MEMORANDUM FOR DD/S FROM ACQUISITIONS BRANCH

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP84-00780R001500050047-2
Release Decision: 
RIPPUB
Original Classification: 
K
Document Page Count: 
43
Document Creation Date: 
December 15, 2016
Document Release Date: 
February 20, 2003
Sequence Number: 
47
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
June 3, 1966
Content Type: 
MF
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP84-00780R001500050047-2.pdf6.85 MB
Body: 
Ap,jmq Ap I AUG 54 'Ul WHICH MAY BE USED. 1;41h: pproved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Next 3 Page(s) In Document Exempt Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 CENTRAL INTELLIGERtCE AGENCY WASHINGTON. D.C. 20505 This brochure is being distributed at the request of the Director, who feels that it has important and useful things to say about problems in communication. I can add to this my own view that this is the best "short course" on semantics I have seen. STATINTL R. J. SMITH Deputy Director for Intelligence Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : C144WP84-00780R00150j0050047-2 k'A IHER aLuMiivuna Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 IN TITE BEGIN r e'd For Release 2004/01/29: CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 WA.(4 THE WOItI) "AND" Perhaps all stories should begin with the word "and." Perhaps they should end with the word "and" too. It would remind us that no experience ever begins; there was always something that preceded it. What really began, for us, was our awareness of something going on. At the end, the word "and..." would remind us that no story ever really ends - something more will happen after. eve, Thus, it may be said that we live in the world of "etc." There is always more to start with than we can take, into account. There is always more to say than we can possibly say. There is' always more to end with than we can imagine. ev, You are .now invited to enter the world of etc. COPYRIGHT C 1965 BY THE KAISER ALUMINUM 8 CHEMICAL CORPORATION "Man," it has been said, "is the only creature on earth who can talk himself into trouble." After a couple of million years of practice, we've become pretty good at it. Think back over the last few days -if you can bear to do so-and chances are that you will find that at least some of your tensions, anx- ieties and frustrations arose from situations in which you did not really understand what someone said-or they did not seem to under- stand what you really meant. Perhaps: F a customer's order was improp- erly filled. E a letter or memo was addressed to the' wrong, place or person. . a lot of time was wasted on the wrong assignment. RI you never did get anything out of the conference you spent two hours in this morning. And at home? Well, you're in- clined to agree with the man who said, "There are only three races-men, women and chil- dren. And none of them speak the same language." What is upsetting about all this is that you may feel that you are a pretty good communicator-it's just that everyone else seems to do such a lousy job of it. After all, it should be perfectly.. easy, for us to under- stand. each other in our every day home and .business life. What .can the matter be Is it not at least possible that:;we: have taken our ability to communi* Cate.with each ;other for granted, Perhaps we have felt that it is a rather simple, natural process, and "once we learn the language, we should be able to understand each other pretty well. comnn,nicate and (?c if there ti-, any cl;ies that wilt ir'lp us us r stand each other a ittle better-'.' For the most par , the work of l a world gets done bi,-ause people it) cooperate with one another. I1'acn o us is almost whol v dependent u what other people (to for us. 'l'iar modern world is r of, a "j urn g.i rY competition" as sor e have de,criI it; it is more like a. rr ocean ot. co' p eration. The cooperation that mares I,u- man ;society pos ible is arlrnost wholly dependent -n the skill with which we commui icate. If we 1o not understand eal h other's nee+ls. we cannot fill them very well. So, it seems emin=ntly worthwh le to.examine the pro. ess by which we communicate. Perl aps we will fiid that when commun cation fails. it is not we who are al fault, but t}, at some part of the pr icess has broken down. And that is what we want to U-11k about on the foliom ing pages, Now the questio i rather reasrn- ably arises: Why rould a bus au s- r, firm like ours-exf ractors of oru~s, ?makers of metals and chemical::- discuss this at all? "And the best answer ve can thi.rk of is that we feel` ? hat all business ultimately comes clown to a trans- action between individual human beings. The success of that transac- tion depends almos entirely on how well they. understand each other The material c=n the following ."pages is intended -trot to answ er questions-but to stimulate thirrki ng .;,,about how we comr:runicate and how we might try to im prove the way we do it. If this, in t . irn, helps, a to understand your n -eds better-, t}; e we can hope to fill hem bett4_ r..'" itk our business will r"mnrove -.c ., at, Would it perhaps be useful to extent. examine the process by which we Per naps yours ti Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004101129---aA HOW IS IT WE KNOW SOMETHING TO COMMUNICATE? Here's looking at you-one way, at least. Instead of thinking of yourself as a "thing" in a world of "things" you might try to think of yourself as a whole lot of activities going on near some point in space and at this moment in time. At this "somewhere/somewhen" you are immersed in a great ocean of other happenings. The interactions between the "happening" that is you and the "happenings" that are NOT you, are the raw, basic stuff we try to communicate about. When you talk or write about something, what you are describing is those interactions that happened inside of you-not just what happened outside of you. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 3 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 The world outside us is believed to be made up of at least three levels of "happenings." Of these, we are able to experience only one level with our unaided senses. Visually, the level we do see appears to be made up of radiant light, such as is emitted from the stars and our sun, and the reflection of this light from the edges and surfaces of "things," which usually appear to us as patches and patterns of colors. What we "see" is not the "thing" itself, but a happening- the emission of light or the reflection of it. Beneath the edges and surfaces of "things," there is another layer of events that can be seen with special instruments, like microscopes and x-rays. What these instruments do is to bring this otherwise "invisible" world up to the visible world of colors, edges and patterns that we can experience. This microscopic layer appears to be a world of structures- which we might consider to be forces in equilibrium-and of motion- which we might describe as forces seeking equilibrium. Approved For Release 2004/01/29: CIA-RDP8 - - Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Beyond the microscopic layer is still another, which we believe to be made up of the interaction of electrical and magnetic forces in constant motion. We "see" this world, too, by bringing some of its effects up to the level of patterns of color and the edges of things- such as streaks on a photographic film, or a pointer on a dial. Thus, the world that we can experience directly is made up of patterns of color and edges of things, and it is the effect these have on us that we talk and write about. We react to only a few of all the waves of energy that ceaselessly pour in upon us from all directions. The spectrum of visible light-our "window on the world"- is only a tiny band out of all the waves of energy our instruments tell us are out there. And out of all the sound waves- the "music of the spheres"-that beat in upon us, our ears can pick up and process only a very little bit. What we can talk or write about is only a very small part of all that is going on "out there" .. . Approved For Release 2004101129 -. CIA-RDP84 00780ROO! _ 500060047 2 5 Approved For Release 2004/01/29: CIA 50005 0o047-2-- In a way, we "select" that part of the world that we want to experience at any one time. If we choose to stay indoors instead of going out, we have already selected one field to experience-and cut ourselves off from all the rest. And if we are in a room with four windows, we further narrow the field of our experience when we choose which window we want to look through. The particular place you are in, and the direction you choose to look, decide what experiences you are going to have. Since no two people can be in exactly the same spot at exactly the same time, all of our experiences are, to that extent, different. ev, Many of our problems in communication arise because we forget to remember that individual experiences are never identical. - ~p~srove For-Release- 20041 ' 1/29-:_CtA=RDP8 7&080015-00 50047-2- Another way we select the experience we are to have is by picking out some of the things that are in our field of vision and rejecting the others. These three photographs are all reproduced from the same negative-we show them as they might appear to: So, even when we are looking at "the same thing"-that is, in the same direction and from almost exactly the same spot- we still do not necessarily experience it the same way. (A good example is the quite different reactions people get from watching the same motion picture). A person needing to cash a check Someone who is late for an appointment What happens inside us when we see something? Two things, apparently. In the first place the light rays emitted or reflected from the outside events you have chosen to look at are focused on a tiny %-inch spot on the back wall of the eye. Here, they are changed into electrical impulses which then trigger a chain of electrical and chemical events in your nervous system. When you talk about what you have seen, what you