INFORMATION TRANSMISSION UNDER CONDITIONS OF SENSORY SHIELDING
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
K
Document Page Count:
19
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
September 1, 2000
Sequence Number:
1
Case Number:
Content Type:
RS
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8.pdf | 865.68 KB |
Body:
INFORMATION TRANSMISSION UNDER CONDITIONS OF
SENSORY SHIELDING
by
Dr. Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ
Stanford Research Institute
Menlo Park, California 940Z5
Published in NATURE Magazine, October, 1974
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-.RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
We present results of experiments suggesting the existence of one or more
perceptual modalities through which. individuals obtain information about their
environment, although this information is not presented to any known sense. The
literature1'-3 and our observations lead us to conclude that such abilities can be
studied under laboratory conditions.
We have investigated the ability of certain people to describe graphical
material or remote scenes shielded against ordinary perception. In addition, we
performed pilot studies to determine if electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings
might indicate perception of, remote happenings even in the absence of correct
overt responses.
We concentrated on what we consider to be our primary responsibility- -to
resolve under conditions as unambiguous as possible the basic issue of whether
a certain class of paranormal perception phenomena exists. So we conducted our
experiments with sufficient control, utilising visual, acoustic and electrical
shielding, to ensure that all conventional paths of sensory input were blocked.
At all times we took measures to prevent sensory leakage and to prevent decep-
tion, whether intentional or unintentional.
Our goal is not just to catalogue interesting events, but to uncover patterns
of cause-effect relationships that lend themselves to analysis and hypothesis in
the forms with which we are familiar in scientific study. The results presented
here constitute a first step towards that goal; we have established under known
conditions a data base from which departures as a function of physical and psycho-
logical variables can be studied in future work.
First, we conducted experiments with Mr. Uri Geller in which we examined
his ability, while located in an electrically shielded room, to reproduce target
pictures drawn by experimenters located at remote locations. Second, we con-
ducted double-blind experiments with Mr. Pat Price, in which we measured his
ability to describe remote outdoor scenes many miles from his physical location.
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Finally, we conducted preliminary tests using EEGs, in which subjects were
asked to perceive whether a remote light was flashing, and to determine whether
a subject could perceive the presence of the light, even if only at a noncognitive
level of awareness.
In preliminary testing Geller apparently demonstrated an ability to repro-
duce simple pictures (line drawings) which had been drawn and placed in opaque
sealed envelopes which he was not permitted to handle. But since each of the
targets was known to at least one experimenter in the room with Geller, it was
not possible on the basis of the preliminary testing to discriminate between
Geller's direct perception of envelope contents and perception through some
mechanism involving the experimenters, whether paranormal or subliminal.
So we examined the phenomenon under conditions designed to eliminate all
conventional information channels, overt or subliminal. Geller was separated
from both the target material and anyone knowledgeable of the material, as in the
experiments of ref. 4.
In the first part of the study a series of 13 separate drawing experiments
were carried out over 7 days. No experiments were deleted from the results
presented here.
At the beginning of the experiment either Geller or the experimenters entered
a shielded room so that from that time forward Geller was at all times visually,
acoustically, and electrically shielded from personnel and material at the target
location. Only following Geller's isolation from the experimenters was a target
chosen and drawn, a procedure designed to eliminate pre-experiment cueing.
Furthermore, to eliminate the possibility of pre-experiment target forcing, Geller
was kept ignorant as to the identity of the person selecting the target and as to
the method of target selection. This was accomplished by the use of three different
techniques: (1) pseudo-random technique of opening a dictionary arbitrarily and
choosing the first word that could be drawn (Experiments 1-4); (2) targets, blind
to experimenters and subject, prepared independently by SRI scientists outside
the experimental group (following Geller's isolation) and provided to the experi-
menters during the course of the experiment (Experiments 5-7, 11-13); and (3)
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
arbitrary selection from a target pool decided upon in advance of daily experi-
mentation and designed to provide data concerning information content for use in
testing specific hypotheses (Experiments 8-10). Geller's task was to reproduce
with pen on paper the line drawing generated at the target location. Following a
period of effort ranging from a few minutes to half an hour, Geller either passed
(when he did not feel confident) or indicated he was ready to submit a drawing to
the experimenters, in which case the drawing was collected before Geller was
permitted to see the target.
