLASSIFIED 70 ~-- U i- 5-BIT MAJORITY-VOTE CODE GLE-BIT RATE) 219/416 (52.6%) eon ~ I ~ I ~ I I I I I I 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 UNCLASSIFIED TRIAL NUMBER FIGURE 4 (U) RANDOM BINARY TASK PERFORMANCE, PERCIPIENT #642 60/100 (60%) ip = 2.8 X 10-2) ti - i - TY-VOTE CODE Approved For Release 20f~~CpL~~~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007878000300030001-7 UNCLASSIFIED 60/100 (60%) NO MAJORITY-VOTE CODE (SINGLE-BIT RATE) 0~ I ~ I 0 20 220/400 (55%) I ~ I ~ I ~ I 't' 40 60 80 100 120 TRIAL NUMBER FIGURE 5 (U) RANDOM BINARY TASK PERFORMANCE, PERCIPIENT #730 Approved For Release 200~~~OL~i~g~!~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Releas~2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-007$7R000300030001-7 F. (U) Computer Assisted Search (CAS) To determine the applicability of computers as aids in the general search problem, we have developed a graduated se_r_ies of studies-beginning at a basic level of investigation and evolving toward real-time! ~; applications. As a first step in determining the degree to which a computer can be used as an aid in search problems, an experiment was conducted that demonstrated that an abstract computer-generated target .could be located by psychoenergetic means. The next stage involved testing whether an "association" between an abstract computer "target" and an actual target could be established. 1. (U) Basic Investigation (Simulation) (U) We have conducted an experiment to determine if an abstract computer- generated target can be located by psychoenergetic means. To accomplish this, we designed an experiment that would also provide information with regard to two possible mechanisms: ? To search in space for a target that remains fixed for the duration of a trial. ? To search for a target that is rapidly moving in space. (U) The first case is the more familiar "dowsing" situation; a target, whose location is unknown, must be [ound by psychoenergetic means. The second case was established on the basis of earllier research at SRI. Namely, that it is possible for an individual to initiate an action a1 the proper time to optimize the result of a psychoenergetic experiment. (U) During the CAS experiment, seven individuals were asked to contribute 50 trials each. To test the "dowsing" hypothesis, the target location was fixed throughout a trial. To test the timing hypothesis, the target location was changed once each millisecond. Dynamic .versus static target trials were determined by a balanced random protocol that was the same for each of the participants. Furthermore, they were unaware that there were two test conditions. Abounded area representing the perimeter of a 20 X 20 cell matrix was shown to the participant, who could register his/her response by moving a graphics pointing device (mouse), and could press a button to indicate the choice. Each participant was told that the target could be any place within the display boundary, and when the moment "seemed right," could register his/her choice by pressing the button on the mouse. Each Approved For Releas~ 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-Op787R000300030001-7 Approved For Released 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-00'~87R000300030001-7 participant was constrained to provide a few trials (< 5) at each sitting. The analysis that was used is described in Section III.A. (U) (U) From a purely mechanistic point of view, we might expect that locating a dynamically-moving target would be "easier" than a static one. Because the target is moving once a millisecond, the target is, on the average, actually "under" the mouse button twice each second. Thus, two times a second, the participant has the opportunity to locate the target correctly. Helshe must, however, press the mouse button at the proper time (finding a dynamically-moving target translates to finding a moment in time to initiate a response). In the static case, it is possible (worst case) that the location will never be under the mouse button, because the RVer, in moving the button display around the screen, misses the actual location. Thus, we would expect a bias in favor of finding the dynamic target simply because the number of opportunities of registering the correct response is greater. (U) The results of the basic investigation are shown in Table 5. Five of the seven participants produced significant evidence of psychoenergetic functioning (p < 0.0004). Of these five, three produced results favoring the timing hypothesis, and two favored the "dowsing" hypothesis. Two participants produced significant differences between temporal and spatial "dowsing;" one (#531) favoring temporal, the other (#807) favoring spatial. Participant 531 showed a 27 percent reduction from the mean chance expectation (MCE) distance of 10.4 units, while Participant 807 showed a 16.1 percent reduction. Overall, there is strong evidence that individuals can locate abstract computer generated targets, yet there is no evidence that there is a preference for the timing technique. 2. (U) Location of Real-World Targets (Known) f ~ The first step in determining if the CAS technique is capable of locating unknown targets of interest, is to demonstrate that the technique can be applied to real-world targets whose locations are known. This step has been taken under _ controlled test conditions, as described in Section G. -22- Approved For Releas 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96- 07878000300030001-7 Approved For Release X000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-007J87R000300030001-7 (U) RESULTS OF BASIC INVESTIGATION (p-values) RVer Space Time Difference* 531 .560 .009t ,038t 240 .365 .042t .164 859 .528 .0421 .100 452 .752 .911 .691 310 .184 .363 .652 807 .047t .994* .998* 164 .031t .295 .826 *Tested against the hypothesis that time > space. tSignificant $Target avoidance. 