AN ASSESSMENT OF THE EVIDENCE FOR PSYCHIC FUNCTIONING
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00791R000200070001-9
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
K
Document Page Count:
32
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
May 22, 2000
Sequence Number:
1
Case Number:
Content Type:
RP
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00791R000200070001-9.pdf | 1.99 MB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
AN ASSESSMENT OF THE EVIDENCE FOR PSYCHIC FUNCTIONING
Professor Jessica Utts
Division of Statistics
University of California, Davis
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
TABLE OF CONTENTS
0.
Abstract .............................................. 1
1.
Introduction ............................................ 2
2.
Science Notes ...........................................
2
2.1
Definitions and Research Procedures ........................
2
2.2
Statistical Issues and Definitions ...........................
3
2.2.1 P-values and Comparison with Chance ....................
4
2.2.2 Replication and Effect Sizes ..........................
4
2.2.3 Randomness and Rank-Order Judging ....................
5
2.3
Methodological Issues .................................
6
2.4
Prima Facie Evidence ..................................
7
3.
The SRI Era
3.1
Early Operation Successes and Evaluation .....................
8
3.2
The Early Scientific Effort at SRI ..........................
9
3.3
An Overall Analysis of the SRI Experiments: 1973-1988 ...........
10
3.4
Consistency with other Laboratories in the Same Era .............
11
4. The SAIC Era ..........................................
12
4.1
An Overview ......................................
12
4.2
The Ten Experiments .................................
12
4.3
Assessing the Remote Viewing Experiments by Homogeneous Sets
of Sessions .......................................
13
4.4
Consistency and Replicability of the Remote Viewing Results ........
14
4.5
Methodological Issues in the Remote Viewing Experiments at SAIC ....
15
4.6
Was Anything Learned at SAIC? ..........................
17
4.6.1 Target Selection ................................
17
4.6.2 Remote Staring .................................
18
4.6.3 Enhanced Binary Computer Guessing ...................
18
5. External Validation: Replications of Other Experiments ................
19
5.1
Conceptual Similarity: Ganzfeld Experiments ..................
19
5.2
Ganzfeld Results from Four Laboratories .....................
19
5.3
Conclusions about External Replication ......................
20
6. Is Remote Viewing Useful? .................................. 20
7. Conclusions and Recommendations ............................. 21
8. References ............................................ 22
Appendix 1: Effect Size Measures Used with Rank Order Judging .......... 23
Appendix 2: A Brief Description of the SAIC Experiments .............. 24
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
ABSTRACT
Research on psychic functioning, conducted over a two decade period, is examined to
determine whether or not the phenomenon has been scientifically established. A secondary
question is whether or not it is useful for government purposes. The primary work examined
in this report was government sponsored research conducted at Stanford Research Institute,
later known as SRI International, and at Science Applications International Corporation,
known as SAIC.
Using the standards applied to any other area of science, it is concluded that psychic
functioning has been well established. The statistical results of the studies examined are far
beyond what is expected by chance. Arguments that these results could be due to
methodological flaws in the experiments are soundly refuted. Effects of similar magnitude to
those found in government-sponsored research at SRI and SAIC have been replicated at a
number of laboratories across the world. Such consistency cannot be readily expla.ned by
claims of flaws or fraud.
The magnitude of psychic functioning exhibited appears to be in the range between what
social scientists call a small and medium effect. That means that it is reliable enough to be
replicated in properly conducted experiments, with sufficient trials to achieve the long-run
statistical results needed for replicability.
A number of other patterns have been found, suggestive of how to conduct more productive
experiments and applied psychic functioning. For instance, it doesn't appear that a sender is
needed. Precognition, in which the answer is known to no one until a future time, appears
to work quite well. Recent experiments suggest that if there is a psychic sense then it works
much like our other five senses, by detecting change. Given that physicists are currently
grappling with an understanding of time, it may be that a psychic sense exists that scans the
future for major change, much as our eyes scan the environment for visual change or our
ears allow us to respond to sudden changes in sound.
It is recommended that future experiments focus on understanding how this phenomenon
works, and on how to make it as useful as possible. There is little benefit to continuing
experiments designed to offer proof, since there is little more to be offered to anyone who
does not accept the current collection of data.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
1. INTRODUCTION
The purpose of this report is to examine a body of evidence collected over the past few
decades in an attempt to determine whether or not psychic functioning is possible.
Secondary questions include whether or not such functioning can be used productively for
government purposes, and whether or not the research to date provides any explanation for
how it works.
There is no reason to treat this area differently from any other area of science that relies on
statistical methods. Any discussion based on belief should be limited to questions that are
not data-driven, such as whether or not there are any methodological problems that could
substantially alter the results. It is too often the case that people on both sides of the
question debate the existence of psychic functioning on the basis of their personal belief
systems rather than on an examination of the scientific data.
One objective of this report is to provide a brief overview of recent data i-.s well as the
scientific tools necessary for a careful reader to reach his or her own conclusions based on
that data. The tools consist of a rudimentary overview of how statistical evidence is typically
evaluated, and a listing of methodological concerns particular to experiments of this type.
Government-sponsored research in psychic functioning dates back to the early 1970s when a
program was initiated at what was then the Stanford Research Institute, now called SRI
International. That program was in existence until 1989. The following year, government
sponsorship moved to a program at Science Applications International Corporation (SAIC)
under the direction of Dr. Edwin May, who had been employed in the SRI program since the
mid 1970s and had been Project Director from 1986 until the close of the program.
This report will focus most closely on the most recent work, done by SAIC. Section 2
describes the basic statistical and methodological issues required to understand this work;
Section 3 discusses the program at SRI; Section 4 covers the SAIC work (with some of the
details in an Appendix); Section 5 is concerned with external validation by exploring related
results from other laboratories; Section 6 includes a discussion of the usefulness of this
capability for government purposes and Section 7 provides conclusions and
recommendations.
2. SCIENCE NOTES
2.1 Definitions and Research Procedures
There are two basic types of functioning that are generally considered under the broad
heading of psychic or paranormal abilities. These are classically known as extrasensory
perception (ESP), in which one acquires information through unexplainable means and
psychokinesis, in which one physically manipulates the environment through unknown
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
means. The SAIC laboratory uses more neutral terminology for these abilities; they refer to
ESP as anomalous cognition (AC) and to psychokinesis as anomalous perturbation (AP).
The vast majority of work at both SRI and SAIC investigated anomalous cognition rather
than anomalous perturbation, although there was some work done on the latter.
Anomalous cognition is further divided into categories based on the apparent source of the
information. If it appears to come from another person, the ability is called telepathy, if it
appears to come in real time but not from another person it is called clairvoyance and if the
information could have only been obtained by knowledge of the future, it is called
precognition.
It is possible to identify apparent precognition by asking someone to describe something for
which the correct answer isn't known until later in time. It is more difficult to rule out
precognition in experiments attempting to test telepathy or clairvoyance, since it is almost
impossible to be sure that subjects in such experiments never see the correct answer at some
point in the future. These distinctions are important in the quest to identify an explanation
for anomalous cognition, but do not bear on the existence issue.
The vast majority of anomalous cognition experiments at both SRI and SAIC used a
technique known as remote viewing. In these experiments, a viewer attempts to draw or
describe (or both) a target location, photograph, object or short video segment. All known
channels for receiving the information are blocked. Sometimes the viewer is assisted by a
monitor who asks the viewer questions; of course in such cases the monitor is blind to the
answer as well. Sometimes a sender is looking at the target during the session, but
sometimes there is no sender. In most cases the viewer eventually receives feedback in
which he or she learns the correct answer, thus making it difficult to ru!?: out precognition as
the explanation for positive results, whether or not there was a sender.
Most anomalous cognition experiments at SRI and SAIC were of the free-response type, in
which viewers were simply asked to describe the target. In contrast, a forced-choice
experiment is one in which there are a small number of known choices from which the
viewer must choose. The latter may be easier to evaluate statistically but they have been
traditionally less successful than free-response experiments. Some of the work done at SAIC
addresses potential explanations for why that might be the case.