talk about is that chain of events in your nervous system, its interaction with the object outside you. This experience-because of what you chose to look at, the time you chose to look at it, and the fact that the light rays that entered your eye never entered anyone else's, make this experience individual and unique. It is not exactly like that of anyone else. pprov - - 500050047-3- Approved For Release 2004/01/2 How then, can we ever discover what is similar in our individual experiences of the same outside event? Well, one theory is that, while any one experience is uniquely individual-the series of individual experiences is (or can be) nearly identical. If we walk around a chair, its shape will constantly change as we change the angles at which we look at it. If someone else then walks around the chair and looks at it in the same angles, he will have different individual experiences, but the series will be much the same for him as it was for us. Thus the succession of individual experiences enables us to agree upon what we have experienced, even though the individual experiences To a certain extent, we are "taught" what to see. The event which has not been experienced before does not "make sense." Successive experiences enable us finally to recognize the sameness of the sequence of experiences, even though individually they are different. For instance, take these pictures: This appears to be a cup and saucer 4. So does this ... And this ... CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 are somewhat different. If this were not true, effective communication would be almost impossible. When we talk to someone, we establish communication best by discovering what is common in the succession of our experiences, while keeping in mind that we may differ in our interpretation of any individual experience. Although we experience the world in bits and pieces, the sequence in which we experience them flows together and we feel the world around us as a continuous panorama. When we try to communicate about it, we have to break it down into bits and pieces. Perhaps a large part of our trouble starts there. Yet, considered as separate experiences, each image we form of the cup and saucer is entirely different. Somewhat the same thing applies to sounds. The interpretation of sounds as intelligible experience depends on our ability to recognize a sequence of patterned sound waves moving through time. Thus, we have to be taught to "see" what we see and to "hear" what we hear. Since we are each taught differently, the very basis of our understanding of what we see and hear differs to some extent from what others see and hear. This is one reason why verbal communications often are less satisfactory than written ones, because the spoken language allows of so many different intonations, pitches and variations. Two or more people, hearing the same sounds, do not experience nor interpret them the same way. When we assume that everyone sees or hears "the same thing," then we base our personal communication on a false and misleading premise. e Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780R00150005- 004--2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 A good way to show that we have to learn to see is the ink blot. Looking at the one here, different people will "see" different things in it; their internal experience of this event will tend to be different than that of other people looking at it. You will find, also, that if you look away from it, and then look back again, you may discover new shapes that give you new experiences that you had not seen before. When you are talking to other people, it is sometimes useful to keep in mind that if the experience you are talking about is new to them, they will have trouble "making sense" out of it, just as you perhaps had trouble "making sense" out of this ink blot when you first saw it. Much of our difficulty in introducing new ideas- and much of the very human resistance to change-arises from the fact that we have to learn what to experience in the events we experience. These are some oA &o(&jPF8 W 2004/O1/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 people have reported seeing in the ink Not on the preceding page. Did you see them? Looking back at the original ink blot, do you see them now? Have you perhaps been taught by other people what to see? When you are talking to someone, it is well to keep in mind that they may have been "taught" to experience things differently than you have, and perhaps the reason you have difficulty understanding them is that you are not allowing for this. Two Statues Two Birds Pecking Food 7 IF YOU USE YOUR IMAGINATION, YOU CAN SEE LOTS OF THINGS IN THE CLOUD FORMATIONS... U1HAT DO YOU THINK YOU SEE, LINUS? THAT CLOUD UP THERE LOOKS A LITTLE LIKE THE PROFILE OF THOMAS EAKINS, THE FAMOUS PAINTER AND SCULPTOR... UH HUH...THA1'5 VERY GOOD... WHAT DO YOU 5EE IN THE CLOUDS, CHARLIE BROWN? / WELL, THOSE CLOUDS UP THERE LOOK TOME LIKE THE MAP OF THE BRITISH HONDURAS ON THE CARIBBEAN.. AND THAT GROUP OF CLOUDS OVER THERE GIVES ME THE IMPRESSION OF THE STONING 0F STEPHEN ...I CAN 5EE THE APOSTLE PAVL STANDING THERE TO ONE SIDE... WELL, I WAS GOING TO SQY i71, Approved For Release 2004/01/29 CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 n ~ ,a r_r n 1 nnr n~iee._, C-I onoo~ nn7PflDfl .nnnenrL~~ n THE BLIND MEN ON THE ELEPHANT It was six men of Indostan To learning much inclined, Who went to see the Elephant (Though all of them were blind), That each by observation Might satisfy his mind. The First approached the Elephant, / And happening to fall Against his broad and sturdy side, / At once began to bawl: "God bless me! but the Elephant / Is very like a wall!" The Second, feeling of the tusk / Cried, "Ho! what have we here So very round and smooth and sharp? / To me'tis very clear This wonder of an Elephant / Is very like a spear!" The Third approached the animal And, happening to take The squirming trunk within his hands Thus boldly up he spake: "I see," quoth he, "the Elephant & Is very like a snake!" LVr1 The Fifth, who chanced to touch the ear, Said: "E'en the blindest man Can tell what this resembles most; Deny the fact who can This marvel of an Elephant Is very like a fan!" The Sixth no sooner had begun About the beast to grope Than, seizing on the swinging tail That fell within his scope. "I see," quoth he, "the Elephant is very like a rope!" The Fourth reached out an eager hand, And felt about the knee: "What most this wondrous beast is like Is very plain," quoth he; 'Tis clear enough the Elephant Is very like a tree!" P And so these men of Indostan / Disputed loud and long, Each in his own opinion / Exceeding stiff and strong. Though each was partly in the right, / They all were in the wrong! r 01 This is the composite elephant the blind men saw. It reminds us that the world we live in is built up of the quite different experiences that people have had, and which they have managed to communicate with each other. But let us also consider that if the blind men had exchanged places, and individually experienced that portion of the elephant that they had not felt previously, they ultimately would have been able to agree upon a much better looking elephant than this one. As we read, and listen, and experience, we establish the basis for a closer understanding of what others experience, and thus we can-in the end-find that we have much more to agree on than to disagree about. It is when we stop learning that we begin to build the barrier that keeps us from understanding other people-and perhaps finding common agreement with them. HOW DO WE CREATE SYMBOLS? So far, we have managed to get some part of some event happening outside-your-skin inside your skin. We have said that, as a result, you have had some kind of an experience inside of you, and you have tried to interpret it. rv., Now the question arises-how can you get this interpretation you have made back out again so that it will pass through the world outside-your-skin and possibly inside-some-one-else's-skin? How, indeed, can a purely personal, internally experienced event be shared with someone else? (For that is what we do when we communicate.) m, Well, for the most part, what you do is to create a new event in the world outside-your-skin. As we shall shortly see, this new event that you create has all the outside-your-skin "objectivity" of any other event in the world outside, and it is received by whomever you are talking or writing to, just like any other "real" event. But it is not the original event you experienced-and it is not the inner event you experienced. ee' The word (or symbol) you create is not the event that it reports on- it is a new event entirely. As far as we now know, there are three principal ways in which human individuals can communicate with each other: By actual physical touch We communicate this way nearly every day -we make ourselves understood with a tap on the shoulder a pat on the back a slap on the cheek .. and the ritualistic extension of the handshake. The physical pressure of some part of your body, or an exten- sion of it, acts as an outside event to stimulate responses in someone else's nervous system. We also communicate- by visible movements of some portions of our bodies... much of our daily communication occurs by means 'of a finger pointing a wink of an eye .. a nod of the head .. a shrug of the shoulders a smile ... grimace ... or scowl By moving some portion of our bodies in space, we change the angles of inci- dence and the angles of re- flection of visible light waves. These changes, if seen by someone else, are an event to them which they can inter- pret in the same way they would any other "outside" event. Approved For Release 2004/01/2 And we communicate by symbols, which "stand for" something we have experienced internally. We use audible symbols .. . Spoken symbols are "outside" events we create by directing our vocal muscles to vibrate a pulsed code, setting into motion similarly pulsed vibrations in the air. These are picked up as "real" outside events by the person we are talking to ... And we use visible symbols ... Visible symbols are created by manipulating our muscles to create patterns in some material that is not a part of our bodies. These patterns reflect visible light in unique ways, and can thus be distinguished from each other. To the person who sees them, they are real events that are experienced by him and can be 9: CIA-RDP84-00780R001500050 Most of our present day failures in communication can be traced either to misunderstandings of the role that symbols play in inter-human communication, or to inadequacies in the way that we create, transfer and perceive symbols-whether they are spoken or written. Perhaps if we had some clear-cut idea of what symbols do (remember, they are events, as we create and experience them, not things) we would find it easier to reduce or even eliminate some errors we make that lead to our misunderstanding each other. Here are some commonly used "visual" symbols : Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004ISM : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 O 9 aNv ~ NSTRUCT iJ?sA 'F L AS ous TodaySOUTi(5 NEGROES SLOWTOREGISTER: ,U.SHELPSOUGHT ~' x~r, r,aen u=K was E u?