To prevent sensory cueing of the target information, Experiments 1 through
10 were carried out using a shielded room in SRI's facility for EEG research.
The acoustic and visual isolation is provided by a double-walled steel room, locked
by means of an inner and outer door, each of which is secured with a refrigerator-
type locking mechanism. Following target selection when Geller was inside the
room, a one-way audio monitor, operating only from the inside to the outside,
was activated to monitor Geller during his efforts. The target picture was never
discussed by the experimenters after the picture was drawn and brought near the
shielded room. In our detailed examination of the shielded room and the protocol
used in these experiments, no sensory leakage has been found.
The conditions and results for the 10 experiments carried out in the shielded
room are displayed in Table 1 and Fig. 1. All experiments except 4 and 5 were
conducted with Geller inside the shielded room. In Experiments 4 and 5, the pro-
cedure was reversed. For those experiments in which Geller was inside the
shielded room, the target location was in an adjacent room at a distance of about
4 m, except for Experiments 3 and 8, in which the target locations were, respec-
tively, an office at a distance of 475 m and a room at a distance of about 7 m.
A response was obtained in all experiments except Numbers 5-7. In Experiment
5, the person-to-person link was eliminated by arranging for a scientist outside
the usual experimental group to draw a picture, lock it in the shielded room before
Geller's arrival at SRI, and leave the area. Geller was then led by the experi-
menters to the shielded room and asked to draw the picture located inside the
room. He said that he got no clear impression and therefore did not submit a
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
drawing; The elimination of the person-to-person link was examined further in
the second series of experiments with this subject.
Experiments 6, and 7 were carried out while we attempted to record Geller's
EEG during his efforts to perceive the target pictures. The target pictures were,
respectively, a tree and an envelope. He found it difficult to hold adequately still
for good EEG records, said that he experienced difficulty in getting impressions
of the targets, and again submitted no drawings.
Experiments 11 through 13 were carried out in SRI's Engineering Building,
to make use of the computer facilities available there. For these experiments,
Geller was secured in a double-walled, copper-screen Faraday cage 54 m down
the hall and around the corner from the computer room. The Faraday cage provides
120 dB attenuation for plane wave radio frequency radiation over a range of 15 kHz
to 1 GHz. For magnetic fields the attenuation is 68 dB at 15 kHz and decreases
to 3 dB at 60 Hz. Following Geller's isolation, the targets for these experiments
were chosen by computer laboratory personnel not otherwise associated with either
the experiment or Geller, and the experimenters and subject were kept blind as
to the contents of the target pool.
For Experiment 11, a picture of a kite was drawn on the face of a cathode
ray tube display screen, driven by the computer's graphics programme. For Experi-
ment 12, a picture of a church was drawn and stored in the memory of the computer.
In Experiment 13, the target drawing, an arrow through a heart (Fig. 2c), was
drawn on the face of the cathode ray tube and then the display intensity was turned
off so that no picture was visible.
To obtain an independent evaluation of the correlation between target and
response data, the experimenters submitted the data for judging on a 'blind' basis
by two SRI scientists who were not otherwise associated with the research. For
the 10 cases in which Geller provided a response, the judges were asked to match
the response data with the corresponding target data (without replacement). In
those cases in which Geller made more than one drawing as his response to the
target, all the drawings were combined as a set for judging. The two judges
each matched the target data to the response data with no error. For either judge
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
such a correspondence has an a rp iori probability, under the null hypothesis of
no information channel, of p = (10!)-i = 3 x 10-7.
A second series of experiments was carried out to determine whether direct
perception of envelope contents was possible without some person knowing of the
target picture.
One hundred target pictures of everyday objects were drawn by an SRI artist
and sealed by other SRI personnel in double envelopes containing black cardboard.
The hundred targets were divided randomly into groups of 20 for use in each of
the three days' experiments.
On each of the three days of these experiments, Geller passed. That is, he
declined to associate any envelope with a drawing that he made, expressing dis-
satisfaction with the existence of such a large target pool. On each day he made
approximately 12 recognisable drawings, which he felt were associated with the
entire target pool of 100. On each of the three days, two of his drawings could
reasonably be associated with two of the 20 daily targets. On the third day, two
of his drawings were very close replications of two of that day's target pictures.