2.2 Statistical Issues and Definitions
Few human capabilities are perfectly replicable on demand. For example, even the best
hitters in the major baseball leagues cannot hit on demand. Nor can we predict when
someone will hit or when they will score a home run. In fact, we cannot even predict
whether or not a home run will occur in a particular game. That does not mean that home
runs don't exist.
Scientific evidence in the statistical realm is based on replication of the same average
performance or relationship over the long run. We would not expect a fair coin to result in
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
five heads and five tails over each set of ten tosses, but we can expect the proportion of
heads and tails to settle down to about one half over a very long series of tosses. Similarly,
a good baseball hitter will not hit the ball exactly the same proportion of times in each game
but should be relatively consistent over the long run.
The same should be true of psychic functioning. Even if there truly is an effect, it may
never be replicable on demand in the short run even if we understand how it works.
However, over the long run in well-controlled laboratory experiments we should see a
consistent level of functioning, above that expected by chance. The anticipated level of
functioning may vary based on the individual players and the conditions, just as it does in
baseball, but given players of similar ability tested under similar conditions the results should
be replicable over the long run. In this report we will show that replicability in that sense
has been achieved.
2.2.1 P-values and Comparison with Chance. In any area of science, evidence based on
statistics comes from comparing what actually happened to what should have happened by
chance. For instance, without any special interventions about 51 percent of births in the
United States result in boys. Suppose someone claimed to have a method that enabled one to
increase the chances of having a baby of the desired sex. We could study their method by
comparing how often births resulted in a boy when that was the intended outcome. If that
percentage was higher than the chance percentage of 51 percent over the long run, then the
claim would have been supported by statistical evidence.
Statisticians have developed numerical methods for comparing results to what is expected by
chance. Upon observing the results of an experiment, the p-value is the answer to the
following question: If chance alone is responsible for the results, how likely would we be to
observe results this strong or stronger? If the answer to that question, i.e. the p-value is
very small, then most researchers are willing to rule out chance as an explanation. In fact it
is commonly accepted practice to say that if the p-value is 5 percent (0.05)' or less, then we
can rule out chance as an explanation. In such cases, the results are said to be statistically
significant. Obviously the smaller the p-value, the more convincingly chance can be ruled
out.
Notice that when chance alone is at work, we erroneously find a statistically significant result
about 5 percent of the time. For this reason and others, most reasonable scientists require
replication of non-chance results before they are convinced that chance can be ruled out.
2.2.2 Replication and Effect Sizes. In the past few decades scientists have realized that
true replication of experimental results should focus on the magnitude of the effect, or the
effect size rather than on replication of the p-value. This is because the latter is heavily
dependent on the size of the study. In a very large study, it will take only a small magnitude
effect to convincingly rule out chance. In a very small study, it would take a huge effect to
convincingly rule out chance.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
In our hypothetical sex-determination experiment, suppose 70 out of 100 births designed to
be boys actually resulted in boys, for a rate of 70 percent instead of the 51 percent expected
by chance. The experiment would have a p-value of 0.0001, quite convincingly ruling out
chance. Now suppose someone attempted to replicate the experiment with only ten births
and found 7 boys, i.e also 70 percent. The smaller experiment would have a p-value of
0.19, and would not be statistically significant. If we were simply to focus on that issue, the
result would appear to be a failure to replicate the original result, even though it achieved
exactly the same 70 percent boys! In only ten births it would require 90 percent of them to
be boys before chance could be ruled out. Yet the 70 percent rate is a more exact replication
of the result than the 90 percent.
Therefore, while p-values should be used to assess the overall evidence for a phenomenon,
they should not be used to define whether or not a replication of an experimental result was
"successful." Instead, a successful replication should be one that achieves an effect that is
within expected statistical variability of the original result, or that achieves an even stronger
effect for explainable reasons.
A number of different effect size measures are in use in the social sciences, but in this report
we will focus on the one used most often in remote viewing at SRI and SAIC. Because the
definition is somewhat technical it is given in Appendix 1. An intuitive explanation will be
given in the next subsection. Here, we note that an effect size of 0 is consistent with chance,
and social scientists have, by convention, declared an effect size of 0.2 as small, 0.5 as
medium and 0.8 as large. A medium effect size is supposed to be visible to the naked eye of
a careful observer, while a large effect size is supposed to be evident to any observer.
2.2.3 Randomness and Rank-Order Judging. At the heart of any statistical method is a
definition of what should happen "randomly" or "by chance." Without a random
mechanism, there can be no statistical evaluation.
There is nothing random about the responses generated in anomalous cognition experiments;
in other words, there is no way to define what they would look like "by chance." Therefore,
the random mechanism in these experiments must be in the choice of the target. In that way,
we can compare the response to the target and answer the question: "If chance alone is at
work, what is the probability that a target would be chosen that matches this response as well
as or better than does the actual target?"
In order to accomplish this purpose, a properly conducted experiment uses a set of targets
defined in advance. The target for each remote viewing is then selected randomly, in such a
way that the probability of getting each possible target is known.
The SAIC remote viewing experiments and all but the early ones at SRI used a statistical
evaluation method known as rank-order judging. After the completion of a remote
viewing, a judge who is blind to the true target (called a blind judge) is shown the response
and five potential targets, one of which is the correct answer and the other four of which are
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
"decoys." Before the experiment is conducted each of those five choices must have had an
equal chance of being selected as the actual target. The judge is asked to assign a rank to
each of the possible targets, where a rank of one means it matches the response most closely,
and a rank of five means it matches the least.
The rank of the correct target is the numerical score for that remote viewing. By chance
alone the actual target would receive each of the five ranks with equal likelihood, since
despite what the response said the target matching it best would have the same chance of
selection as the one matching it second best and so on. The average rank by chance would
be three. Evidence for anomalous cognition occurs when the average rank over a series of
trials is significantly lower than three. (Notice that a rank of one is the best possible score
for each viewing.)
This scoring method is conservative in the sense that it gives no extra credit for an excellent
match. A response that describes the target almost perfectly will achieve the same rank of
one as a response that contains only enough information to pick the target as the best choice
out of the five possible choices. One advantage of this method is that it is still valid even if
the viewer knows the set of possible targets. The probability of a first place match by
chance would still be only one in five. This is important because the later SRI and many of
the SAIC experiments used the same large set of National Geographic photographs as
targets. Therefore, the experienced viewers would eventually become familiar with the range
of possibilities since they were usually shown the answer at the end of each remote viewing
session.
For technical reasons explained in Appendix 1, the effect size for a series of remote viewings
using rank-order judging with five choices is (3.0 - average rank)//. Therefore, small,
medium and large effect sizes (0.1, 0.5 and 0.8) correspond to average ranks of 2.86, 2.29
and 1.87, respectively. Notice that the largest effect size possible using this method is 1.4,
which would result if every remote viewing achieved a first place ranking.
2.3 Methodological Issues
One of the challenges in designing a good experiment in any area of science is to close the
loopholes that would allow explanations other than the intended one to account for the
results.
There are a number cf places in remote viewing experiment where infoi.nation could be
conveyed by normal means if proper precautions are not taken. The early SRI experiments
suffered from some of those problems, but the later SRI experiments and the SAIC work
were done with reasonable methodological rigor, with some exceptions noted in the detailed.
descriptions of the SAIC experiments in Appendix 2.
The following list of methodological issues shows the variety of concerns that must be
addressed. It should be obvious that a well-designed experiment requires careful thought and
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
planning:
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target should have any contact with the
viewer until after the response has been safely secured.
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target or even of whether or not the
session was successful should have any contact with the judge until after that task has
been completed.
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target should have access to the r. -sponse
until after the judging has been completed.
? Targets and decoys used in judging should be selected using a well-tested
randomization device.
? Duplicate sets of targets photographs should be used, one during the experiment and
one during the judging, so that no cues (like fingerprints) can be inserted onto the
target that would help the judge recognize it.
? The criterion for stopping an experiment should be defined in advance so that it is not
called to a halt when the results just happen to be favorable. Generally, that means
specifying the number of trials in advance, but some statistical procedures require or
allow other stopping rules. The important point is that the rule be defined in advance
in such a way that there is no ambiguity about when to stop.