-eF k...euer i. In Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 15 any :1i.~e Since words, and AppldvediF)o R 1easeA04/01/2 seem to give us more trouble in communication than other types of visual symbols, let us see what a word appears to be when we consider it as a pattern that reflects wave lengths of visible light .. . We might try the word Star. The four configurations that make up the written word have 24 possible left-to-right combinations ... STAR TSAR SATR TASR SART TRSA SRAT TARS T AS STRA TSRA SRTA TRAS FAST Notice that only one of the configurations reflects the wavelength pattern we call Star. So, in the patterns we create in our nervous systems and transmit to our muscles, which move a pen or paint brush or typewriter keys-we are instructing our muscles to arrange something outside ourselves in a left-to-right code. It does not matter whether the image we thus create is big or small ... It does not matter, within wide limits, what type faces the word is set in ... It does not matter if the word is set in type or written out in script ... Approved For Release 2004/01/29 (t tFI$44W QOic5Q4O5W47e2d "Star" and as nothing else, because the relationship of each letter, from left to right, or from top to bottom, is precisely maintained. If we were to "program" instructions for writing the word "Star" we could say. . . I' " is always first whether the word is written from left or right, or fro in top to bottom. always appears immediately to the right, or just below "S". IT " always appears to the right, or below "ST". h" always appears to the right or below "STA". AR T A R S T A R S R S A R S T A R S T STAR'. ., , At, STA TA S T A R S T A R ARSTARSTARS RSTARSTARST We have to be taught to recognize these unique left-to-right patterns, in the same way we are taught to recognize a cup and saucer (as on page 8), no matter at what angle we view them, or under what lighting conditions, providing only that the edges remain visible. Thus, in the use of words for communication, it is the spelling-the rigid adherence to a "positional notation"-that is of the most importance. It is not the "meaning" because the meaning is not in the word, but in the person who is using it. Spellings change but slowly over a period of time, and when they do, the left-to-right uniqueness of the pattern remains much the same. Meanings change with speakers, regions, contexts, and time. In summary: Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 We translate an inner experience, inside our nervous system, into pulsed electrochemical codes that instruct our muscles to contract or expand in specific ways ... Our muscles translate these codes into actions that move objects outside-our-skin into patterns that have a prescribed left-to-right or up-to-down order in space... These patterns reflect wavelengths of light in the visible range. The wavelengths are transmitted to and received by other nervous systems in exactly the same way as any other event in space/time ... Received by another nervous system, these patterns of light waves form the basis for a new experience, which, in turn, may result in a new set of symbol responses. Much the same thing happens, of course, when we communicate with spoken language, except that we use sound waves instead of light waves. When we act as if we believed that a word symbol is the event that was originally experienced, we ignore all the steps that have made it something else.... Common words cannot possibly have meanings in themselves-only people can have meanings. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 IN SEARCH OF THE MEANING OF MEANING F rom now on we are going to talk about common words in everyday use, and not about all the other types of symbols and symbol systems that we use for communicating with each other ... we also will try to explore what is meant when we talk about "meaning"-for it is the transfer of meaning that is the goal of interhuman communications. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780R0015666666411 There are believed to rOj FOO$e1 ase 2004/01/29: CIA-RDP ?e ew to of 0 , 947-2 words in the English language today. The number is constantly growing, as we add new human experiences to be reported upon (through the use of extensions of our natural senses- telescopes, microscopes, spectroscopes, etc.) or as we coin new expressions to describe present experiences-hula hoops, m.e-tooism, high camp, etc. The number of words (other than technical ones connected with a business or profession) that an educated adult uses in. daily conversation is about 2,000. Of these, the 500 most frequently used have 14,000 dictionary definitions. This is a pitifully small number of symbols to describe the infinite richness and diversity of individual human experiences. *With apologies to C. K. Ogden and I. A. Richards who wrote a book called "The Meaning of Meaning." They found 16 groups of meanings for the word "meaning" in the English language. add new experiences for us to report Cyliernetics Min iaturiza?ioo Cryogenic i3ioceno'ogy Electrochenmis1e Old technologies we no longer draw experience from, whose words are disappear- ing from common use Alchemy Chandlery Heraldry 8lacksmithiriY Falconry Cannonry Old words once used to describe one experience, that are now used to describe other experiences Missile Compact Spectacular Twist Carpetbagger Maverick Gauntlet Approved For-ReIeeateo2004/01E/29 CIA-RDP84-0078OR001500050047-2 19 Approved or Release There are about 298 million English speaking people alive in the world today. To some extent, their individual experiences differ from all others. Every fraction of a second in their lives, they are experiencing something that is not exactly like any experience they have had before, or that anyone else has had. Yet they have the same meager store of accepted symbols to use in reporting to each other what they have experienced. Each common word/symbol must, therefore, necessarily be used to cover a wide range of "meanings." Let us look at one common word-perhaps you have used it once or more today- and see what a dictionary says it "means." Having glanced at what the Shorter Oxford Dictionary has to say about the "meaning" of LEAD-can you write down here the "real meaning" of the word? Of course you can't. (A dictionary does not tell us what common words "mean." A dictionary is a history of how a word has been used most fre- quently in some contexts and at different times. A dictionary in- dicates various areas of meaning, at various periods of time.) Lead (led), .eb.I ~OE. Mad sir. neut. = Du- lood lead, MHG. lot (mod .G. lot, loch) plum- met, also solder.] 1. The heaviest of the base metals, of a dull pale bluish-grey colour, easily fusible, soft and malleable. Chemical symbol Pb. Rarely p1. = kinds of lead. tb. Some- times called black lead (= L. plumbum nigrum) in contradistinction to white lead (plumbum album), a name for tin -1753. 2. See RED LEAD, WHITE LEAD. 3. Short for BLACK LEAD, graphite, or plumbago. Hence, a small stick of graphite for filling a pencil. 1840. 4. The metal as fashioned into a leaden coffin, a bullet, etc.; the leaden part of anything ME. 5. a. A large pot, cauldron, or kettle- (Orig., one made of lead.) Now only dial. OE. b. dial. A leaden milk-pan. 6. A sounding-lead 1440. 7. 1. a. The.strips of lead used to cover a root; often collect. for a lead flat, a lead roof, toccas. construed as sing. 1578. b. The lead frames of the panes in lattice or stained glass windows 1705. 8. Printing. A thin strip of type-metal, used in type-composition to separate lines 1808. 1. Phr. $To lie, be wrapt in 1.: to be buried in a 1. coffin. So tto lay,. lap in 1. 4. Heauen keepe L, out of nice Sinits. 6. Phr. To cast, heave the 1. To arm the 1.: to fill the hollow in it with tallow in order to discover the nature of the bottom by the substances adhering (Smyth). 7. a.. A Goodly Leads upon the Top, railed with Statua's interposed BACON. Comb:, : L?arming, the tallow used for arming a lead (see 6); -ash, -ashes litharge; ?bath, (a) the mass of melted I. in a lead-furnace ; (b) the molten 1, with which gold and silver ores are melted before cupellation ; 1. glance (= Du. laadglans), galena ; light, a window in which small panes are fixed in leaden tames, also attrib. ; .line, (a) a sounding,-lead or plumb-line ; (6) a line loaded with leaden weights, running along the bottom of a net; (c) a bluish grey line along the gums at their junction with the teeth, indicating lead-poisoning; -mill. (a) an establishment for producing milled or sheet I.; (6) a circular plateof I. used by the lapidary for . grinding or roughing; nail (mostly pl.), a nail used to fasten a sheet oft. on aroof; -ochre = MASStcor?; -paper, atest.paper treated with a preparation of l.; -pencil, a pencil of graphite, often enclosed in cedar or other wood; lant(U.S.), a shrub (A,norpha canescens) found in the west of the Mississippi valley, and believed to indicate the presence of 1. ore; -plaster = DIACHYLONI poisoning, poisoning by the introduction of 1. into the system; .spar = ANGLESITE or CsaussirE; -tree, (a) a W. Indian name for the tropical leguminous tree Leucana glauca ; (6) a crystalline deposit of metallic 1. or zinc that has been placed in it solution of acetate of I.; -vitriol= ANGLESITE; -water (=.Ger.blei. wasser), dilute solution of acetate of 1. ; -works plumbe`s' work and material; work in I., tap. glaciers work; sworks 41., an establishment for smelting lead-ore; -wort, a herbaceous plant of southern Europe (Plumbago europaae; also, any plant of the genus Plunrbago or the order Plaurbaginacex. Lead (lid), sb.2 ME. If. LEAD V.I] tr. The action of LEAD v.t ; leading -151o, b. Direction given by going in front; example esp. in phr. to follow the 1. of 1863. C. spec. in Hunting, etc., chiefly 'n hr- to iv 1 to go first in leaping a~I 4J' bi guiding indication 1851. 