3. (U) CAS Against Real Targets (Application) ,. As a final test, can CAS techniques be used to locate unknown targets ~ The use of both static and dynamic techniques will be considered. (U) In the case of the latter, a useful technique is to divide the search space into N regions. A counter is incremented once each millisecond as before, but it begins at one and recycles after N + I. The extra cell is used to cover the case of "none of the above". To illustrate how this technique might work, let us consider the case of a kidnap victim who is suspected of being held somewhere in the west Beruit area. This area is divided into 20 regions of interest. (The CAS counter then cycles through 21.) A large number of trials are collected for a given participant, and a large number of participants could be used. Statistics developed during the known target portion of the study would be used as an input to the Approved For Release - 23 - .~- 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-00 87800030003000.1-7 ~~~~~ ~ Approved For Release~1000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96- 07878000300030001-7 (u) `~ .-~ evaluation program to determine the most likely location. Should this technique prove successful, it has wide application beyond the spatial location of targets of interest. Because the CAS algorithm only involves selecting the proper moment to register a response, the meaning of the numbers used in the cycle counter is completely arbitrary, and thus can correspond to the elements of any discrete search problem. G, ~--Controlled Long-Distance Test of "Agent"/Building Search, Facility Level (Continuum) As a measure of progress made during the contract effort, a test was 1 proposed in which the Search team put together by SR[ would attempt to locate targets For this purpose two experimental series were designed and can?ied out. 1. (U) Long-Distance "Agent" Search t ~-'~~~ ~ For the first test series, t personnel chose a series o[ ten locations) __j. Each of the ten locations were to be visited, in turn, for one hour, at predetermined times, h.} an `ta9~'l""` ~~ .1`4embers of the Search team where given a photograph of the "agent" to be located, a listing of the -- , target times (1100 and 1600, EST, on 28-30 i\'ovember, and on 3--i Dec.), and a map ' rnarked with a 1.5 km X 1.5 km square indicating the overall target area (U) Fuur Search R~'ers were chosen for this task (\`os. 16-1, 658, 198, and 232), three of whom demonstrated success in other search tasks covered in this report, the fourth (#232) being reported as having been successful in experiments conducted in a training class taught by one of the others (#198). During the time-frame of the experiment, two of the RVers were located in California (\`os.16-1 and 688), while the remaining two were in Florida. Thus, the experiment was carried out over baselines of several thousand km. (; The experiment was carried out with the SRI researchers blind as to the target locations (that is, the experiment was double blind.) At the end of the targeting period, the search-determined locations were transmitted ~ ~ after which a list of the actual target locations used were made availaUle to the SRI team. ?~ Approved For Release 200/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-007$7800030003000.1-7 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007878000300030001-7 UNCLASSIFIED (U) In the 2.25 km square area, the mean chance expectation (MCE) distance is 780 m. Of the four Search RVers, all generated lesser mean distances (see Table 6), and the results of three of the RVers are each independently statistically significant. In the forty trials generated (4 RVers X 10 trials each), 20 percent (8) yielded target-response distances within 3 squares (225 m) on the 400-square matrix used for analysis; this includes a direct hit which is expected only once in 400 trials by chance. The overall combined probability for this 40-trial series can be determined from the raw data P < 2.5 x 10 -4 . Thus, this test under client-controlled double-blind conditions yields convincing evidence that psychoenergetic search processes can to a statistically significant degree be used as a viable search tool. (U) LONG-DISTANCE "AGENT" SEARCH: CLIENT-DETERMINED TARGET SITES (MCE Distance = 780 m) Mean Target Area RVer Response p Reduction Distance 164 391 m 75% 2.62 X 10 s 688 616 m 38% 0.39* 198 447 m 67% 1.45 X 10 2 232 492 m 60% 4.65 X 10 2 (U) An additional facet that surfaced in the above application was the fact that, due to scheduling conflicts, some percentage of the search attempts were carried out at some Approved For Release 200~'~~,~I~~~?7878000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-007878000300030001-7 UNCLASSIFIED (U> time following the target period, ranging from as little as a few minutes to as much as two days. In these cases, the RVer nonetheless concentrated on the scheduled past time period, rather than on the (present) time of the actual search attempt. Detailed examination of the data generated by the three significant RVers shows that although 40 percent each of their data was produced on this retrospective basis, there was no statistically significant difference between the real-time and retrospective conditions (one-way analysis of variance, df 1 = 1, df2 = 8, F = 0.54, 2.40, and 1.51, respectively, for RVers 164, 198 and 232; F = 5.32 required for significant differences at the p = 0.05 level). (U) The dynamic CAS technique was used to attempt to locate z building at the sponsor's facility, that building being known only to the sponsor. A map of the sponsor's facility, unlabeled as to buildings, was divided into 20 numbered equal-area rectangles in a 4 X 5 matrix, (4 columns, 5 rows). Thus, starting in the upper left corner, the upper row contained rectangles 1 - 4; the