? Reasons, if any, for excluding data must be defined in advance and followed
consistently, and should not be dependent on the data. For example, a rule specifying
that a trial could be aborted if the viewer felt ill would be legitimate, but only if the
trial was aborted before anyone involved in that decision knew the correct target.
? Statistical analyses to be used must be planned in advance of collecting the data so
that a method most favorable to the data isn't selected post hoc. If multiple methods
of analysis are used the corresponding conclusions must recognize that fact.
2.4 Prima Facie Evidence
According to Webster's Dictionary, in law prima facie evidence is "evidence having such a
degree of probability that it must prevail unless the contrary be proved." There are a few
examples of applied, non-laboratory remote viewings provided to the review team that would
seem to meet that criterion for evidence. These are examples in which the sponsor or
another government client asked for a single remote viewing of a site, known to the
requestor in real time or in the future, and the viewer provided details far beyond what could
be taken as a reasonable guess. Two such examples are given by May (1995) in which it
appears that the results were so striking that they far exceed the phenomenon as observed in
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
the laboratory. Using a post hoc analysis, Dr. May concluded that in one of the cases the
remote viewer was able to describe a microwave generator with 80 percent accuracy, and
that of what he said almost 70 percent of it was reliable. Laboratory remote viewings rarely
show that level of correspondence.
Notice that standard statistical methods cannot be used in these cases because there is no
standard for probabilistic comparison. But evidence gained from applied remote viewing
cannot be dismissed as inconsequential just because we cannot assign specific probabilities to
the results. It is most important to ascertain whether or not the information was achievable
in other standard ways. In Section 3 an example is given in which a remote viewer allegedly
gave codewords from a secret facility that he should not have even known existed. Suppose
the sponsors could be absolutely certain that the viewer could not have known about those
codewords through normal means. Then even if we can't assign an exact probability to the
fact that he guessed them correctly, we can agree that it would be very small. That would
seem to constitute prima facie evidence unless an alternative explanation could be found.
Similarly, the viewer who described the microwave generator allegedly knew only that the
target was a technical site in the United States. Yet, he drew and described the microwave
generator, including its function, its approximate size, how it was housed and that it had "a
beam divergence angle of 30 degrees" (May, 1995, p. 15).
Anecdotal reports of psychic functioning suffer from a similar problem in terms of their
usefulness as proof. They have the additional difficulty that the "response" isn't even well-
defined in advance, unlike in applied remote viewing where the viewer provides a fixed set
of information on request. For instance, if a few people each night happen to dream of plane
crashes, then some will obviously do so on the night before a major plane crash. Those
individuals may interpret the coincidental timing as meaningful. This is undoubtedly the
reason many people think the reality of psychic functioning is a matter of belief rather than
science, since they are more familiar with the provocative anecdotes than with the laboratory
evidence.
3. THE SRI ERA
3.1 Early Operational Successes and Evaluation
According to Puthoff and Targ (1975) the scientific research endeavor at SRI may never have
been supported had it not been for three apparent operational successes in the early days of
the program. These are detailed by Puthoff and Targ (1975), although the level of the
matches is not clearly delineated.
One of the apparent successes concerned the "West Virginia Site" in which two remote
viewers purportedly identified an underground secret facility. One of them apparently named
codewords and personnel in this facility accurately enough that it set off a security
investigation to determine how that information could have been leaked. Based only on the
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
coordinates of the site, the viewer first described the above ground terrain, then proceeded to
describe details of the hidden underground site.
The same viewer then claimed that he could describe a similar Communist Bloc site and
proceeded to do so for a site in the Urals. According to Puthoff and Targ "the two reports
for the West Virginia Site, and the report for the Urals Site were verified by personnel in the
sponsor organization as being substantially correct (p. 8)."
The third reported operational success concerned an accurate description of a large crane and
other information at a site in "emipalatinsk, USSR. Again the viewer was provided with
only the geographic coordinates of the site and was asked to describe what was there.
Although some of the information in these examples was verified to be highly accurate, the
evaluation of operational work remains difficult, in part because there is no chance baseline
for comparison (as there is in controlled experiments) and in part because of differing
expectations of different evaluators. For example, a government official who reviewed the
Semipalatinsk work concluded that there was no way the remote viewer could have drawn the
large gantry crane unless "he actually saw it through remote viewing, or he was informed of
what to draw by someone knowledgeable of [the site]." Yet that same analyst concluded that
"the remote viewing of [the site] by subject S1 proved to be unsuccessful" because "the only
positive evidence of the rail-mounted gantry crane was far outweighed by the large amount of
negative evidence noted in the body of this analysis." In other words, the analyst had the
expectation that in order to be "successful" a remote viewing should contain accurate
information only.
Another problem with evaluating this operational work is that there is no way to know with
certainty that the subject did not speak with someone who had knowledge of the site,
however unlikely that possibility may appear. Finally, we do not know to what degree the
results in the reports were selectively chosen because they were correct. These problems can
all be avoided with well designed controlled experiments.
3.2 The Early Scientific Effort at SRI
During 1974 and early 1975 a number of controlled experiments were conducted to see if
various types of target material could be successfully described with remote viewing. The
results reported by Puthoff and Targ (1975) indicated success with a wide range of material,
from "technical" targets like a xerox machine to natural settings, like a swimming pool. But
these and some of the subsequent experiments were criticized on statistical and
methodological grounds; we briefly describe one of the experiments and criticisms of it to
show the kinds of problems that existed in the early scientific effort.
The largest series during in the 1973 to 1975 time period involved remote viewing of natural
sites. Sites were randomly selected for each trial from a set of 100 possibilities. They were
selected "without replacement," meaning that sites were not reused once they had been
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
selected. The series included eight viewers, including two supplied by the sponsor. Many of
the descriptions showed a high degree of subjective correspondence, and the overall statistical
results were quite striking for most of the viewers.
Critics attacked these experiments on a number of issues, including the selection of sites
without replacement and the statistical scoring method used. The results were scored by
having a blind judge attempt to match the target material with the transcripts of the
responses. A large fraction of the matches were successful. But critics noted that some
successful matching could be attained just from cues contained in the transcripts of the
material, like when a subject mentioned in one session what the target had been in the
previous session. Because sites were selected without replacement, knowing what the answer
was on one day would exclude that target site from being the answer on any other day.
There was no way to determine the extent to which these problems influence the results.
The criticisms of these and subsequent experiments, while perhaps unwelcome at the time,
have resulted in substantially improved methodology in these experiments.
3.3 An Overall Analysis of the SRI Experiments: 1973-1988
In 1988 an analysis was made of all of the experiments conducted at SRI from 1973 until that
time (May et al, 1988). The analysis was based on all 154 experiments conducted during
that era, consisting of over 26,000 individual trials. Of those, almost 20,000 were of the
forced choice type and just over a thousand were laboratory remote viewings. There were a
total of 227 subjects in all experiments.
The statistical results were so overwhelming that results that extreme or more so would occur
only about once in every 1020 such instances if chance alone is the explanation (i.e., the p-
value was less than 10'2). Obviously some explanation other than chance must be found.
Psychic functioning may not be the only possibility, especially since some of the earlier work
contained methodological problems. However, the fact that the same level of functioning
continued to hold in the later experiments, which did not contain those flaws, lends support
to the idea that the methodological problems cannot account for the results. In fact, there
was a talented group of subjects (labeled G1 in that report) for whom the effects were
stronger than for the group at large. According to Dr. May, the majority of experiments
with that group were conducted later in the program, when the methodology had been
substantially improved.
In addition to the statistical results, a number of other questions and patterns were examined.
A summary of the results revealed the following:
1. "Free response" remote viewing, in which subjects describe a target, was much more
successful than "forced choice" experiments, in which subjects were asked to choose from a
small set of possibilities.
2. There was a group of six selected individuals whose performance far exceeded that of
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
unselected subjects. The fact that these same selected individuals consistently performed
better than others under a variety of protocols provides a type of replicability that helps
substantiate the validity of the results. If methodological problems were responsible for the
results, they should not have affected this group differently from others.