2. The front or lead- ing place; the place in front of (something). Also, the position or function of leading (e.g. The action or right of playing the first card in a round or trick ; also, the card so played, or proper to be played, or the suit to which it be- longs 1742. 5. Curling. The first player or the stone first played. 1685. 6. Mining. a. LODE 5. 1812. b. Gold-mining An alluvial deposit of gold along the bed of an ancient river 1855. 7. Theatr. 'I he principal part in a play; also, one who plays such a part 1874. 8. Friendly lead (see FRIENDLY a. 2). Also simply lead. 185x. 9. techn. a. Electricity. (a) The angle between the plane through the lines of contact. of the brushes or collectors of a dynamo or electric motor. with the commutator and the transverse plane bisecting the magnetic field. (6) A conductor conveying electricity from the source to the place where it is used. 1881. b. Engineering, etc. The distance to which ballast, coal, soil, etc. has to be conveyed (see LEAD V.' I b). .to its destination 1841. C. Horology. The action of a tooth, as a tooth of a wheel, in impelling another tooth or pallet 1880. d. Naut. The direction in' which running ropes lead fair, and come down to the deck (Smyth) s86o. e. Steam-engine. (See quots.) 1838- a. Phr. To take the (or a) 1. to occupy the front place, to assume the function o!` leader. Each of our porters took the 1. in turn TYNDALL. 4. Phr. To return one's partners 1.: to play from the same suit on getting the 1. 9. e. L. c/the crank, the setting of the crank of one engine a little in advance of the right angle to the other; name]yy at coon or xso? in place of ?. This assists in rendering the motion of the piston more uniform, by moderating its velocity at the end of the stroke. L. ofthe valve, the amount of opening which a valve has when the engine is on the centre 1887. Cwnb.: 1.-off, a commencement !? also that which leads o(f, the first of a series; -rents Coaching, the P: ins; -screw, 'the main screw of a lathe,. which gives the feed motion to the slide-rest' (Web. ster). Lead (lid), v.1 Pa. t. and pa. pple:'led. [Corn. Teut. wk. vb.: OE. lfdan : OTeut. laidjan, f. *laidB road, journey (see LOAD, LODE sbs.), related to OE. 116an to go, travel.] L To conduct. 1. trans. To cause to go along with oneself. fa. To bring or take (a person or animal) to a place. (Phrases like to 1. captive. are now understood in - sense 2.) -1704.` b.,To carry or convey, usu. In a cart, etc.. Now only n. dial.: To -cart (coal, corn, etc.). . To 1. in. (grain) : to house. OE. c. To bring forward, adduce (testimony) ; to bring (an action). Now only in Sc. Law. ME2. To conduct, guide, esp. by going on in. ad- vance OE. b. Of motives, circumstances. etc.: To guide, direct to a place ME. c. Of a clue, light, sound, etc.: To serve (a person)' as an indication of the way ; to mark the course for. Also absol. to 1. in (Naut.) : to mark the course.for entering port. 1697. d. absaal. 1580. a.. Phr.To 1..thee way: t(a) to (`t'dtcL; tfYe~dT1tT 8`EA'p~dM~fi' dtZ 30 Of a commander: To'march at the head of and direct the movement of. Also with on. OE. a. To conduct (a person) by holding the by argument, etc., to a conclusion; to induce to do something ME. 6. Of a way, road, etc.: To conduct (a person) to or into a place. Hence absol. or intr.: to have a specified direction. ME. b. intr. To form a channel into, a connecting link to (something) 1833. c. intr. To 1. to: to have as a result 1970. 7. To 1. (a person) a dance: transf. and fig., to put to the trouble of hurrying from place to place; hence, to compel to go through a course of irksome action. So to 1. (a person) a chase, a life. 1529. 8. it. To conduct (water, steam) through a channel or pipe ME. b. To guide the course or direction of (something flexible; e. g. a. rope, etc. over a pulley, through a hole. etc.) OE. C. Naut. intr. Of a rope : To admit of being led i86o. tg. To conduct (affairs) ; to manage, govern -1$79. 1. b. Faith, sir, ha's led the drumme before the English Tragedians SHAHS. C. No evidence has yet been led to show Sne W. HAMILTON. a. Therefore shall notMoses...his people into Canaan 1. Mtt.T. P.L. x1I, 309. b. Instinct early led him into the political arena 1892. c. L., Kindly Light, amid the encircling gloom, L. Thou me on J. H. NP.WMAN. d. Pray you 1. on Oih. t. i. 3t1. 3. The Prince.. led them on with great gallantry 1736. . 4. The captive soldier was led forth Goi.nsst. Phr. To F. a es (in hell): see APE s6. To 1. (a bride) to the altar, to church: to marry. b. The Moore..will as tenderly be lead by th' Nose As Asses are Oth.I. UP. 407. S. Tintoret. array I. you wrong if you don't understand him Ruseiu. 6. Broad steps 1. down into a garden x86x. c. Several seixuresof English cargoes led to reprisals on our part; reprisals led to a naval war M. PATTISON. 7. She had led him the life of a dog 1892. B. b. Ropes..led through blocks fixed to stakes xtgs. IL 'To carry on. t1. To engage or take part in, to perform (dances, songs), to utter sounds. Cf. L. ducere carmen, chores. -1493. 2. To, go through, pass (life, to portion of time.).. Cf. L. dueere vitam. Rarely, tTo sup- port life by (bread). OE. a. Do 1. your own life and let ours alone l BROWNING. IIL To precede, be foremost. (Cf. sense I. 2.) I. To have the first place in; lit. and ffieg. esp. in to 1. the dance, the van ME. b. absol. To go first. Also with of. 1998. 2. trans. To direct by one's example to set (a fashion); to take the directing or, principal part in (a pro- ceeding, performance, etc.); to be chief of (a party, a movement) ; to have the official initia- tive in the proceedings of (a deliberative body) 1642. ? 8. Of a barrister: trans. To act as leading counselin'(a cause); to act as leader to (another barrister).; to take precedence of. Also absol. or intr. 1806. 4. Card-playing. a. intr. To play the first card. Also with off. Said also of the card. Also-in indirect passive. b, trans. As first player, to play (a specified card) ; to play one of (a suit or a specified suit). Also with out. 1731. s. b. The Admiral's frigate led 19oo. a. To 1. an insurrection x8441,, the singing x859, the prayers x866, .o) Disraeled k1QY4IibPOPL r1+ 44f A# To 1. toi ort1.lup to: to playa card in order to bring out (cards held by another player). b. 11. a heart Sw: rr. Combs. (with advs.). Lead away. a. trans. To induce to follow unthinkingly. Chiefly in parr. b. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 These are some of the most commonly used nouns and verbs in our language. Each one must serve to convey a vast num- ber of different meanings ... CUT COME WELL DOG AID When talking about everyday words-the kind we use in talking to our family and friends, and to our business colleagues, it does not get us very far to ask of a word that has just been used- "What does it mean?" It may get us further along the way toward understanding each other, if we ask the speaker (or writer-they are the same thing using different wave-length systems)- "What do you mean?" Lebd (led) Eb, [OE. Lead : led [ d ~ ;t' sb 1 ?1Nmd~aa .o ',ie~a h fie n otr o g oP lt o; $q y ?co I orf,?li: T Fl e E p. "l4a e'arcc g18, ; ( ni{ .trot [1~ ~. "uy b W yr nrgJe y Lead (lid), v.1 Pa. t. and pa. pple. led. Lead (lid), sb.2 ME. [Com. Teut. wk. vb.-,OE. laedan ,-OTeut. [f. LEAD v.1] 'Iaidjan, f. 'laid6 'road, journey (see LOAD, LODE,sbs 1, related to OE. /i6an I. .To conduct 1 f ?To car r pn. ? l i t. To precede, c trans To 4t !To engage be foremost ause to no i ortakeportit (Cf sense;I - oneself;to To (dances, ~'thefirstplace a utferA.. in; . and Aa_ 'bring or'fake? tongs) , `anirr)al!'to a docerecarmen, the'dance, the olace.((Phrases _choros,-149&ritvan ME`+b tine are.,0ow+' through, ass t 71 with': understood in life p dr- off '"198 sense 2 )x tio~j of 9lme] 2 franc To 1704.rb ,To ACf ;,l ucero direct by one's (Unavoidable parenthesis: it is equally unproductive to ask an artist about his painting- "What does it mean?" It is sometimes useful to ask the artist "What do you mean?") 11 . The action of LEAD v.1; leading -1510. b. Direction given by going in front; exam- ple; esp. in phr. to follow the 1. of 1863. c. spec. in Hunting, etc., chiefly in phr. to give a I., i.e. to go first in leaping a fence, etc. 1859. d. A guiding indication 1851. 2. The front or leading place; the place in front of (something). Also, the position or function of leading (e.p a nartvl, le,'de- . Some day, we would like to correct what we feel is a misleading wLy that die tionaries describe common words. They list "mean- ings" in vertical columns, one below the other. We seem to give some value of "superiority" to that which is higher than some thing else. We have the` feeling that, the first definition is somehow "more right" than the second, third, etc. Let us instead consider a dic tionary that uses an illustrated "spectrum" to indicate areas in which a word has been used, how frequently, and over what period of time. No such dictionaries ap- parently exist today. But it may be useful when you look up common words in a dictionary-one that has many definitions-to think of them as being die,trib- uted across a spectrum. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Ago far, we have tAttlglz?rsfW?latr91/2 these give us quite a bit of trouble when we use them in trying to express what we have experienced. "llig" words seldom give us much trouble ... LOOK UP ? ICOSAHEDRON ? IN ANY DICTIONARY. IT WILL SAY "A GEOMETRIC FIGURE HAV- ING 20 SIDES." WE MAY HAVE TROUBLE SPELLING O ICOSAHEDRON ? WE MAY HAVE TROUBLE SAYING ICOSAHEDRON ? ? BUT WE HAVE NO TROUBLE UNDERSTANDING WHAT IS MEANT WHEN SOMEONE SAYS THAT SOMETHING IS AN ? ICOSAHEDRON, S HERE IS A BETTER WAY OFSAYING " Ito WHY'DESCRIBE SOMETHING^ WHEN :YOU' CAN SHOW IT? One reason for this is that "big" words frequently are ."labels." They "define themselves" and these ' definitions are nearly always "circular." Most of the technical terms used in the professions and sciences fall into this classification. "Little" words often tell us what is meant by them, from the context in which they are used- cIA~~~ayA4~~$~44~~oq~Q,~,7rr~~ ruefully at her ho( "I've got a run." We are pretty sure she does n 4 mean the same thing as V illie Mays lows when he says "I've got a run." The "medium" sized v ords give ? the mnrst trouble because: 1. We get little or no womfort frrrn looking them up in a dictionary. 2. We sometimes cannot figure of t what the speaker "means" just fron the cont:ex in which he has used the word Here are a few medium-sized wo?ds that we use nearly every day wh ?h~r can give us trouble ... I country patriotism society gov :rnment democracy republic people ex,:erience business management leadership of jectives politician natural virtue morals !cation communism delinquent criminal jnsanity etc, etc. etc. Here's just one example: you will run into dozens of others every day, if you listen or look for them. Yet people act: rally believe they communicate when they use words in this way: "Too much government is bad for business!" This statement may mean sometl ing to the person who used it-but it is utterly, without effective meaning to the person who hears or reads it. You can lose a friend, but clarify the statement, by asking a few questions: What government do you mean? Federal, state, county, city!- Executive; legislative or judicial? Elective 'or appointed? Where is the government you mein? In Nigeria? Athens? Outer Slobo :pia? What time are you talking about Today? Last month? In the Age of" Pericles? During the American Colonial Pc icd?' 22 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Your friend may thApp1*VF?i1a004101 (I t?ee l that) WoWt bu4 it he is s what she he means) a,ns) "The policies of the Committee of Ten (where?) in Carthage :,tihen?~ in the 4th Century, B.C. were deStrU(tiVe ("bad" in what way?) to the rowth of commercial competition. You may either agree or disagree with the ;oeaker-hut at least you now know what it is you agree or disagree with. Sometimes it is useful to know that.) At this point, you might be tempted to ask- "Why didn't you say so in the first place?" Don't do it! It's a sure-fire way to make people mad at you. But it may be a good question to ask yourself when people don't understand you. 29 : CIA P84i.1)1) & ,019th QQ0yOQ47i k irrofou is statements like -Then, fill in the what, the place and t're time. Sometimes you will di?(? )ver that y i don't. know the what, where , r when-i pact, y i haven't the slightest ide -i what yoi are talking about. This is always ; gather interesting discovery. Or, you go ahead and s.; y it, with i a.mes, places and dates. Perhaps nobody vill agr(,,, with you, but at least tl ey won't a;],-,.. "Why didn't you say so n the first plice?" You already have- Common words do not have meanings- Only people do. And sometimes they don't, either. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Lc i ` ,. u C~ I i;= Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 THE HUMAN TRANSACTION t is sometimes useful to think of human communications as "transactions."-& In the sense we mean here, a transaction involves the interaction of the observer and what he observes. This can take place between ourselves and the world-outside-our-skin. Or it can take place between two or more human beings. The transaction may be an exchange of specially-created visual events, such as written words, works of art, photographs, charts, equations etc., or in the form of spoken symbols, as in ordinary conversation, speeches, songs and music. Whatever the symbolization, the basic ingredients of the transactions are the same. These transactions may include: Something trarisforme Something create Something transmitted Light and sound waves transformed into electrical "codes" for transmission to the muscles Memory patterns stored in the nervous sys-: tem as electro-chemical codes Muscular movements .move objects so'that' they' emit or'reflect. ;unique patterns of visible light waves (or'create pulsed sound waves) which are received by others as events seen or heard. Approved or Release - - 500050047-2 Light waves, sound waves, tactile impres- sions, odors, tastes, etc. from outside-our- Approy ?, Teas Q04/O1/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 "Why didn't you say you had a full-grown, brown and white St. Bernard as a pet in the first place?" "Oh, what kind of a pet?" When we communicate with each other, it is useful to keep in mind that our common words may not evoke the same image in someone else's mind as they do in ours ... Knowing this, we can help improve our communications by being as specific as possible in the way we use words ... And, if you are on-the receiving end, it often helps to ask questions.. Assuming that "everyone knows what you are talking about"... and assuming you know what others are talking about without asking questions to make "sure"... ... are two common causes of communications failure.. Human communications frequently* seem to have a "loop" or "closed circuit" pattern. ',RECEIVER;' Many forms of eommunicatio, require a closed"loopaor,ircalt' ::~ RECEIVE RCE~VE R Nis MESSAGEE ESSAGE ',::= EN DER `Frequently, but not always. When you read a book, or other pattern of symbols, your response may be purely internal, and does not go back to the person who originated the message. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 In our daily lives, particularly in business, education and government operation,;, many interlocking communications loops may have to be used in order to perform even quite simple communications. In the oversimplified case diagrammed below, we see some of the interlocking loops that might be required in order to get a piece of advertising for a new product started, prepared, submitted, and returned for correction. Notice through how many minds the information has to pass-and remember, each mind will sei.e the problem somewhat differently and the message may change slightly as it passes along. RECEIVER DIVISION MANAGER response: don't.! like it -.,; k response "OK,, :(ll gets the agency to watk on'"lt:'~' RECEIVER message o sn`t~ke. essage' ~'ThtCIi n Woes Itke I . f. e;sage: ~' n.e i. ADVERTISING MANAGER SEN DER ti RECEIVER ADVERTISING AGENCY ACCOUNT REPRESENTATIVE SENDER N " "_11" RECEIVER AGENCY CREATIVE SUPERVISOR SENDER VINgwilown RECEIVER COPY WRITER/ ARTIST message Prepare , Ia is ing Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2' ^response It stawrk right awa. ' response We ire fined ,~t?,~ve to'n l; creel de rtmen ," rc, message:'.,,,',, Wants lug announce a new ;; product. sages fl r mete ~~ message a 1 roduct ~ l lerc s the d Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 hat we have tried to do so far is to discuss some of the things that go into the process by which we communicate with each other. We have talked about Input ("What's Gotten Into You?") and Processing ("What's Going On in There?") and Output ("Words, Words, Words") for all the world as if we were some kind of computer, or something. Well, that's sort of the way it is; something comes in, something is done to it, and something comes out. Now we want to talk about some of the everyday troubles we have when we try to understand each other. These troubles seem to be very much the same whether we are talking to members of our family, or at a luncheon of the Greater Metropolitan Daffodil Club, or making a presentation to a board meeting. Business communication differs from other communication only in the environment in which it takes place. We shouldn't have too many problems if we can keep a few things in mind: 1. At best, our perception and knowledge of what we can talk about is limited and fragmentary..-.- 2. Whenever we talk about something, we are talking about something that happened inside of us, not something outside of us ... 3. What happened inside of us did not necessarily happen the same way inside someone else ... 4. Common words do not in themselves have meanings; only the people who use them have meanings ... With these precepts firmly in mind, we can trudge forward with confidence toward the molehills of fact and the mountains of inference we make out of them; the "ism of is," and other strange places ... Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 ROO1500050047-2 To most of us, this is a pretty familiar scene. So familiar that we may tend to feel tha- we see mor+ in it than is there for us to see. Try answering the questions below and see how y~,u come )u Which ? of the following statements -are true, false, or cannot be answ, ered :at ail? The Jones family owns a TV set watches TV Johnny's father is a stockholder LIFE and ;FOR UNE =" TheJones family subscribes to TIME t$ 'Chere arse three people k the room , 6 Mrs Jones is knitting a" sweater Mrs. Jones. and, Johnny 11. _ They are watching`an evening television show (Answers on page 30) 28 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 ROO1500050047-2 Approved or a ease 2004101129 : - - 500050047-2 AS A MATTER OF FACT IF YOU PLAYED the little game on the opposite page, chances are that you felt that at least some of the statements about the people in the picture were "true." You may have considered them statements of "fact". And you may have been a little dis- concerted to discover that some of the things you called "facts" are what we are going to call "inferences." Let us see if we can explain what we mean by "fact" and "inference" be- cause everything we have said so far in this issue has been built around what we believe to be the difference between the two. (For instance, the first pages of the book tried to show that "facts" are something that happens inside of us and that everything we say about what happened inside of us is not the fact, but is only what we say about it. We can say that something looks like something, but we cannot say what it "is," apart from what it looks-sounds, tastes, feels, etc.