The drawings resulting from this experiment do not depart significantly from
what would be expected by chance.
In a simpler experiment Geller was successful in obtaining information under
conditions in which no persons were knowledgeable of the target. A double-blind
experiment was performed in which a single 3/4 inch die was placed in a 3 x 4 x 5
inch steel box. The box was then vigorously shaken by one of the experimenters
and placed on the table, a technique found in control runs to produce a distribution
of die faces differing nonsignificantly from chance. The orientation of the die
within the box was unknown to the experimenters at that time. Geller would then
write down which die face was uppermost. The target pool was known, but the
targets were individually prepared in a manner blind to all persons involved in the
experiment. This experiment was performed ten times, with Geller passing twice
and giving a response eight times. In the eight times in which he gave a response,
he was correct each time. The distribution of responses consisted of three 2s,
one 4, two 5s, and two 6s. The probability of this occurring by chance is approxi-
5
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
mately one in 106.
In certain situations significant information transmission can take place
under shielded conditions. Factors which appear to be important and there fore
candidates for future investigation include whether the subject knows the set of
targets in the target pool, the actual number of targets in the target pool at any
given time, and whether the target is known by any of the experimenters.
It has been widely reported that Geller has demonstrated the ability to bend
metal by paranormal means. Although metal bending by Geller has been observed
in our laboratory, we have not been able to combine such observations with ade-
quately controlled experiments to obtain data sufficient to support the paranormal
hypothesis.
A study by Osis5 led us to determine whether a subject could describe ran-
domly chosen geographical sites located several miles from the subject's position
and demarcated by some appropriate means (remote viewing). This experiment
carried out with Price, a former California police commissioner and city council-
man, consisted of a series of double-blind, demonstration-of-ability tests involv-
ing local targets in the San Francisco Bay area which could be documented by
several independent judges. We planned the experiment considering that natural
geographical places or man-made sites that have existed for a long time are more
potent targets for paranormal perception experiments than are artificial targets
prepared in the laboratory. This is based on subject opinions that the use of arti-
ficial targets involves a 'trivialisation of the ability' as compared with natural
pre-existing targets.
In each of nine experiments involving Price as subject and SRI experimenters
as a target demarcation team, a remote location was chosen in a double-blind
protocol. Price, who remained at SRI, was asked to describe this remote location,
as well as whatever activities might be going on there.
Several descriptions yielded significantly correct data pertaining to and
descriptive of the target location.
In the experiments a set of twelve target locations clearly differentiated from
each other and within 30 minutes driving time from SRI had been chosen from a
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
target-rich environment (more than 100 targets of the type used in the experimental
series) prior to the experimental series by an individual in SRI management, the
director of the Information Science and Engineering Division, not otherwise assoc-
iated with the experiment. Both the experimenters and the subject were kept blind
as to the contents of the target pool, which were used without replacement.
An experimenter was closeted with Price at SRI to wait 30 minutes to begin
the narrative description of the remote location. The SRI locations from which
the subject viewed the remote locations consisted of an outdoor park (Experiments
1, 2), the double-walled copper-screen Faraday cage discussed earlier (Experi-
ments 3, 4, and 6-9), and an office (Experiment 5). A second experimenter would
then obtain a target location from the Division Director from a set of travelling
orders previously prepared and randomised by the Director and kept under his
control. The target demarcation team (two to four SRI experimenters) then pro-
ceeded directly to the target by automobile without communicating with the sub-
ject or experimenter remaining behind. Since the experimenter remaining with
the subject at SRI was in ignorance both as to the particular target and as to the
target pool, he was free to question Price to clarify his descriptions. The demar-
cation team then remained at the target site for 30 minutes after the 30 minutes
allotted for travel. During the observation period, the remote-viewing subject
would describe his impressions of the target site into a tape recorder. A com-
parison was then made when the demarcation team returned.