3. Mass-screening efforts found that about one percent of those who volunteered to be
tested were consistently successful at remote viewing. This indicates that remote viewing is
an ability that differs across individuals, much like athletic ability or musical talent. (Results
of mass screenings were not included in the formal analysis because the conditions were not
well-controlled, but the subsequent data from subjects found during mass-screening were
included.)
4. Neither practice nor a variety of training techniques consistently worked to improve
remote viewing ability. It appears that it is easier to find than to train good remote viewers.
5. It is not clear whether or not feedback (showing the subject the right answer) is
necessary, but it does appear to provide a psychological boost that may increase
performance.
6. Distance between the target and the subject does not seem to impact the quality of the
remote viewing.
7. Electromagnetic shielding does not appear to inhibit performance.
8. There is compelling evidence that precognition, in which the target is selected after
the subject has given the description, is also successful.
9. There is no evidence to support anomalous perturbation (psychokinesis), i.e. physical
interaction with the environment by psychic means.
3.4 Consistency with Other Laboratories in the Same Era
One of the hallmarks of a real phenomenon is that its magnitude is replicable by various
researchers working under similar conditions. The results of the overall SRI analysis are
consistent with results of similar experiments in other laboratories. For instance, an
overview of forced choice precognition experiments (Honorton and Ferrari, 1989) found an
average "effect size" per experimenter of 0.033, whereas all forced choice experiments at
SRI resulted in a similar effect size of .052. The comparison is not ideal since the SRI
forced choice experiments were not necessarily precognitive and they used different types of
target material than the standard card-guessing experiments.
Methodologically sound remote viewing has not been undertaken at other laboratories, but a
similar regime called the ganzfeld (described in more detail in Section 5) has shown to be
similarly successful. The largest collection of ganzfeld experiments was conducted from
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
1983 to 1989 at the Psychophysical Research Laboratories in Princeton, NJ. Those
experiments were also reported by separating novices from experienced subjects. The overall
effect size for novice remote viewing at SRI was 0.164, while the effect size for novices in
the ganzfeld at PRL was a very similar 0.17. For experienced remote viewers at SRI the
overall effect size was 0.385; for experienced viewers in the ganzfeld experiments it was
0.35. These consistent results across laboratories help refute the idea that the successful
experiments at any one lab are the result of fraud, sloppy protocols or some methodological
problem and also provide an indication of what can be expected in future experiments.
4. THE SAIC ERA
4.1 An Overview
The review team decided to focus more intensively on the experiments conducted at Science
Applications International Corporation (SAIC), because they provide a manageable yet varied
set to examine in detail. They were guided by a Scientific Oversight Committee consisting
of experts in a variety of disciplines, including a winner of the Nobel Prize in Physics,
internationally known professors of statistics, psychology, neuroscience and astronomy and a
medical doctor who is a retired U.S. Army Major General. Further, we have access to the
details for the full set of SAIC experiments, unlike for the set conducted at SRI. Whatever
details may be missing'from the written reports are obtainable from the principal
investigator, Dr. Edwin May, to whom we have been given unlimited access.
In a memorandum dated July 25, 1995, Dr. Edwin May listed the set of experiments
conducted by SAIC. There were ten experiments, all designed to answer questions about
psychic functioning, raised by the work at SRI and other laboratories, rather than just to
provide additional proof of its existence. Some of the experiments were of a similar format
to the remote viewing experiments conducted at SRI and we can examine those to see
whether or not they replicated the SRI results. We will also examine what new knowledge
can be gained from the results of the SAIC work.
4.2 The Ten Experiments
Of the ten experiments done at SAIC, six of them involved remote viewing and four did not.
Rather than list the details in the body of this report, Appendix 2 gives a brief description of
the experiments. What follows is a discussion of the methodology and results for the
experiments as a whole. Because of the fundamental differences between remote viewing
and the other types of experiments, we discuss them separately.
In the memorandum of 25 July 1995, Dr. May provided the review team with details of the
ten experiments, including a short title, number of trials, effect size and overall p-value for
each one. His list was in time sequence. It is reproduced in Table 1, using his numbering
system, with the experiments categorized by type, then sequentially within type. The effect
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
size estimates are based on a limited number of trials, so they are augmented with an interval
to show the probable range of the true effect (e.g..124?.071 indicates a range from .053 to
.195). Remember that an effect size of 0 represents chance, while a positive effect size
indicates positive results.
TABLE 1: SAIC EXPERIMENTS LISTED BY DR. EDWIN MAY
Expr
Title
Trials
Effect Size
p-value
Remote Viewing Experiments
1
Target dependencies
200
.124?.071
0.040
4
AC with binary coding
40
-.067?.158
0.664
5
AC lucid dreams, base
24
.088?.204
0.333
6
AC lucid dreams, pilot
21
.368?.218
0.046
9
ERD AC Behavior
70
.303?.120
0.006
10
Entropy II
90
.550?.105
9.1 x 10-$
Other Experiments
2
AC of binary targets
300
.123?.058
0.017
3
MEG Replication
12,000s
MCE
MCE
7
Remote observation
48
.361+.144
0.006
8
ERD EEG investigation
7,000s
MCE
MCE
4.3 Assessing the Remote Viewing Experiments by Homogeneous Sets of Sessions
While Table 1 provides an overall assessment of the results of each experiment, it does so at
the expense of information about variability among viewers and types of targets. In terms of
understanding the phenomenon, it is important to break the results down into units that are as
homogeneous as possible in terms of procedure, individual viewer and type of target. This is
also important in order to assess the impact of any potential methodological problems. For
example, in one pilot experiment (E6, AC in Lucid Dreams) viewers were permitted to take
the targets home with them in sealed envelopes. Table 2 presents the effect size results at
the most homogeneous level possible based on the information provided. For descriptions of
the experiments, refer to Appendix 2. Overall effect sizes for each viewer and total effect
sizes for each experiment are weighted according to the number of trials, so each trial
receives equal weight.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
TABLE 2: INDIVIDUAL EFFECT SIZES
Experiment
Expert Remote Viewers
Viewer
009
131
372
389
518
Unknown/
Other
Total
Static Targets (National Geographics)
El: Static
.424
-.071
.424
.177
.283
n.a.
.247
E9
.432
n.a.
.354
.177
n.a.
n.a.
.303
E10: Static
.566
n.a.
.801
-.071
.778
n.a.
.550
E5 (Note 1)
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
.088
.088
E6 (Note 2)
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
n.a.
.370
.370
E4 (Note 3)
-.112
n.a.
0
.112
n.a.
-.559
-.067
Dynamic Targets (Video Film Clips)
El: Dynamic
0
.354
-.283
0
-.071
n.a.
.000
E10: Dynamic
.919
n.a.
.754
0
.424
n.a.
.550
Overall
.352
.141
.340
.090
.271
n.a.
1. Experiment 5 did not include any expert viewers.
2. Experiment 6 included 4 expert viewers but separate results were not provided.
3. Experiment 4 used a specially designed target set and only 4 choices in judging.
4.4 Consistency and Replicability of the Remote Viewing Results
One of the most important hallmarks of science is replicability. A phenomenon with
statistical variability, whether it is scoring home runs in baseball, curing a disease with
chemotherapy or observing psychic functioning, should exhibit about the same level of
success in the long run, over repeated experiments of a similar nature. The remote viewing
experiments are no exception. Remember that such events should not replicate with any
degree of precision in the short run because of statistical variability, just as we would not
expect to always get five heads and five tails if we flip a coin ten times, or see the same
batting averages in every game.
The analysis of SRI experiments conducted in 1988 singled out the laboratory remote viewing
sessions performed by six "expert" remote viewers, numbers 002, 009, 131, 372, 414 and
504. These six individuals contributed 196 sessions. The resulting effect size was 0.385
(May et al, 1988, p. 13). The SRI analysis does not include information individually by
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
viewer, nor does it include information about how many of the 196 sessions used static
versus dynamic targets. One report provided to the review team (May, Lantz and
Piantineda) included an additional experiment conducted after the 1988 review was
performed, in which Viewer 009 participated with 40 sessions. The effect size for Viewer
009 for those sessions was .363. None of the other six SRI experts were participants.