-like.) So, we'll say that a "fact" (for you) is something you personally experi- enced. And we will go further and say that everything else is an "inference" (for you). These are harsh definitions. Indeed, if we had to live in a world where there were only "facts" (as we have defined them), it would be a pretty small world, limited to those few things we could personally touch, hear, taste, smell or see. We do not feel that we live in such a world and "common sense" tells us that there are many things we can ac- cept as "facts" that we have not person- ally experienced. And we are going to agree with you. We are simply going to say that as long as you are conscious of the difference between "facts" and "inferences" you are less likely to run into trouble when you talk and listen to other people than if you don't distinguish between them. We can further say that for most common, everyday living a "fact" is something that is socially agreed upon. It can be considered a "fact" if most people believe it to be "true" or if those people who are expert in it agree upon it. We can act as if such things were "facts," but we must remember that facts defined in this way are subject to change. (For example, for a good many cen- turies most people believed that the world was flat, even though no one actu- ally had reported having seen anyone "fall over the edge." And for several centuries top scientists believed that it was light, shining out of your eyes, that made the world visible. Mere consensus does not establish unchanging "facts" because the consensus is based upon what is said about an experience). We may also find it useful to keep in mind that "facts" change with time. Or rather, that facts relate to time. The statements that New York City has a GOOD GRIEFf population of 10,000; of 200,000 and of 7,891,000 people were statements of fact at the time somebody counted them. So, perhaps a good way to think about facts is that they are something that happened to you; that they are always historical whenever you try to talk about them. And a good way to think about "inferences" is that they indi- cate higher or lower probabilities. These probabilities may be high be- cause people have reported them widely and repeatedly. (That the sun will come up tomorrow morning enjoys a very high probability, because in the experi- ence we have had of it, it has always done so. But, as any commuter knows, the fact that the 8:15 train has been on time every morning for the past four days does not mean that it will for sure be on time tomorrow morning. Both statements are inferences ; but one has STOPlOOjameATMil Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 "J 1.11E other and we can aApp Ved;jQd's ReIe se 2004/0 it were a fact). We can-and do-disagree on "facts" to the extent that our individual experi- ence of the world is different. But what we mostly get into trouble about are disagreements about inferences, which we have stated as if they were facts. Suppose we both looked through a microscope. We might reasonably agree that that wiggly little thing down there is a wiggly little thing down there. But if I say that it is a paramecium, and if you say that it is an amoeba (both statements necessarily inferences) we may be in for an argument. And we can make this area of dis- agreement even wider-and thus our chances of understanding each other smaller-if we continue then to make new inferences concerning the first in- ference. We can, and sometimes do, build a whole crazy superstructure of inference built on inference-as in the chart on this page concerning the "man with a briefcase." You can build similar charts around almost any common event in your daily life and quickly discover why you some- times have difficulty understanding other people-and why, sometimes, they misunderstand you. WHAT HAPPENED' MRS"A" SAY$ i3 ..A.,.xi zt i,1 fer q e e Is k n THE EVENT,~~.?Ifsee;a r "Hemust be.a' e 3 l (n a `nee dedicated'man&to x take Work'home '1madn E>at ded sated i s pound to' e a success in life and an asset to our cam Ut 1/29 :CIA-RDP84-00780R001500050~47 2 - - NONE OF THE STATEMENTS ON PAGE 28 CAN BE SAID TO BE TRUE FROM WHAT YOU ACTUALLY SAW IN THE PICTURE 1. You do not know that the set is owned by them; it could be borrowed, or a demonstration set. 2. You do not know whether Johnny is doing homework or n.,t; all you can see is that he has a book in front of him. 3. You do not know that Johnny's father is a stockholder; you only know he is looking at the stock market report. Matter of fact, you don't know he is Johnny's father, either. Ile may be an uncle or friend just visiting in the house. 4. You do not know that it is a Western. It could be a commercie I or a foreig-n- made movie, or almost anything. 5. You do not know that it is Mrs. Jones, and you cannot tell what she is knitting. 6. You do not know that Mr. Jones (if, indeed, that is Mr. Jones) actually smokes cigars. You only can see that there is a cigar on the ashtray. Perhaps someone else left it there. 7. You do not know how many people might be in the room; you can only see that there are three people in the part of the room shown in the pic- ture. 8. You do not know what magazines they subscribe to. The ones on the table may have been purchased at a newsstand or loaned by a friend. 9. You do not know if this is the Jones family; nor can you tell if there are other members of the family who are not present. 10. Could be a neighbor's cat, making itself "at home." 11. You cannot tell if it is evening or not; only that the lights ar,~ on. Perhaps it is midday and the shades have been drawn. e ;. surprised ` f tha " ~Tht coq t is? 'Ilfes. da J , Ilt nless tithe ! som t ` e h ing abo we re pin'trou lei? Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 THE TROUBLE WITH IS, IS IS If we were to track down and corner in its lair what we believe to be one of the chief causes of problems in everyday communications, we would describe it as the misuse of the word... "It is good.." For one thing, what we consider "bad" are the many ways in which it can be misused in everyday speech : So what's so bad about "is"? BEHIND the unqualified use of the word "is" lurk a number of assump- tions, each of which can lead to trouble. (We use the word "unqualified" because there certainly appear to be places in our common speech where trying to avoid using the word "is" is-see?- not worth the effort it takes). "He is lazy..." "That is a rock all have one thing in common. The "is" implies that we are describing some- thing "out there" that has a certain quality-"goodness,' I "laziness," or "rocklike"-which exists independently of. our personal experience of it. And the next implication is that you must agree because "obviously" that is what it is. But what we really are describing is an `internal experience (see pages 3 through 12) which may have validity. ' only for us. ; One way out of this dilemma may be to say: "h"think it is good ... believe he is lazy It.looks tome like a rock. limited 'numbert;;of possibilities (an ex ~ 94, pert, may beTdefined as- a person "who knows how much he does not know'.'). Of these,pos'sil ilities, we have chosen one (or several) for a personal reason that may have validity only for us. how,? est_it can be,described _But,? In ,n: b~w? oroughly' and lave determined ~' the ndd the subject (whatever itnnay k her thing we imply whenxwe use ? rit" 4 `kr :. pe }ante inside ourselves .W P-MW desc l e s not"'"out there" but an ex' we can at least think this wayto'our selves as a .'reminderthat ,what, we In our everyday. speech, in memos Approved For Release 2004/01/29 CIA-RDP84-007808001500050047-2 31 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780RiD04M0M4frences and con- versation, we hear or read pronounce; ments like,"He is an organization man," or, "He is unimaginative," rendered with the air of finality that would be more proper coming from the Princeton Institute of Advanced Learning. If you take a look at the chart on this page which partially lists some of the things that might be said about an individual (just as many things could be said about the company you work for, or an organization you belong to, or your neighborhood) you will see that they represent a wide spectrum of different experiences that different people have had at different times with this one individual. To choose one and to speak of it as if it characterized the real, living person, is to imply "all." When you imply "all" you seem to have 32 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 closed the subject; yoA d'Uf R6fei 2>W4/(M29d td3A4MM**0760iR6 1 0 0U471Wn operations. It said, "Ile's , and that's all there is to it." Now, no one believes you should try to say all about a subject every time you say something about it; that would be nonsensical, even if it were possible. But you can describe your reaction to a person or situation in such a way as to make it clear that you are making an inference (see pages 28 through 30) based on your own limited personal experience. You can say, "I've only seen him a few times but he seemed to be a nice guy," (instead of, "He is a good guy,") or you could say, "I've seen him several times at club meetings and he strikes me as a loudmouth," (instead of, "He is a loudmouth.") When we make clear the limitations ing about that experience rather than