SEVEN PAGES FROM BOOK 'PARAPSYCHOLOGY AND OTHER FIELDS'

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP96-00788R001100200009-0
Release Decision: 
RIFPUB
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
7
Document Creation Date: 
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date: 
June 17, 1998
Sequence Number: 
9
Case Number: 
Content Type: 
OPEN
File: 
Body: 
Approved F When there is a very large number of targets, such as all the drawable words in a die enary, data are best evaluated by a special formula developed by R. A. Fisher Carington, I944). The baseline for chance expectation is determined by the num er of responses for a word (or other item) when it was not the target. The meth thus requires many test administrations with diverse targets, to pro- vide a large espouse pool. (Having a large response pool is especially important because Cari ton, after years of working with the technique, found that it is advisable to u narrow categories for scoring a hit. For example, he reports a substantially hig r hit ratio when "elm" was scored correct only for the target "elm" than when elm, along with oak, maple, poplar, etc., were all classed together as hits for y kind of tree.) A problem here is that any particular catalogue of response requencies, such as the one Carington prepared, may be inappropriate for sub( is drawn from a different population or tested at a dffereat time. Open-ended Responses. Wt the target is complex, no response is likely to be completely right or wrong. Tlt standard way of scoring such data is to use blind judges who score each response gainst each of the targets. For example, a sub- ject may try on ten nights to d am about whatever target picture an agent happened, that night, to select fro a target pool. Then each of three blind ;judges (whose scores will be average is presented with a 10 X 10 array. Along the top are listed the ten pictures, in ra dom order;down the side are the dream reports. The judge rates (for example, n a scale of 0 to 100) how well the dream reports for the first night correspon to each of the ten pictures. He then continues with ratings for each of the subse ent nights. Analysis of variance of the array shows whether the ten correct targe response pairings are significantly different from the 90 incorrect pairings. An alternative with the same general approach Ns to have the blind judge rank ch response for how well it matches the various rgets. With ten possibilities, ')e would enter into the array 1, 2, 3...10 for e :\-h response. Some experi- menters choose to class all high ranks together, and a low ranks together, but such pooling of scores discards so much information th it seems inadvisable. Similar ratings or rankings can be used to judge the ccuracy of "readings" fife histories). Good research must be double blind. The erson for whom the reading is held is absent. The proxy sitter (a blind note aker) asks for the reading and records it. The best judge of the accuracy of a re ding is ordinarily he absent sitter for whom the sitting was held. Each absent s ter is then pre- sented with the entire set of readings, coded so that he does not k w which was thtended for him, and is asked to rate or to rank them. The expe ' enter puts these ratings or rankings into an array for analysis of variance: read gs are the columns, and sitters' scores are the rows. In'scorlng, an alternative to such global ranking or rating requires tha\ a blind , experimenter put a pair of parentheses after each scorable item in the res ?nses. Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100200009-0 renthcses would, for example, litter the protocol thus: "I think this is a man ()f about 50 or 55 () who is going to have a lot of happiness in his future. He hash some troubles in his past, but things will go better for him soon. He is very clo to someone named Anna . He wears a green ( )jacket () with brass O button )." The subject is asked to respond to each item singly, so far as possible, so at a woman of 55 who wore a red sweater with a brass clasp should respond `firmatively to the age and the brass, but not to the man, green, jacket, or buttons. Responses typically consist of entering each pair of paren- theses with a check the item is clearly right, a cross if it is wrong, and a ques- tion mark if it is ambig us. (Subjects find this easier than entering only checks and crosses.) Checks are hen summed, and the frequency of checks for each reading is entered into the pr ocol X subject array. The items on any of these p tocols may be subdivided, and a different array set up for each division. Dream otocols, for example, may have items evalu- ated separately for responses that ere or were not associated with color. Readings for absent sitters may have ' ems subdivided for personality descrip- tions of the sitters, references to living dividuals, references to dead indi- viduals, etc. Open-ended Responses Scored as Forced Choi c A method for retaining both freedom in the subject's response and also the situp, ity of forced-choice scoring is to use complex material for the target but take on prespecified categories as the scorable responses. Schmeidler and Lewis (1'96 prepared a set of 81 pictures which showed all combinations of three levels four variables: sex (two males, two females or one male and one female); age (y fig, adult but not old, old); activity (passive, normal, active); and emotion (u appy, neutral, happy). Subjects were told that each picture showed two peop ;they were