Price's ability to describe correctly buildings, docks, roads, gardens, and
so on, including structural materials, color, ambience, and activity, sometimes
in great detail, indicated the functioning of a remote perceptual ability. But the
descriptions contained inaccuracies as well as correct statements. To obtain a
numerical evaluation of the accuracy of the remote viewing experiment, the exper-
imental results were subjected to independent judging on a blind basis by five SRI
scientists who were not otherwise associated with the research. The judges were
asked to match the nine locations, which they independently visited, against the
typed manuscripts of the tape-recorded narratives of the remote viewer. The
transcripts were unlabelled and presented in random order. The judges were
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
asked to find a narrative which they would consider the best match for each of
the places they visited. A given narrative could be assigned to more than one
target location. A correct match requires that the transcript of a given date be
associated with the target, of that date. Table 2 shows the distribution of the
judges' choices.
Among all possible analyses, the most conservative is a permutation
analysis of the plurality vote of the judges' selections assuming assignment with-
out replacement, an approach independent of the number of judges. By plurality
vote, six of the nine descriptions and locations were correctly matched. Under
the null hypothesis (no remote viewing and a random selection of descriptions
without replacement), this outcome has an a priori probability of p = 5. 6 x 10-4
since, among all possible permutations of the integers one through nine, the
probability of six or more being in their natural position in the list has that
value. Therefore, although Price's descriptions contain inaccuracies, the
descriptions are sufficiently accurate to permit the judges to differentiate among
the various targets to the degree indicated.
An experiment was undertaken to determine whether a physiological measure
such as EEG activity could be used as an indicator of information transmission
between an isolated subject and a remote stimulus. We hypothesised that per-
ception could be indicated by such a measure even in the absence of verbal or
other overt indicators. 6, 7
It was assumed that the application of remote stimuli would result in responses
similar to those obtained under conditions of direct stimulation. For example,
when normal subjects are stimulated with a flashing light, their EEG typically
shows a decrease in the amplitude of the resting rhythm and a driving of the brain
waves at the frequency of the flashes8. We hypothesised that if we stimulated one
subject in this manner ( a sender), the EEG of another subject in a remote room
with no flash present (a receiver), might show changes in alpha (9-11 Hz) activity,
and possibly EEG driving similar to that of the sender.
We informed our subject that at certain times a light was to be flashed in
a sender's eyes in a distant room, and if the subject perceived that event, con-
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
sciously or unconsciously, it might be evident from changes in his EEG output.
The receiver was seated in the visually opaque, acoustically and electrically
shielded double-walled steel room previously described. The sender was seated
in a room about 7 m from the receiver.
To find subjects who were responsive to such a remote stimulus, we ini-
tially worked with four female and two male volunteer subjects, all of whom
believed that success in the experimental situation might be possible. These were
designated 'receivers'. The senders were either other subjects or the experi-
menters. We decided beforehand to run one or two sessions of 36 trials each with
each subject in this selection procedure, and to do a more extensive study with
any subject whose results were positive.
A Grass PS-2 photostimulator placed about 1 m in front of the sender was
used to present flash trains of 10 s duration. The receiver's EEG activity from
the occipital region (Oz), referenced to linked mastoids, was amplified with a
Grass 5P-1 preamplifier and associated driver amplifier with a bandpass of 1-120
Hz. The EEG data were recorded on magnetic tape with an Ampex SP 300 recorder.
On each trial, a tone burst of fixed frequency was presented to both sender
and receiver, and was followed in one second by either a 10 s train off lashes or
a null flash interval presented to the sender. Thirty-six such trials were given
in an experimental session, consisting of 12 null trials - no flashes following the
tone - 12 trials of flashes at 6 f.p. s. and 12 trials of flashes at 16 f.p. s. , all
randomly intermixed, determined by entries from a table of random numbers.
Each of the trials generated an 11-s EEG epoch. The last 4 s of the epoch was
selected for analysis to minimise the desynchronising action of the warning cue.
This 4-s segment was subjected t o Fourier analysis on a LINC 8 computer.
Spectrum analyses gave no evidence of EEG driving in any receiver, al-
though in control runs the receivers did exhibit driving when physically stimu-
lated with the flashes. But of the six subjects studied initially, one subject (H. H. )
showed a consistent alpha blocking effect. We therefore undertook further study
with this subject.