the person, event or thing, we leave open the way to further discussion (rather than disagreement and argu- ment.) No one can seriously question that that was what you felt; but every- one can-and most people do-argue with the categorical statement, "That is There is still another way in which we sometimes use "is" that can lead to trouble. We may use it in the sense of "identities"-as in the phrase "2+2= 4." We substitute "is" for the "=" sign and say "2+2 is 4." They are not the same thing at all. "2+2 is 2+2." But it does not appear to be "4." "4" is some- thing else altogether. The arithmetical expression simply says that we can use the symbol "4" instead of "2+2" in cer- ,bua+'f~~ sa-cam l~ . (Zp~GL 4 fib CI - ALiti 44 7ry~Gl~~ a.GU O'~u . is permissive but it is not descriptive. When we use "is" as if it was an "_" sign in common speech, as in "truth is beauty" or "knowledge is power" we begin to wander rather far afield from the world we actually experience. This all may sound so obvious as to be almost childlike. Yet the "fact" re- mains that many of us, every day, use "is" as if it were some kind of a weapon. In doing so, we replace the richness and diversity of human experience with a dull and lifeless monochrome. We kill the animal and dry its skin and nail it to the temple wall, and in the end reduce the world we describe to a two-dimen- sional diagram-sans color, sans depth, sans motion-sans everything. "Approved For Rase 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 IT'S A MAD, MAD MAZE Just for fun, read the fol- lowing description, which is taken from the Encyclopaedia Britannica, and then see which of the illustrations it de- scribes. It is small, with a long nose, ears and tail, the latter being naked and prehensile. The op- posable first hind toe is claw- less and the tip is expanded into a flat pad. The other digits all bear claws. The best known species is about the size of a cat, gray in color, the fur being woolly. (We gave three artists the description above-nothing more-dnd this is what they drew.) THE DESCRIPTION at left is of an opossum. If you didn't guess that, we don't blame you, because first of all, none of the drawings looks very much like what we know to be an opossum, does it? And yet, you may notice that each drawing is still a legitimate inter- pretatign of the description. Who can deny that in each picture the tail is naked, the nose is long, etc.? And second, we didn't include any statements that would conform to what is probably your common conception of an opossum, such as "it hangs upside down when it sleeps and carries its young in a pouch." But perhaps the point to be made here is that this has been a demonstra- tion of something that occurs very fre- quently in our everyday dealing with others. We could call it "by-passing," for it happens when someone says (or writes) something, and someone else hears (or reads) something quite dif- ferent. It is the missing of meaning between persons. "Time flies." "You can't. They fly too fast." This, too, is a very simple and harm- less instance of by-passing. Why does it occur? Well, to begin with, a very cogent argument could be developed that there are more than 31/2 billion different lan- guages in the world. Each of us talks, listens, and thinks in his own special language (and sometimes he uses sev- eral) which contains slight variations of agreed-upon meanings that are uniquely individual, and which may change each second. Our personal country, province, section, neighbor- hood, profession, personality, attitudes and mood of the moment. And the chances that even a few of us will share all of these "ingredients" in the same way at the same time is pretty remote. Lawyers and dentists might disagree, for instance, over the word "closure," children could misinterpret the warn- ing of a parent about crossing the street as merely a distasteful restric- tion, the company vice-president might view a polite comment from the com- pany president as a "subtle suggestion," and so on. It all depends, we might say, on which window we're looking through. What sometimes renders the win- dows a bit opaque is the misleading emphasis that can be placed on the words. An assertive intonation or a subtle inflection may "change" the feel- ing of the words. The plain question, "What are you doing?" may become a sarcastic "What are you doing?" or a shocked "What are you doing?" or per- haps a belittling "What are youdoing?" depending upon the way it is spoken. Perhaps the little "conversation maze" on page 35 shows how easily our different "language worlds" can cause dead-ends of misunderstanding. In looking for a clue to solve this often troublesome problem, we might return to what we said on page 21 about listening to people, not just words, to find meaning. If, instead of asking, "What does that mean?" we ask, "What does he mean?" and if we are careful to ask this of ourselves before we talk, perhaps we can avoid some of the every- day missed meanings in our communi- language is shaped by our culture, cation with others. 34 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 .`,'George, have you seen.. the Simmons report? I can't find it "It: should be in the file, Ted. I used it a few days ago." "Are you sure you put it back? It isn't'thsre." 1;1 "Don't you think I put things back when I'm through with them?" "I'll look; just in case." Of course I do. Just thought it might be r buried underpsome papers." to the file. Mind telling 71. 77 "Are you going along? I thought you were tied: up on that other account Lope l ffmished that o thought we coy d o togetherr' "Good! Two of us will ; , make a better impression -,.'than just one 7 "I thought I was going alone on that one." "I fe It two of us'","' might.make more'of animpression "+ "Don't you think:I can hand Tipat's not he~reason,, George We want that account, don't wee" "Sure.`Maybe you-think you can get it better than I can." "I didn't say that," Why is Ted always picking on me? Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 THE BIG ROCK CANDY MOUNTAIN Now we enter a very difficult part of our exploration. Tread carefully here, and avoid the enticements along the way. There are three large traps that lie along the path, and they are easy to fall into. We call them the traps of non-differentiation, and we come upon them every day ... "See one and you've seen 'em all" HOW WOULD YOU DESCRIBE what you see in the picture at the left? Perhaps we can guess that your de- scription included some mention of the figures climbing that very precipitous mountain. You may have said some- thing like "Men climbing a mountain," or "Mountain climbers," or "Some people scaling a peak." Yet it is obvious from their dress that these "mountain climbers" also appear to be a construction worker, a cowboy, a policeman, etc. Each is different from the others in some way. This might lead us to an observation, for if you first noticed them as "men," or "mountain climbers" or "people" you were tying them together with words in very much the same way as they are tied together with ropes in the picture. You expressed what you first saw-a similarity among them. What this demonstrates, perhaps, is that in emphasizing the degree to which they are alike you've made no mention of the differences among them. In our everyday talking, listening, thinking, reading, etc., we tend to do very much the same sort of "tying to- gether" of things, people, attitudes, and the like. Perhaps you've noticed from time to time how very easy it is to "categorize" the elements of our world, while it often takes a conscious effort to recognize the differences, the nu- ances that make each thing unique in its own way. Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 The point to be mall pproAl rEepRef e?@04/Q462?idcGIA-RDP84-0078CROM45on'@!*04?lf2e other when de- is that to the extent we see only simil- to every question" fining something-largely because it s aritios and ignore differences, we limit our observation of the world about us. We raise a flag over our brigade and issue each member the same size cloth- ing. Now look at the illustration directly below. What do you see? Perhaps the first thing you notice is how different each view of the ball is from the others. Sometimes the difference between them is slight, but taken together the illus- tration shows quite a wide range of possible aspects of one single ball. We can see that in the world we experience about us there is some cor- respondence to that illustration. Some- times the differences between two things are slight-often they are signi- ficant. And surely in the whole range of things in a "category," there is bound to be an opportunity for great disparity. You might argue here that our ability to learn is largely dependent upon the extent to which we are able to see sim- ilarities-to associate and relate vari- ous facts, theories and subjects. We agree there wouldn't be much point in teaching a child the rather abstract concept of 1+1=2 if he couldn't use this knowledge to keep track of his marbles at recess and much later, to evaluate the financial solvency of his company ! And perhaps, then, we come to a con- clusion. We might say that it is often beneficial to discriminate among the things we experience, so that we notice the differences among things that are apparently similar, and the similarities among things that appear different. It would seem that this approach might be more rewarding as we deal with our world-it might be good to add, when next we are tempted to say, "See one and you've seen them all," the question, "or have you ?" WE PROBABLY CAN AGREE that in the world we experience every day there are very few events that we can classify as being either one thing or another- ours is not a black or white world, for the most part. We realize that people do not have to be either fat or thin, tall or short, rich or poor, etc. They may be a little of both. Yet we find it difficult to describe the multitude of "in-betweens," the not quite blacks and not quite whites, be- cause our language is