asked to describe the picture; and responses were scored for accu iy on the four variables. Honorton (1975) has recently prepared an assembly `1.024 pictures which permits binary scoring (present or absent) for all combinati Hof ten variables. Physiological,Pleasures. When targets are preselected as emotionally neutral or emotionally charged, the subject's physiological changes can appropriately be used as the response measure. Tart (1963) applied painful electric shock to the agent on some trials of a GESP experiment and measured subjects' responses by CSR, EEG, plethysmograph, and key taps (conscious report). Each of the phys- iological measures showed a significant difference between shock and nonshock trials, but the key taps did not. Another example is research reported by Dean (1966), who made use of only a single physiological measure. in his procedure, the subject lay quietly in a darkened room. A plethysmograph record showed changes in his finger's fluid volume (a measure of autonomic activity). The agent in another room looked at names, randomly ordered, of three types: persons important to the subject but not to the experimenter; persons important Approved Fbr Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 ,.~, r,v,_s rGrf ~Efi n,.3:..cU ntaeAnt,!s Ur4 tS~' Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 nr .u ....w? u s, :v autjccr, uuu persons Knows; to neither. Timing was recorded on the plethysmograph record. Scoring of plethysmograph changes was blind. Dean's data, and those of several similar experiments, showed increase in autonomic activity when the agent contemplated a name personally relevant to the subject. Typically, as found by Tart, the subject reported no awareness of the target. Other possibilities are obvious but have not as yet been adequately explored. No controlled research has been published on the wave form of the cortical evoked potential, e.g., for auditory vs. visual ESP targets. Only preliminary in- vestigation reports similarity in the timing of alpha waves as the ESP response in paired subjects. Selection of Appropriate ESP Targets The experimenter, obviously, need not restrict himself to using ESP cards as argets. There is an infinite number of possibilities, and for some subjects or The`rpnge of choice might be suggested by the following list, culled from 5 years of rep s in a single journal. The Journal of the American Society for Psychical Resecrch"in 1970-1974, published ESP research with these targets: cards that were green 'one side and white on the other; the inner containers that con- cealed these ca: s; ESP cards; multiple-choice questions, each consisting of four items relevant to" ~story that the subject had heard; elaborate pictures (usually art prints or magazfhe~ illustrations); slides of patterns and faces (including the subject's own face); Haines (including the subject's own name); the sex of a person in a concealed pho graph; a particular square within a 5 X 5 matrix; red vs. black papers; Identi-Kit c ponents to match the face of a target person; a multisensory environment whit the subject would soon experience; erotic pic- tures affixed to some ESP cards, the timing of radioactive emissions from a Schmidt machine; several sets of five,.nature cards; the 12 positions of a clock face; audio-visual programs of slides a d music; geometric symbols; the five vowels; a pool of 100 simple line drawin of objects, with their names; char- acteristics of the persons who would sit in, specified auditorium chairs; word associates; relevant statements about individuals't concealed photographs; series 14 Selection of Appropriate Subjects have been reported for such diverse animals as cockroaches, lizart rodents, matic work has been done in comparative parapsychology, ex chance data Species. Humans are not necessarily the subjects of choice. "Ilzbough no syste- cats, dogs, horses, and humans, Probably the two best directives for the researt worker are: (a) to study psi in the species where he is himself most expert as a experimentalist; and (b) to set up the laboratory conditions and reinforcemer schedules which most sensitively elicit meaningful data in other types research Demographic Variables. Since psi has been demonstrated in young childrel adolescents, and adults, in males and females, and in many races, the same dire tives apply as'in the preceding paragraph. in general, the experimenter shoal work with the subjects with whom he feels most comfortable and whom he most competent to test. remarkably high. Preselection of Subjects. If the experimenter wants to study the relation of p to some other variable';, such as imagery, creativity, extroversion, or psychosil he may choose to use preselected criterion groups. it may be even more nece; nary in psi than in other research, however, to do careful pretesting to ensue that such special conditions'as the choice of targets, the setting, or the connot tions of the wording of the instructions do not in themselves have a differenti effect upon the groups. In the more common case of pr selection, the experimenter may choose to us gifted subjects. Here he can fail into a trap. The natural way to find gifted sub jects is to look for those who have`,,shown marked psi success outside of th' laboratory, but this is often counterproductive. A subject who considers himse] gifted while working ender his own conditions will often feel either a sullen or fierce resentment of laboratory restrictions, and this attitude may defeat the experimenter's purpose