second row, 5 - 8; third row 9 - 12; fourth row, 13 - 16; fifth (bottom) row, 17 - 20. (U) An incremental counter was used to cycle through the numbers 1 - 20 at a rate of 1 millisecond each. A search RVer's task was to interrupt the counter (by pressing the "mouse" button) at the number corresponding to the map area that contains the building designated as "target" by the sponsor, given only the name of the building (Whitesell building). For each RVer, a histogram is constructed corresponding to the number of times he/she selects each of the numbers. (U) The two individuals (531 and 859) who had done well in the abstract experiment in timing were asked to provide 50 trials each to start; in the event an individual's selections resulted in a tie or ties for amaximally-selected rectangle, trials were. added in 5-trial blocks until a single choice was outstanding. In the case of 531, 55 trials were required to meet this criterion; for 859, 75 were required. (U) In Figure 6 we display the histograms for each participant. For RVer 531, the expected number of choices for each rectangle is 55/20 = 2.75; the maximally-selected rectangle (#17) was chosen 6 times, the second choice (#14) was chosen 5 times. For RVer 859, the expected number of choices for each rectangle is 75/20 = 3.75; the maximally-selected (#6) was chosen 7 times, the second choice (nos. 4 and 18, a tie ) -26- Approved For Release 200~'~L~~~~~787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 20QQ~0$/1R ~I~;~,Pf~,,6:,Q0787R000300030001-7 (u) was chosen 6 times. Following RVer selections, the answer (rectangle #15) was provided to the experimenters, who, in turn, provided it to the search RVers as feedback. (U) With regard fo evaluation of the results, as in the previous section one can use the 400-square matrix to calculate the probability of observed separations between target and response rectangle centers as compared with the MCE distance. For RVer 531, the first-choice selection yields atarget/response rectangle separation distance somewhat greater than MCE (3.4% ), while the second choice yields a better result, a 52.0% reduction from MCE. For RVer 859, the first-choice selection yields a 9.5% reduction from MCE, while the second (a tie) yields one result at 38.5% reduction from MCE, a second at 24.8% greater than MCE distance. Given the small-sample nature of this particular pilot test of the CAS technique, it is not possible to draw meaningful statistical conclusions. We are nonetheless encouraged by observation of relatively large reductions from MCE among the top two selections for each of the search RVers, and therefore consider this procedure a promising candidate for further development. Number of Choices Map Position -27- Approved For Release 200~'~ ~.~~~~?787R000300030001-7 Approved For Release 2,000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0078~R000300030001-7 V SUMMARY (U) The research effort described in this report addresses chef requirement. Ito develop techniques to locate of interest whose positions are not known, or are known only approximately. Specifically, we have investigated, the possible use of psychoenergetic search techniques as an adjunct to other technological means. Experiments have been carried out, examining several promising methods that have emerged in the field over the past decade or so. Care has been taken to assess the various procedures under strict double-blind conditions, and have included successful application to the location of targets under .client control over baselines of several thousand km. B. (U) Focus of Investigation (U) In this study the search techniques under consideration have been investigated in applications ranging from the locations of objects hidden in the same building as the search RVer, to the search for an individual or building over transcontinental baselines. The methods examined range from simple one-step map marking ("map dowsing"), to calculator- and computer-assisted search involving sophisticated statistical averaging techniques. (U) Strong evidence emerged in this study that certain individuals using certain techniques were capable of narrowing down the area of target location to a statistically significant degree, indicating the basic viability of the psychoenergetic approach as a search tool. Approved For Releas 2000/08/10 :CIA-RDP96-0 7878000300030001-7 R i Approved For Release 20QQ/Q$/,1,Q ~~,R~R~6~Q0787R000300030001-7 C. (U) Recommendations for Follow-On Actions (U) Given the quality of response to the search task, and the observations emerging from detailed examination of various facets of that response, the following recommendations for follow-on actions are offered: 1. Since various individuals showed "preferences" for various tasks (as far as their capabilities were concerned), examine personality profiles obtained for these individuals, and determine by testing with other individuals if personality structure is an important factor. 2. Given the apparent relative insensitivity to RVer-target baseline distance emerging in this pilot work, examine this variable in depth in further studies. 3. Given the apparent relative insensitivity to temporal distance between RVer and target event (at least for retrospective conditions) observed in this pilot work, examine this variable in further depth. 4. Investigate further the power of statistical averaging techniques to determine whether the modest amplification obtained with such techniques to date can be improved significantly. 5. Follow up on statements by Search RVers that success is often associated with certain physiological sensations (e.g., "stickiness" in the fingers during "map dowsing"), by investigating the possibility of using physiological-correlate measures to obtain a more sensitive indicator of "signal" detection. -29- Approved For Release 2d6U10~.1~~~6~~0787R000300030001-7