Data from seven sets of 36 trials each were collected from this subject on
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
three separate days. This comprises all the data collected to date with this sub-
ject under the test conditions described above. The alpha band was identified from
average spectra, then scores of average power and peak power were obtained from
individual trials and subjected to statistical analysis.
Of our six subjects, H. H. had by far the most monochromatic EEG spectrum.
Figure 3 shows an overlay of the three averaged spectra from one of this subject's
36-trial runs, displaying changes in her alpha activity for the three stimulus con-
ditions.
Mean values for the average power and peak power for each of the seven
experimental sets are given in Table 3. The power measures were less in the
16 f. p. s. case than in the 0 f. p. s. in all seven peak power measures and in six
out of seven average power measures. Note also the reduced effect in the case
in which the subject was informed that no sender was present (Run 3). It seems
that overall alpha production was reduced for this run in conjunction with the sub-
ject's expressed apprehension about conducting the experiment without a sender.
This is in contrast to the case (Run 7) in which the subject was not informed.
Siegel's two-tailed t approximation to the nonparametric randomisation test9
was applied to the data from all sets, which included two sessions in which the
sender was removed. Average power on trials associated with the occurrence of
16 f.,,p. s. was significantly less than when there were no flashes (t= 2. 09,
d. f. = 118, P < 0. 04). The second measure, peak power, was also significantly
less in the 16 f. p. s. conditions than in the null condition ( t = 2. 16, d. f. = 118,
P < 0. 03). The average response in the 6 f. p. s. condition was in the same direc-
tion as that associated with 16 f. p. s. , but the effect was not statistically signi-
ficant.
Spectrum analyses of control recordings made from saline with a 12 k~2
resistance in place of the subject with and without the addition of a 10 Hz, 50 PV
test signal applied to the saline solution, revealed no indications of flash frequen-
cies, nor perturbations of the 10 Hz signal. These controls suggest that the results
were not due to system artifacts. Further tests also gave no evidence of radio
frequency energy associated with the stimulus.
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Subjects were asked to indicate their conscious assessment for each trial
as to which stimulus was generated. They made their guesses known to the experi-
menter via one-way telegraphic communication. An analysis of these guesses has
shown them to be at chance, indicating the absence of any supraliminal cueing. So,
arousal as evidenced by significant alpha blocking occurred only at the noncogni-
tive level of awareness.
We hypothesise that the protocol described here may prove to be useful as
a screening procedure for latent remote perceptual ability in the general popula-
tion.
From these experiments we conclude that:
A channel exists whereby information about a remote location can be obtained
by means of an as yet unidentified perceptual modality.
As with all biological systems, the information channel appears to be imper-
fect, containing noise along with the signal.
- While a quantitative signal-to-noise ratio in the information-theoretical
sense cannot as yet be determined, the results of our experiments indicate
that the functioning is at the level of useful information transfer.
It may be that remote perceptual ability is widely distributed in the general
population, but because the perception is generally below an individual's level of
awareness, it is repressed or not noticed. For example, two of our subjects (H.H.
and P. P.) had not considered themselves to have unusual perceptual ability before
their participation in these experiments.
Our observation of the phenomena leads us to conclude that experiments in
the area of so-called paranormal phenomena can be scientifically conducted, and
it is our hope that other laboratories will initiate additional research to attempt
to replicate these findings.
This research was sponsored by The Foundation for Parasensory Investiga-
tion, New York City. We thank Mrs. Judith Skutch, Dr. Edgar D. Mitchell of the
Institute of Noetic Sciences - as well as our SRI associates, Mr. Bonnar Cox,
Mr. Earle Jones and Dr. Dean Brown - for support and encouragement. Construc-
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
tive suggestions by Mrs. Jean Mayo, Dr. Charles Tart, University of California,
and Dr. Robert Ornstein and Dr. David Galin of the Langley Porter Neuropsychia-
tric Institute are acknowledged.
Russell Targ
Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory,
Stanford Research Institute,
Menlo Park, California 94025
1 Pratt, J. , Rhine, J. B. , Stuart, C. , and Greenwood, J. , Extra Sensory
Perception after Sixty Years (Henry Holt, New York, 1940).