The same subject identifying numbers were used at SAIC, so we can compare the
performance for these individuals at SRI and SAIC. Of the six, three were specifically
mentioned as participating in the SAIC remote viewing experiments. As can be seen in
Table 2, viewers 009, 131 and 372 all participated in Experiments 1 and viewers 009 and
372 participated in Experiments 4, 9 and 10 as well.
The overall effect sizes for two of the three, viewers 009 and 372, were very close to the
SRI effect size of 0.385 for these subjects, at .35 and .34, respectively, and the .35 effect
size for Viewer 009 was very similar to his .363 effect size in the report by May, Lantz and
Piantineda (1994). Therefore, we see a repeated and, more importantly, hopefully a
repeatable level of functioning above chance for these individuals. An effect of this size
should be reliable enough to be sustained in any properly conducted experiment with enough
trials to obtain the long run statistical replicability required to rule out chance.
It is also important to notice that viewers 009 and 372 did well on the same experiments and
poorly on the same experiments. In fact the correlation between their effect sizes across
experiments is .901, which is very close to a perfect correlation of 1.0. This kind of
consistency warrants investigation to determine whether it is the nature of the experiments, a
statistical fluke or some methodological problems that led these two individuals to perform so
closely to one another. If methodological problems are responsible, then they must be subtle
indeed because the methodology was similar for many of the experiments, yet the results
were not. For instance, procedures for the sessions with static and dynamic targets in
Experiment 1 were almost identical to each other, yet the dynamic targets did not produce
evidence of psychic functioning (p-value = .50) and the static targets did (p-value = .0073).
Therefore, a methodological problem would have had to differentially effect results for the
two types of targets, even though the assignment of target type was random across sessions.
4.5 Methodological Issues in the Remote Viewing Experiments at SAIC
As noted in Section 2.3, there are a number of methodological considerations needed to
perform a careful remote viewing experiment. Information necessary to determine how well
each of these were addressed is generally available in the reports, but in some instances I
consulted Dr. May for additional information. As an example of how the methodological
issues in Section 2.3 were addressed, an explanation will be provided for Experiment 1.
In this experiment the viewers all worked from their homes (in New York, Kansas,
California, and Virginia). Dr. Nevin Lantz, who resided in Pennsylvania, was the principal
investigator. After each session, viewers faxed their response to Dr. Lantz and mailed the
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
original to SAIC. Upon receipt of the fax, Dr. Lantz mailed the correct answer to the
viewer. The viewers were supposed to mail their original responses to SAIC immediately,
after faxing them to Dr. Lantz. According to Dr. May, the faxed versions were later
compared with the originals to make sure the originals were sent without any changes. Here
are how the other methodological issues in Section 2.3 were handled:
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target should have any contact with the
viewer until after the response has been safely secured.
No one involved with the experiment had any contact with the viewers, since they
were not in the vicinity of either SAIC or Dr. Lantz's home in Pennsylvania.
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target or even of whether or not the session
was successful should have any contact with the judge until after that task has been
completed.
Dr. Lantz and the individual viewers were the only ones who knew the correct
answers, but according to Dr. May, they did not have any contact with the judge during the
period of this experiment.
? No one who has knowledge of the specific target should have access to the response
until after the judging has been completed.
Again, since only the viewers and Dr. Lantz knew the correct target, and since the
responses were mailed to SAIC by the viewers before they received the answers, this
condition appears to have been met.
? Targets and decoys used in judging should be selected using a well-tested
randomization device.
This has been standard practice at both SRI and SAIC.
? Duplicate sets of targets photographs should be used, one during the experiment and
one during the judging, so that no cues (like fingerprints) can be inserted onto the
target that would help the judge recognize it.
This was done; Dr. Lantz maintained the set used during the experiment while the set
used for judging was kept at SAIC in California.
? The criterion for stopping an experiment should be defined in advance so that it is not
called to a halt when the results just happen to be favorable. Generally, that means
specifying the number of trials in advance, but some statistical procedures require
other stopping rules. The important point is that the rule be defined in advance in
such a way that there is no ambiguity about when to stop.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
In advance it was decided that each viewer would contribute 40 trials, ten under each
of four conditions (all combinations of sender/no sender and static/dynamic). All sessions
were completed.
? Reasons, if any, for excluding data must be defined in advance and followed
consistently, and should not be dependent on the data. For example, a rule specking
that a trial could be aborted if the viewer felt ill would be legitimate, but only if the
trial was aborted before anyone involved in that decision knew the correct target.
No such reasons were given, nor was there any mention of any sessions being aborted
or discarded.
? Statistical analyses to be used must be planned in advance of collecting the data so
that a method most favorable to the data isn't selected post hoc. If multiple methods
of analysis are used the corresponding conclusions must recognize that fact.
The standard rank-order judging had been planned, with results reported separately
for each of the four conditions in the experiment for each viewer. Thus, 20 effect sizes were
reported, four for each of the five viewers.
4.6 Was Anything Learned at SAIC?
4.6.1 Target Selection. In addition to the question of whether or not psychic functioning is
possible, the experiments at SAIC were designed to explore a number of hypotheses.
Experiments 1 and 10 were both designed to see if there is a relationship between the
"change in visual entropy" in the targets and the remote viewing performance.
Each of the five senses with which we are familiar is a change detector. Our vision is most
readily drawn to something that is moving, and in fact if our eyes are kept completely still,
we cease to see at all. Similarly, we hear because of moving air, and our attention is drawn
to sudden changes in sound levels. Other senses behave similarly. Thus, it is reasonable
that if there really is a "psychic sense" then it would follow that same pattern.
Experiments 1 and 10 were designed to test whether or not remote viewing performance
would be related to a particular type of change in the target material, namely the "change in
visual entropy." A target with a high degree of change would be one in which the colors
changed considerably throughout the target. A detailed explanation can be found in the
SAIC reports of this experiment, or in the article "Shannon Entropy: A Possible Intrinsic
Target Property" by May, Spottiswoode and James, in the Journal of Parapsychology,
December 1994. It was indeed found that there was a correlation between the change in
entropy in the target and the remote viewing quality. This result was initially shown in
Experiment 1 and replicated in Experiment 10. A simulation study matching randomly
chosen targets to response showed that this was unlikely to be an artifact of target complexity
or other features.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
It is worth speculating on what this might mean for determining how psychic functioning
works. Physicists are currently grappling with the concept of time, and cannot rule out
precognition as being consistent with current understanding. Perhaps it is the case that we
do have a psychic sense, much like our other senses, and that it works by scanning the future
for possibilities of major change much as our eyes scan the environment for visual change
and our ears are responsive to auditory change. That idea is consistent with anecdotal
reports of precognition, which are generally concerned with events involving major life
change. Laboratory remote viewing may in part work by someone directing the viewer to
focus on a particular point in the future, that in which he or she receives the feedback from
the experiment. It may also be the case that this same sense can scan the environment in
actual time and detect change as well.
Another hypothesis put forth at SAIC was that laboratory remote viewing experiments are
most likely to be successful if the pool of potential targets is neither too narrow nor too wide
in terms of the number of possible elements in the target. They called this feature the
"target-pool bandwidth" and described it as the number of "differentiable cognitive
elements." They reasoned that if the possible target set was too small, the viewer would see
the entire set and be unable to distinguish that information from the psychic information. If
the set was too broad, the viewer would not have any means for editing an extensive
imagination.
Combining these two results would indicate that a good target set would contain targets with
high change in visual entropy, but that the set would contain a moderately-sized set of
possibilities. The set of 100 National Geographic photographs used in the later days at SRI
and at SAIC may have inadvertently displayed just those properties.