structured in such a way that there is a sad shortage of "in-between" words. Try, for example, to fill in the blank spaces below with words that indicate the degrees of possibility between "either-or:" good evil pleasant unpleasant sweet sour Difficult, isn't it? In fact, it's nearly impossible without using some rather imprecise adjectives such as "very," "more," "fairly," etc. But this is all our language allows in many cases-there are few precise "medium ground" words. As a result, we tend to slip from one easier than searching for an interme- diate word. For example, if a man can't be described as honest, we are likely to call him dishonest. If he is not a suc- cess, he's a failure, etc. This isn't to say that there is no such thing as an "either-or" proposition. We can understand how at any given time a person has to be either dead or alive (he can't be both), standing or sitting, that the sun will rise at 6:05 tomorrow or it will not, and so on. Practically all things that can be accurately termed as "either-or," however, are happenings that must be related to a certain point in time. It is when we apply "either-or" alter- natives to events that have many de- grees of possibility, and when we treat them as "either-or" in our attitudes and daily conversation, that trouble may begin. The old expression, "There are two sides to every question," generally thought to demonstrate a broadminded view of things, is quite restricting when we stop to realize that there are SOONER OR LATER, CHARLIE BROWN, THERESONE THING YOU"RE GOING TO NAVE TO LEARN... AND NO LE55!'. 7IUD KIND OF LIKE TO 5EE A LITTLE MORE MAR61N FOR ERROR! 'OO REAP WHAT'OU 500! YOO 6ET CUT OF LIFE EXACTLY (GHAT YOU POT INTO IT! NO MORE Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 rea113 But N as we may I langu this 1 many sides toA rp sEol .Rele t2> .4/ %129s; JZ~"rDIRUtQQ78ARWJ?, iQQ QQ4;7r12iinde;? pres,ure. e are inhibited by our language, have seen, and since our attitudes e viewed as largely the product of tge, we can perhaps begin to solve roblem at the level of language. Ne It time we are tempted to imply "eith r-or" in describing something, then, aye might try instead to place the state; tent we want to make against a scale f "how much," "how important," etc. I; stead of saying "Johnny is a bad boy," for instance, we might substitute some hing like "Johnny deliberately broke two dishes today. He said he was sorry " Pei haps happiness little pore margin for is, after all, a error. ONE WAY we might look at our ex- perie ices of the day-to-day world arour, I us is as if they were a continu- ous n ovie film on which are captured the co istantly moving, changing events we ob ;erve. We can close our eyes while watching some event and the movie stops-our mind pictures the frozen image of the last " rame" we saw in much the same way a ; a football play or an actor's ex- pressi )n on television is "stopped" to dram: tize an event. CrtherTrain+>5g Scho1asticyHonor -GENERAL, INFORMATION:,- Why Do You desire To'Atfihate 6!.'i his Organi image is frozen in our mind's eye or on So you tell the man. -Sorry she was a the television screen we do not for an poor secretary." instant suppose that the action was never completed in reality. We know from experience that a football player doesn't "freeze" in mid-air and an actor's expression isn't immobilized on his face. We can vividly imagine the action continuing even if we don't see it. And yet sometimes, in our dealings with the people and things around us, we tend to close our eyes and "freeze" them as if they had not changed since we last experienced them. Attending a 20-year class reunion is a good example of this. How shocked we are to see how much others have changed because we remember them as we last saw them! When we pause to think about it, many of our attitudes and actions are influenced by the "frozen" images we have of people and things. Have you ever returned to that "quiet, scenic place" you had visited 10 years ago and been appalled to find a crowded resort with gaudy signs? Suppose someone calls to obtain a ref- erence for a secretary who used to work for you. You had to "let her go" because she didn't seem interested in her work Rrr-r %fA,~~ ~ ' ase s~,kd~.. q"11 Post c +, d ? r? '~ '~t x~j PesentlxzE!Op ?' re 41"T l ': ;` 11, L 1 11 11 1 ,t+r ' vim' , Have o er.Been;; cste , t et _ han Minor Traffic Violations? (If yes,explain TypingSpet!a' TERM. E s c.Q EN s tliot g this' man m comes oo ?'e }soriaY t ipre r ion, I~fee `1P`. '2"' d ,.~ ^f:w. ~ \.rAi"5M A+.n i i ..i WTC9t ~ .M 4F+i 44 M, rson~'ecord ndarreston`suspicion i~ a e trus or4thinesrssarounc expens ve tools j~ a ni an to is"tee ,. t m ght harm the co A ~, v xna i rith" hiss ja4 ;~Arecordon'ourpar11' ~v~t n 1965, But that was six .ears ago, when she was only 19. And you're still thinking of her as if she ha:sn't cha'tged in all that time! When we overlook the "when" in our statements, we are referring to a single frame in a film that continues to run and is always changing. But if we stop to think "when" we last experienced that person, that thing, that event., then perhaps we will qualify our statement with "I don't know, really-that was a long time ago." Perhaps we can see an example of a "frozen attitude" in the illustration be- low. The interviewer has judged the applicant as if he has not changed since a certain previous event in his life. Do you think the interviewer's judgment has very much to do with the applicant's present abilities? And how often do we find ourselves disagreeing violently with someone, largely because we are discussing some- thing we saw at one time and the other person saw at another time" Isn't it time you started asking your- self "when"? Don't you wish everybody did? 38 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 THIS is THE BECll\ t`JING - i\,' 1 t L. ei at the end of the road, seine reade ?s may feel that discussing elects o-magnetic waves, visible spent, a, electro-chemical coding, sync logy and loop circuitry has little, if anything, to do with the conve ?sation, dictation, giving or receiv ng orders, writing notes and letter.-, studying reports and parti- cipating in conferences that make up their working days. We''d go along with that. There's no nec d to understand the combus- tion engine in order to drive a car, provicing the car is working prop- erly. And that's the point. If human communications in everyday home and business?liferan perfectly. smoothly, there would be no need to understand the mechanism. But- most of us would agree-our daily communications frequently are`:; faulty. "~` When communications "do go awry, it is often useful to review the process to see what went wrong. And this we cannot do unless we know what we mean by process. Then, too, it may be that there are some who feel that "communica- tions" is something that technicians do (line the man who fixes the tele- phone) . Thus, they feel, communi- cation; is someone else's job not theirs. Not too long ago, a leading indus- trial firm studied the way a~ ~`typi caI" eyecutive spends his working hours. The resultsishowecl hat he spent ;. about' 90 per cent,of isydayr communicating In ari average )i(ur, s Fv 'c he spent butin~tes m 9111 inutesreadg, $ ut s talking and 21, rn1nutes$11Ste is to belhopedththe~arriaave spent the other six min utes thinking, com (itself aform of mutiicati.on);' but the study doesn't mention it. A goveriment.sponsored study, carried out bythe University of Chica,o `and ,theDepartment of Labor, shows that-as long ago as 1955- :t least 50 per cent of the cost of run sing the American economy was for coIi,rnunications. And a lending aerospace firm ro- cently "costed out" a major project and found that 40 per cent of its total cost was for communications. We cannot pretend, in this bri4 format, to have said very much about communications; but we have tried to say several things we feel may be important: (1) The ability to communicate is not something we are born with; we have to learn it-often the hard way. (2) Whenever we talk or write about anything, what we are talking or writing about is something that happened inside us-not outside us. (3) If we have difficulty under- standing-or being, understood-it is likely we have ignored some part of the communications process. It is up to us, individually, to find that part and correct it. This is not an easy thing to do. Meanwhile, there is a useful little catechism you can apply, every time you hear or read something. Its con- stant use can save"a lot of frustra- tion and ease a lot of tension. It goes like this : (1) WHO said so? (Don't accept "they" or "a company official" or "someone close to the (2) WHAT did he say? (What someone says he "thinks" someone else said is probably wrong; forget it.) (3) What did he MEA? (If you are talking to someone directly, ask= ing some questions may help ?If lie's not around, then possibly what,he meant cannot be established; burn asking the question , you at leasf1 make it clear to 'yourself;hathe `may. n of mean whatyoi think:.' e ". does.) (4) HOW does he know.? `(Is he an expert? Was he there? What are his sources of information?) For us, the intensive use of this little set of questions comes as clo;,e to a "magic formula" as our latter- day materialism allows. Perhaps it will work as well for you. I \t h : Ve put to tFer a bibIiog :;p! 1 of the sources Is c,, i ; pre?aring ILi- If you V ant to pursuo the s_zt;ect further, ti=rite to The Edito*', Kaiser A'I. rninl!m NEWS, r.dess giver: } = 1,~; .1 KAISER ALUMINUM NEWS !:DITOR, DON FABIJN F T`RIAL ASS; F-TA NT. -E - S SUNDEP-EVER ART OIRF. TOR; B(;13 CONOVER MOST O^ THE WORK MARJ1 WELHAM SECRLTARY P BLI HED BY THE PUBIC AFFAIRS DEPARTMENT. KA15ER. ALUMINUM & CHEMICAL CORPORATION. KAISER CENTER. OAKLAND., CAL.IFORNIA 94604 DESIGNED AND PRINTED BY K=15FR GRAPHIT ARTS GAU ST I "URG , I L I N A - TO Af,? hn' erR - I'IS I J x,.'i N S Its TH 2; 5 JL G S E L L e A F _ Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2 kAI5ER ALUM/NUM Approved For Release 2004/01/29 : CIA-RDP84-00780 R001500050047-2