2 Soal, S., and Bateman, F. , Modern Experiments in Telepathy (Faber and
Faber, London, 1954).
3 Vasilliev, L. L. , Experiments in Mental Suggestion (ISMI Publications,
Hampshire, England, 1963).
Musso, J. R. , and Granero, M. , J. Parapsychology, 37, 13-37 (1973).
Osis, K. , ASPR Newsletter, No. 14 (1972).
Tart, C. T. , Physiological Correlates of Psi Cognition, Int. J. Parapsychology,
V, No. 4 (1963).
7 Dean, E. D., Int. J. Neuropsychiatry 2 (1966).
8 Hill, D. , and Parr, G. , Electroencephalography: A Symposium on its
Various Aspects (Macmillan, New York, 1963).
9 Siegel, S. , Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, pp. 152-156
(McGraw-Hill, New York, 1956).
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
a4 ao 00
C G C
0) 44 hn
r-l r-1 r-1 r'-I r-1 r-I N N
b b b
z z? z
E
cn
0 r-I
O aJ r-I
ca > r-I - a~i E r co
C) A uoj E W U P4 c/
r-I r-i r-I , -I
I`r
r. r. 00 00 CO O' r-I r-I
00 00 00 00 CO 00 00 00
"O r. CO O\ O -I N M
-1 r-1 r-1 rl
o J., P -
0
4J 41 )a
) N a) J?J 4J 4J
+J U U O O
0 CO n 6 E 8
U
-I r-1 N N N
O O O O 0
~4 P P P >4
aJ aJ a) a) a)
b b b b b
r-I
rl r ?r r
C
Qj
(0
'C7
.
l
-1
?rl
,bJ
a
7,+
Cn
4J
rl
I
-I
4
0
rl
a)
rl
U
>
b
L4
a)
U
C,h
la
H
W
aJ
P4
w
a
cn
ca -
U
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Apprgyed For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
U
In Q)
N r-I
r. k p
N
O U) 4.1
U 0 ?r1 N Ca
O ,.C y
6
3 ra bQ ?r1 0
u O U
N O 1 a)
u >
^ ?r1 4J ?r1 Q)
0 4-I U
to c- .14 0
U m c0 O
?r1 1-I c) ,'3 U
O U bA
U SO-I
1-I b0
O 'c3 N
U : u
p ?n O
U) (U
O to a) P
bA ) a) D P
4J u 4-I U
?r1 U
? dJ p-4
4-3 'a
U) O U Ey CO
O c0 W 4 O II
?r1 r-I ~4 44 ~
4J a o rn W
U u w ?rl
a) . (L) 4-4
, 4 o U 4-1 C O cn
W C O ,U U
/, bA
+J ~~
P co
0 -r4 :3
o W
4-I 4J ?ri
N U 4 (1)
U a S:: 0 CU
O aJ
+) u ~+ o
44 W 4 0 fa 4-1
0 0 H u a 0
(M
q
W
pq
00
A
w
U
~
~`
A
PO W
a~
bC
PQ
?r1
LO
q
W
W
to
?rl
9
r
U
W
U
ra
M
W
0
q
N
A
V
r-I
~ A
N
M
n
CO
N
00
fA
O
0 :3
cl
z
O
y
N
E-1
4-)
ctl
N
r1
P4
r-4
r-4
E-i
a)
10
4)
i-)
w
k
U
?a 44
a)
04
V
Cd
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Approved For Release 2001/03/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100220001-8
Flash
Frequency
Average Power
Peak Power
Sender
0
6
16
u
ii
0
6
16
94.8
84.1
76.8
357.7
329.2
289.6
41.3
45.5
37.0
160.7
161.0
125.0
No Sender
(Subject: informed)
25.1
54.2
35.7
55.3
28.2
44.8
87.5
191.4
95.7
170.5
81.7
149.3
56.8
50.9
32.8
240.6
178.0
104.6
39.8
24.9
30.3
145.2
74.2
122.1
No Sender
(Subject not
86.0
53.0
52.1
318.1
180.6
202.3
informed)
Averages
56.8
49.9
43.1
11
214.5
169.8
153.5
-12% -24%
(P