4.6.2. Remote Staring. Experiment 7, described in Appendix 2, provided results very
different from the standard remote viewing work. That experiment was designed to test
claims made in the Former Soviet Union and by some researchers in the United States, that
individuals could influence the physiology of another individual from a remote location. The
study was actually two separate replications of the same experiment, and both replications
were successful from a traditional statistical perspective. In other words, it appeared that the
physiology of one individual was activated when he or she was being watched by someone in
a distant room. If these results are indeed sound, then they may substantiate the folklore
indicating that people know when they are being observed from behind.
4.6.3 Enhanced Binary Computer Guessing. Experiment 2 was also very different from
the standard remote viewing experiments, although it was still designed to test anomalous
cognition. Three subjects attempted to use a statistical enhancement technique to increase the
ability to guess forced choice targets with two choices. This clever computer experiment
showed tx at for one subject, guessing was indeed enhanced from a raw rate of just above
chance (51.6% instead of 50%) to an enhanced rate of 76 percent. The method was
extremely inefficient, and it is difficult to imagine practical uses for this ability, if indeed it
exists.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
5. EXTERNAL VALIDATION: REPLICATIONS OF OTHER EXPERIMENTS
5.1. Conceptual Similarity: Ganzfeld Experiments
While remote viewing has been the primary activity at SRI and SAIC, other researchers have
used a similar technique to test for anomalous cognition, called the ganzfeld. As noted in the
SAIC Final Report of 29 Sept. 1994, the ganzfeld experiments differ from remote viewing in
three fundamental ways. First, a "mild altered state is used," second, senders are [usually]
used, so that telepathy is the primary mode, and third, the receivers (viewers) do their own
judging just after the session, rather than having an independent judge.
The ganzfeld experiments conducted at Psychophysical Research Laboratories (PRL) were
already mentioned in Section 3.4. Since the time those results were reported, other
laboratories have also been conducting ganzfeld experiments. At the 1995 Annual Meeting
of the Parapsychological Association, three replications were reported, all published in the
peer-reviewed Proceedings of the conference.
The ganzfeld experiments differ in the preferred method of analysis as well. Rather than
using the sum of the ranks across sessions, a simple count is made of how many first places
matches resulted from a series. Four rather than five choices are given, so by chance there
should be about 25 % of the sessions resulting in first place matches.
5.2 Ganzfeld Results from Four Laboratories
In publishing the ganzfeld results from PRL, Bern and Honorton (1994) excluded one of the
studies from the general analysis for methodological reasons, and found that the remaining
studies showed 106 hits out of 329 sessions, for a hit rate of 32.2 percent when 25 percent
was expected by chance. The corresponding p-value was .002. As mentioned earlier, the
hallmark of science is replication. This result has now been replicated by three additional
laboratories.
Bierman (1995) reported four series of experiments conducted at the University of
Amsterdam. Overall, there were 124 sessions and 46 hits, for a hit rate of 37 percent. The
hit rates for the four individual experiments were 34.3 percent, 37.5 percent, 40 percent and
36.1 percent, so the results are consistent across his four experiments.
Morris, Dalton, Delanoy and Watt (1995) reported results of 97 sessions conducted at the
University of Edinburgh in which there were 32 successes, for a hit rate of 33 percent. They
conducted approximately equal numbers of sessions under each of three conditions. In one
condition there was a known sender, and in the other two conditions it was randomly
determined at the last minute (and unknown to the receiver) that there would either be a
sender or not. Hit rates were 34 percent when there was a known sender and when there
was no sender, and 23 percent when there was a sender but the receiver did not know
whether or not there would be. They did discover post hoc that one experimenter was more
19
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
successful than the other two at achieving successful sessions, but the result was not beyond
what would be expected by chance as a post hoc observation.
Broughton and Alexander (1995) reported results from 100 sessions at the Institute for
Parapsychology in North Carolina. They too found a similar hit rate, wit-i 33 hits out of 100
sessions, or 33 percent hits.
Results from the original ganzfeld work and these three replications are summarized in Table
3, along with the SRI and SAIC remote viewing results. The effect sizes for the ganzfeld
replications are based on Cohen's h, which is similar in type to the effect size used for the
remote viewing data. Both effect sizes measure the number of standard deviations the results
fall above chance, using the standard deviation for a single session.
TABLE 3: REMOTE VIEWING AND GANZFELD REPLICATIONS
Laboratory
Sessions
Hit Rate
Effect Size
All Remote Viewing at SRI
770
N/A
.209
All Remote Viewing at SAIC
455
N/A
.230
PRL, Princeton, NJ
329
32 percent
.167
University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
124
37 percent
.261
University of Edinburgh, Scotland
97
33 percent
.177
Institute for Parapsychology, NC
100
33 percent
.177
5.3 Conclusions about External Replication
The results shown in Table 3 show that remote viewing has been conceptually replicated
across a number of laboratories, by various experimenters and in different cultures. This is a
robust effect that, were it not in such an unusual domain, would no longer be questioned by
science as a real phenomenon. It is unlikely that methodological problems could account for
the remarkable consistency of results shown in Table 3.
6. IS REMOTE VIEWING USEFUL?
Even if we were all to agree that anomalous cognition is possible, there remains the question
of whether or not it would have any practical use for government purposes. The answer to
that question is beyond the scope of this report, but some speculations can be made about
how to increase the usefulness.
First, it appears that anomalous cognition is to some extent possible in the general
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
population. None of the ganzfeld experiments used exclusively selected subjects. However,
it also appears that certain individuals possess more talent than others, and that it is easier to
find those individuals than to train people. It also appears to be the case that certain
individuals are better at some tasks than others. For instance, Viewer 372 at SAIC appears
to have a facility with describing technical sites.
Second, if remote viewing is to be useful, the end users must be trained in what it can do
and what it cannot. Given our current level of understanding, it is rarely 100 percent
accurate, and there is no reliable way to learn what is accurate and what is not. The same is
probably true of most sources of intelligence data.
Third, what is useful for one purpose may not be useful for another. For instance, suppose a
remote viewer could describe the setting in which a hostage is being held. That information
may not be any use at all to those unfamiliar with the territory, but could be useful to those
familiar with it.
7. CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS
It is clear to this author that anomalous cognition is possible and has been demonstrated.
This conclusion is not based on belief, but rather on commonly accepted scientific criteria.
The phenomenon has been replicated in a number of forms across laboratories and cultures.
The various experiments in which it has been observed have been different enough that if
some subtle methodological problems can explain the results, then there would have to be a
different explanation for each type of experiment, yet the impact would have to be similar
across experiments and laboratories. If fraud were responsible, similarly, it would require
an equivalent amount of fraud on the part of a large number of experimenters or an even
larger number of subjects.
What is not so clear is that we have progressed very far in understanding the mechanism for
anomalous cognition. Senders do not appear to be necessary at all; feedback of the correct
answer may or may not be necessary. Distance in time and space do not seem to be an
impediment. Beyond those conclusions, we know very little.
I believe that it would be wasteful of valuable resources to continue to look for proof. No
one who has examined all of the data across laboratories, taken as a collective whole, has
been able to suggest methodological or statistical problems to explain the ever-increasing and
consistent results to date. Resources should be directed to the pertinent questions about how
this ability works. I am confident that the questions are no more elusive than any other
questions in science dealing with small to medium sized effects, and that if appropriate
resources are targeted to appropriate questions, we can have answers within the next decade.
S
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
8. REFERENCES
Bem, Daryl J. and Charles Honorton (1994). "Does psi exist? Replicable evidence for an
anomalous process of information transfer," Psychological Bulletin, 115, 4-18.
Bierman, Dick J. (1995). "The Amsterdam Ganzfeld Series III & IV: Target clip
emotionality, effect sizes and openness," Proceedings of the 38th Annual Parapsychological
Association Convention, 27-37.
Broughton, Richard and Cheryl Alexander (1995). "Autoganzfeld II: The first 100 sessions,"
Proceedings of the 38th Annual Parapsychological Association Convention, 53-61.
May, Edwin C. (1995). "AC Technical trials: Inspiration for the target entropy concept,"
May 26, 1995, SAIC Technical Report.
May, Edwin C., Nevin D. Lantz and Tom Piantineda (1994). "Feedback Considerations in
Anomalous Cognition Experiments," Technical Report, 29 November 1994.
