ARTICLE EXTRACT
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
U
Document Page Count:
3
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
April 10, 2000
Sequence Number:
2
Case Number:
Content Type:
PAPER
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3.pdf | 172.12 KB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/11 : CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3
aesthetic criteria, subjects consistently selected certain
instances and avoided other ones; whereas one would expect
each Instance of a particular element to be chosen about an
equal number of times - specifically 106/16, or 6.625 times-
there were Instances which had been chosen just once or
twice, and others chosen 15 or even 20 times!.
By comparison to the conflicts induced by the scanning task,
the timing task was effortless and entertaining. Given that
all subjects went through both conditions, it is possible
that the significant timing-task result simply. reflects a
preferential effect: this condition may have fared better
because subjects perceived it as less frustrating. Alter-
natively, Insofar as subjects had little control over their
selections during the timing-task, and just had to press the
button when they felt the time was "right", they were less
prone to counter-productive psychological sets (e.g., trying
too hard) and more apt to adopt a passive, "goal-oriented"
approach. The study's results may be seen as analogous to
those of RNG-PK studies suggesting the superiority of goal-
oriented over process-oriented strategies (Morris, Nanko and
Phillips, 1979; Levi, 1979), or of hidden RNG-PK tasks over
explicit ones (Berger, 1988; PRL,1984; Varvoglis, 1989).
It should be noted, though, that the specific distribution
of scores in the timing task does not lend Itself to any
simple interpretations . It is not clear why hitting should
manifest at a level at which it is least rewarding, and at
which psI-Information Is least useful. Similarly, it is not
clear why the trend toward missing (most apparent in the
overall results) should manifest at the pair-level, at which
psi-information was quite precise, and a hit, presumably,
quite rewarding.
CONCLUSION
It Is obvious that this study's results, though significant,
are not 'particularly encouraging for psychic criminology
applications. The overall patterning of experimental scores
seems to have been due to a combination of hitting and
missing, with the most conspicuous hitting occurring at too
low an informational level to be especially useful.
On the other hand, It should be stressed that the task was
as removed from real psychic criminology as could be
involving repeated trials, "normal" subjects, and fictional
faces, rather than a single trial by a "psychic" attempting
to describe a dangerous criminal. Also, Insofar as the
procedures encouraged subjects to focus upon facial-features
rather than upon the face as a whole, our tasks may have
been simply too elementaristlc to fairly assess the utility
of faces as psi targets.
Be that as It may, we are looking to shift our approach with
Visages. Leaving aside psychic criminology, for now, we are
Preparing to recast the program as a PK-orie
U&?pr8v88rF'V1 Wel8YJre 2 d/?811 IAeRp
p 0 ~ 2-3
Approved For Release 2000/08/11 : CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3
wou r
block subjects" receptivity y Nextslt 1nfor than askang
pre
them effective PK n
and participants to fight
going toetel igthemdto go for ncIets, and
response biases, we're
(try to) have it their way... .
REFERENCES
Agor, W.H. (1984). intuitive iskilmsnaPren-t ice 1Hal gPrelss: NYft
and right brain management
M.A. (in press). Applications et apprent is,o g 3. en
,
paraps
arapychologie. Revue Francalse de Psychotronique,
Berger, R. (1988). Psi eff5cts1without real-time feedback.
Journal of Parapsychology,
Ellis, H. (1975). Recognizing faces. British Journal of
Psychology, 66, 409-426.
J. & Davies, G. (1975). An Investigation
Ellis, H., Sheperd,
of the use of the Ps?ologyte66n129e37 ~r recalling faces.
British Journal of Ych
K., Targ, R., & White, A. (1985). An applications
rrery
J rientation to psi research. 954~ ?W~ppe 1 and 9 1.20)1fvScarecrow
o
Research in Parapsychology
Press: Metuchen, NJ.
Hibbard, W.S. & Worring, R.W. (1982). Pray cric criminologi-
An operations manual for using psychics
gations. Springfield, Ill: Charles C. Thomas
Levi, A. (1979). The influences ooff imagery and8feedback on PK
effects. Journal of Parapsyc
and Phillips, B. (1979). Intentional
Morris B., Nanko, M.
observer influence on measurements emmage yfstrateglesUmIneWhGnRo11
system: a comparison of 1978 (pp.146-150). Scarecrow
(Ed) Research in Parapsychology
Press: Metuchen, NJ
psi. ve, J.
(1986). Psionics: the practical application of In p psi. In B.Shapin and Pa apsychologY Foundation: Yorksi
(Eds.) Current research (pp.130 136 ?
Csis, K. (1984). The application ESP and c criminal
investigations, locating missing persons
ngs24f 7a 4 Symposium on
Anomalous the Proceed(ing
airplane disasters.
Applications of APsychophysical Research Laboratories
Research Laboratories.
Report. Princeton, NJ: PsY P
Saxe, S. & Wagner, C. (1979). An
Reiser, M., Ludwig, L., t~ the Investigation of
Appro~r9gilEe -lea a 0YO 1f# G9AY~56-00792R000400180002-3
Approved For Release 2000/08/11 : CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3
maJor crimes. Journal of P0l Ice clence and AdmInisrr. tion,
7, 18-25.
Sergent, J. (1984). An Investigation into component and
configural processes underlying face perception. British
Journal of Psychology, 75, 221-242.
Varvoglis, M. (1989). A "psychic contest" using a computer-
RNG task In a non- laboratory sett In
R.Berger (Eds) Research in Parapsychology 1988 'Henkel and
Scarecrow Press: Metuchen, NJ. pp.10-15).
Approved For Release 2000/08/11 : CIA-RDP96-00792R000400180002-3