May, Edwin C., J.M. Utts, V.V. Trask, W.W. Luke, T.J. Frivold and B.S. Humphrey
(1988). "Review of the psychoenergetic research conducted at SRI International (1973-1988)"
SRI International Technical Report, March 1989.
Morris, Robert L., Kathy Dalton, Deborah Delanoy and Caroline Watt (1995). "Comparison
of the sender/no sender condition in the Ganzfeld," Proceedings of the 3Sth Annual
Parapsychological Association Convention, 244-259.
Puthoff, Harold E. and Russell Targ (1975). "Perceptual Augmentation Techniques: Part
Two--Research Report," Stanford Research Institute Final Report, Dec. 1, 1975.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
APPENDIX 1
EFFECT SIZE MEASURE USED WITH RANK ORDER JUDGING
In general, effect sizes measure the number of standard deviation the true population value of
interest falls from the value that would be true if chance alone were at work. The standard
deviation used is for one subject, trial, etc., rather than being the standard error of the
sample statistic used in the hypothesis test.
In rank-order judging, let R; be the rank for trial i. If the number of possible choices is N,
then we find:
E(R) = (N + 1)12
NZ - l
Var(R;) =
Therefore, when N = 5, we find E(RA) = 3 and Var(R) = 2. The effect size is therefore:
EffectSize = (3.0 - Average Rank)
F
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
APPENDIX 2
A BRIEF DESCRIPTION OF THE SAIC EXPERIMENTS
Experiments Involving Remote Viewing
There were six experiments involving remote viewing, done for a variety of purposes.
Experiment 1: Target and Sender Dependencies:
Purpose: This experiment was designed to test whether or not a sender is necessary for
successful remote viewing and whether or not dynamic targets, consisting of short video
clips, would result in more successful remote viewing than the standard National Geographic
photographs used in most of the SRI experiments.
Method: Five experienced remote viewers participated, three of whom (#s 009, 131 and
372) were included in the experienced group at SRI; their identification numbers were
carried over to the SAIC experiments. Each viewer worked from his or her home and faxed
the results of the sessions to the principal investigator, Nevin Lantz, located in Pennsylvania.
Whether the target was static or dynamic and whether or not there was a sender was
randomly determined and unknown to the viewer. Upon receiving the fax of the response,
Dr. Lantz mailed the correct answer to the viewer. The original response was sent to SAIC
in California, where the results were judged by an analyst blind to the correct target.
Standard rank-order judging was used.
Since it is not explicitly stated, I asked Dr. May what measures were taken to make sure the
viewer actually mailed the original response to SAIC before receiving the correct answer in
the mail. He said that the original faxed responses were compared with the responses
received by SAIC to make sure they were the same, and they all were.
Results: Each viewer contributed ten trials under each of the four possible conditions
(sender/no sender and static/dynamic target), for a total of 40 trials per viewer. There was a
moderate difference (effect size = .121, p = .08) between the static and dynamic targets,
with the traditional National Geographic photographs faring better than the dynamic video
clips. There was no noticeable difference based on whether or not a sender was involved,
supporting the same conclusion reached in the overall analysis of the SRI work. Combined
over all conditions and all viewers, the effect size was 0.124 (p = .04); for the static targets
alone it was .248 (exact p = .0073) while for the dynamic targets it was 0.00 (p = .50).
Discussion: The SAIC staff speculated that the dynamic targets were not successful because
the possibilities were too broad. They chose a new set of dynamic targets to be more similar
to the static targets and performed another experiment the following year to compare the
static targets with the more similar set of dynamic ones. That experiment is described below
(Experiment 10.)
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Experiment 4? Enhancing Detection of AC with Binary Coding:
Purpose: This experiment was designed to see if remote viewing could be used to develop a
message-sending capability by focusing on the presence or absence of five specific features of
a target. The target set was constructed in packets of four, with possible combinations of the
absence (0) or presence (1) of each of the five features chosen to correspond to the numbers
00000, 01110, 10101, and 11011. This is standard practice in information theory when
trying to send a two digit number (00, 01, 10 or 11); the remaining three bits are used for
"error correction." Different sets of five features were used for each of ten target packs.
Method: Five viewers each contributed eight trials, but the same eight targets were used for
all five viewers. There was no sender used, and viewers were told that each target would be
in a fixed location for one week. They were to spend 15 minutes trying to draw the target,
then fax their responses to SAIC in California. The results were blind judged and the binary
features were coded by both the viewers and an independent analyst.
Results: The results were unsuccessful in showing any evidence of psychic functioning.
Neither standard rank-order judging nor analysis based on the binary guesses showed any
promise that this method works to send messages.
Experiment 5: AC in Lucid Dreams (Baseline):
Purpose: Despite its name, this experiment did not involve lucid dreaming. Instead, it was
used to test three novice remote viewers who were to participate in an experiment involving
remote viewing while dreaming. This baseline experiment was designed to see if these
individuals would be successful at standard laboratory remote viewing.
Method: For this baseline experiment, each of the three viewers contributed eight trials
using a standard protocol common in the SRI era. For each trial, a target was randomly
chosen from the set of 100 National Geographic targets used at SRI and SAIC. The target
was placed on a table (so no sender was used) while the viewer, in another room, was asked
to provide a description. The response was later blind judged by comparing it to the target
and four decoys, and providing a rank-ordering of the five choices.
Results: Of the three novice viewers, one obtained a promising effect size of .265, although
the result was not statistically significant due to the small number of trials (8). Individual
results were not provided for the other two viewers, but the overall effect size was reported
as 0.088 for the three viewers.
Experiment 6: AC in Lucid Dreams (Pilot):
Purpose: A lucid dream is a dream in which one becomes aware that he or she is dreai-.iing,
and can control subsequent events in the dream. This ability has apparently been
successfully trained by Dr. Stephen LaBerge of the Lucidity Institute. He was the Principal
Investigator for this experiment. The experiment was designed to see if remote viewing
could be successfully employed while the viewer was having a lucid dream.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Method: Seven remote viewers were used; four were experienced SAIC remote viewers and
three were experienced lucid dreamers from the Lucidity Institute. The latter three were the
novice viewers used in Experiment 5. The experienced SAIC remote viewers were given
training in lucid dreaming. The number of trials contributed by each viewer could not be
fixed in advance because of the difficulty of attaining the lucid dream state. A total of 21
trials were conducted, with the seven viewers contributing anywhere from one to seven trials
each. The report did not mention whether or not the stopping criterion was fixed in advance,
but according to Dr. May the experiment was designed to proceed for a fixed time period
and to include all sessions attained during that time period.
Unlike with standard well-controlled protocols, the viewers were allowed to take the target
material home with them. The targets, selected from the standard National Geographic pool,
were sealed in opaque envelopes with covert threads to detect possible tampering (there were
no indications of such tampering). Viewers were instructed to place the targets at bedside
and to attempt a lucid dream in which the envelope was opened and the target viewed.
Drawings and descriptions were then to be produced upon awakening.
Results: The results were blind judged using the standard sum of ranks. Since the majority
of viewers contributed only one or two trials, analysis by individual viewer would be
meaningless. For the 21 trials combined, the effect size was 0.368 (p = .046). Information
was not provided to differentiate the novice remote viewers from the experienced ones.
Experiment 9: ERD AC Behavior:
Purpose: The remote viewing in this experiment was conducted in conjunction with
measurement of brain waves using an EEG. The purpose of the experiment was to see
whether or not EEG activity would change when the target the person was attempting to
describe was briefly displayed on a computer monitor in a distant room. Details of the EEG
portion will be explained as experiment 8. Here, we summarize the remote viewing part of
the study.
Method: Three experienced remote viewers (Its 009, 372 and 389) participated. Because of
the pilot nature of the experiment, the number of trials differed for each viewer based on
availability, with viewers 009, 372 and 389 contributing 18, 24 and 28 trials, respectively.
Although it is not good protocol to allow an unspecified number of trials, it does not appear
that this problem can explain the results of this experiment.
Results: Responses were blind judged using standard rank-order analysis. The effect sizes
for the viewers 009, 372 and 389 were 0.432 (p = .033), 0.354 (p = .042) and 0.177 (p =
.175), respectively. The overall effect size was 0.303 (p = 0.006).
Experiment 10: Entropy II:
Purpose: This experiment was designed as an improved version of Experiment 1. After the
unsuccessful showing for the dynamic targets in Experiment 1, the SAIC team speculated that
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
the "target pool bandwidth" defined as the number of "cognitively differentiable elements"
in the target pool might be an important factor. If the possible target material was extremely
broad, viewers might have trouble filtering out extraneous noise. If the set of possibilities
was too small, as in forced choice experiments, the viewer would see all choices at once and
would have trouble filtering out that knowledge. An intermediate range of possibilities, too
large to be considered all at once, was predicted to be ideal. The standard National
Geographic pool seemed to fit that range. For this experiment, a pool of dynamic targets
was created with a similar "band-width." In both Experiments (1 and 10) the researchers
predicted that remote viewing success would correlate with the change in visual entropy of
the target, as explained in Section 4.6.1.
Method: Four of the five viewers from Experiment 1 were used (#s 009, 372, 389 and
518). They each contributed equal numbers of sessions with static and dynamic targets, with
the viewers blind to which trials had which type. Senders were not used, and all sessions ..
were conducted at SAIC in California, unlike Experiment 1 in which the viewers worked at
home. Viewer #372 contributed 15 of each type while the others each contributed 10 of each
type. Standard rank-order judging was used.
Results: Table 4 shows the results for this experiment. Unlike in Experiment 1, the static
and dynamic targets produced identical effect sizes, with both types producing very
successful results. The combined effect size for all trials is .55, resulting in a z-score of
5.22.
TABLE 4: RESULTS FOR EXPERIMENT 10
Static Targets
Dynamic Targets
Viewer
Rank
ES
P
Rank
ES
p
009
2.20
.565
.037
1.70
.919
1.8 x 10-3
372
1.87
.801
9.7 x 10-4
1.93
.754
1.8X10-3
389
3.10
-.071
.589
3.00
.000
.500
518
1.90
.778
7.2 x 10-3
2.40
.424
.091
Total
2.22
.550
1.1x105
2.22
.550
1.1 x 10-5
The Other Experiments at SAIC
There were four additional experiments at SAIC, not involving remote viewing. Two of
them (experiments 3 and 8) involved trying to measure brain activity related to psychic
functioning and will be described briefly. Experiment 3 used a magnetoenchephalograph
(MEG) to attempt to detect anomalous signals in the brain when a remote stimulus was
present. Due to the background noise in the brain measurements and the expected strength
of the signal, the experimenters realized too late that they would not be able to detect a
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
signal even if it existed. Experiment 8 utilized an EEG to try to detect the interruption of
alpha waves when a remote viewing target was briefly displayed on a computer monitor in
another room. The area of the brain tested was that corresponding to visual stimuli. No
significant change in alpha was seen.
The remaining two experiments were replications of previous work measuring psychic
functioning in areas other than remote viewing. They will be described in detail.
Experiment 2: AC of Binary Targets:
Purpose: This experiment attempted to replicate and enhance random number generator
experiments conducted at SRI. In these types of experiments a computer randomly selects
one of two choices to be the target, denoted as 0 or 1. The internal workings of the
computer then rapidly oscillate between 0 and 1 and the subject pushes a mouse button when
he or she thinks the internal choice matches the target choice. This process is repeated over
many trials. The computer tabulates the results and the experiment is a success if the subject
guesses the correct answer more often than would be expected by chance. The purpose is to
see if humans can correctly guess computer-selected binary targets, and hopefully by
extension, correctly solve binary choice problems in real situations. If that were to be the
case, then real problems could be posed as binary ones (e.g. is the lost child still in this city
or not) to narrow down possibilities.
Method: This SAIC experiment was designed to enhance the accuracy of binary guessing by
using a statistical technique called sequential analysis. Rather than just one guess for each
decision, the subject continu- s to guess until the computer ascertains that a decision has been
reached. The computer keeps track of the number of times zero and one have each been
guessed and announces a decision when one of the choices has clearly won out over the
other, or when it is clear that it is essentially an ongoing tie. In the latter case, no decision
is recorded. Three subjects participated (#s 007, 083 and 531) in this experiment. Subject
#531 had been successful in similar experiments at SRI.
Results: Using this method for enhancing the accuracy of the guesses, subject #531, who
had been successful in previous similar experiments, was able to achieve 76 correct answers
out of 100 tries. This remarkable level of scoring for this type of experiment resulted in an
effect size of .520 and a z-score of 5.20. The other two subjects did not differ from chance
results, with 44 and 49 correct decisions out of 100 or 101. (One subject accidentally
contributed an additional trial.)
Although the result for subject 531 is remarkably successful, it does not represent a very
efficient method of obtaining the decision. To reach the 100 decisions required a total of
21,337 guesses, i.e. over 200 guesses for each decision. Of the individual guesses, only
51.6 percent were correct, for an effect size of .032, similar to other forced choice
experiments. Due to the large number of guesses, the corresponding z-score was 4.65.
Combined over all three subjects, 56 percent of the 301 decisions were correct and the effect
size was 0.123. The combined results were still statistically significant, with p = 0.0 17, as
shown in Table 1.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Experiment 7: Remote Observation:
Purpose: It is often reported anecdotally that people know when they are being watched.
Two experiments were conducted at SAIC to determine whether or not these anecdotes could
be supported by a change in physiology when someone is being observed from a distance.
The experimental design was essentially the same for the two experiments. This work was a
conceptual replication of results reported by researchers in the Former Soviet Union (FSU),
the United States and Scotland. The experiments in the FSU were interpreted to mean that
the physiology of the recipient was being manipulated by the sender, an effect that if real
could have frightening consequences.
Method: The "observee" was seated in a room with a video camera focused on him or her,
and with galvanic skin response measurements being recorded. In a distant room the
"observer" attempted to influence the physiology of the observee at randomly spaced time
intervals. During those time intervals, an image of the observee appeared on a computer
monitor watched by the observer. During "control" periods, the video camera remained
focused on the observee but the computer monitor did not display his or her image to the
observer. There were 16 "influence" periods randomly interspersed with 16 "control"
periods, each of 30 seconds, with blank periods of 0 to 5 seconds inserted to rule out
patterns in physiology.
Results: To determine whether or not the galvanic skin response of the observees was
activated while they were being watched, the response during the control periods was
compared with the response during the "influence" periods for each subject. The results
were then averaged across subjects. In both experiments, there was greater activity during
the periods of being watched than there was during the control periods. The results were
statistically significant in each case (p = .036 and .014) and the effect sizes were similar, at
0.39 and 0.49. As preplanned, the results were combined, yielding an effect size of .39 (p
= .005). As an interesting post hoc observation, it was noted that the effect was
substantially stronger when the observer and observee were of opposite sexes than when they
were of the same sex.
Discussion: This experiment differs from the others conducted at SAIC since it involves
interaction between two people rather than one person ascertaining information about the
environment or the future. It raises substantially more questions than it answers, since the
mechanism for the shift in physiology is unknown. Possibilities range from the idea that the
observee was able to know when the computer in the distant room was displaying his or her
image, not unlike remote viewing, to the possibility that the observer actually did influence
the physiology of the observee. Further experimentation as well as a review of similar past
experiments may be able to shed light on this important question.
29
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, DAVIS
BERKELEY ? DAVIS ? IRVINE ? LOS ANGELES ? RIVERSIDE ? SAX DIEGO ? SAN FRANCISCO
Mike Mumford
American Institutes for Research
3333 K Street North West
Suite 300
Washington, DC 20007
August 31, 1995
Dear Mike:
Here is my report. I hope I didn't get too carried away.
It is Thursday at 4pm and I have not received Ray's report. Perhaps he expected you to send
a copy to me. I am sending him a copy of my report today.
I look forward to our conference call on Tuesday. I hope you and Andy have time to read this
opus work before then!
With best wishes,
Jessica Utts
Professor
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00791 R000200070001-9