in selecting him. A method recently used successfully (Ta4 ,\1976), and which may serve as t model, used two preselection steps. The first consisted of group testing by as experimenter who told his subjects that he wad looking for good subjects fo later work and who used rather dull targets. Subje`ts with promising scores were invited to come to the laboratory, where the same a perimenter had them wor4 on two types of interesting ESP machines. Those th the better scores w:' invited for a third session with the same experimenter, in which they wort. with the machine that they preferred. Their scores on,the third session weir The three conspicuous virtues of this approach are: (a~ the subject's goal during the selection trials is to do well enough for later laboratory work, and thus the later work comes as no surprise to him; (b) the experimenter effect is controlled, since it is the experimenter for whom the subject \oduced earlier high scores who works with him later; and (c) test conditions be' ome increas- ingly pleasant. ase 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788 R001100200009-0 .gtrong? On very high-scoring runs he showed alpha abundance before and during ~~ actual time of :,-tr , .e d e just before making his choices, but during the choosing p perioderiod itself g -t self his m his alp abundance dropped. This study at present is the only EEG study involving very st nor psi performance by the subject. Since only one subject was used, the results y not be generalizable. Some re Its are more complex. Lewis and Schmeidler (1971) found a signifi- cant positi relationship between proportion of correct choices and alpha abundance w n their subjects were not aware that they were participating in an ESP test, and a ignificant negative relationship on those runs in which the sub- jects were aware hat they were participating in an ESP test. Stanford (1971) and Stanford and tanford (1969) found no significant relationship between alpha abundance an overall ESP score but did find that high ESP scores were associated with an incr se in the frequency of the alpha rhythm from just before ESP symbol guessing to he guessing period itself. Other studies have not re- ported alpha frequency s "t data. Stanford interpreted such an increase in fre- quency as representing a p ssible "coping" response in which the subject is mobilizing himself for what h construes to be a difficult task, yet at the same time he maintains a general star of relaxed awareness. Stanford's procedures in these studies were described to - e subjects as being precognition procedures rather than clairvoyance, even tho h in fact the random numbers that would determine the targets had themselves ready been selected. Most people intrin- sically regard precognition as a more di cult task than clairvoyance. Only three EEG studies involving fry -response procedures have been pub- lished. Stanford and Stevenson (1973), t Ling Stanford, found his alpha abun- dance not related to his ability to describe concealed line drawing. However, they did find that alpha frequency during a pre ;?ninary mind-clearing period was negatively related to ESP success and also fou, that an increase in alpha fre- quency from the mind-clearing period to the fol wing period of image forma- tion about the target drawing was associated with i success. The two findings are not independent, of course. Rao and Feola (19 found that a single sub- ject familiar with biofeedback and meditation was mor successful at describing concealed magazine pictures when he was asked to pr uce high alpha than when he was asked to produce low alpha. Stanford and men (1975) found that above-chance scorers on a free verbal response task f volving concealed photographs showed significantly more alpha abundance th n below-chance scorers, during both a preliminary period of listening to soot 'ng music and during the ima 1 gery erio As a whole, the EEG results are confusing and contradictory. Part f the prob- lem is that the procedures and methods of data analysis varied wi ely from study to study. The most consistent finding was that alpha abundance t -ded to be positively associated with high ESP scores, especially for subjects pres ected for expertise at the production of one or both. The studies which found . re- tive Q plif i lt}~ i~t9 kQ s sQQa11 O2 i a nega- lationship between alpha abundance and ESP. Ve Tenny ( ittle work has been done with other psychophysiological measurements. 62) failed to find any relationship between galvanic skin response act- ivity (GS choice succ ruff and Dale or vasomotor activity as measured by plethysmograph, and ESP although he did obtain significantly positive ESP results. Wood- 952) found a positive but not quite significant relationship be- ty and ESP card-guessing success, in two consecutive studies. tween GSR acti Otani (1955) fours tance changes as m significantly more hits on ESP cards during large skin resis- ured by basal skin response (BSR)- This result was ob- tained only under a su1 eyes open. Brand and Br tion-inducing instructions whereas those who heard finding was that, over all t condition of indifference toward the results and with d (1974) found that subjects who heard taped relaxa- tored above chance in a free response GESP test, ision-inducing tape scored at chance. A related with less frontalis muscle activit s, positive scoring was significantly associated measured by electromyograph or EMG) dur- with a decrease in frontalis activity from ing the target impression period, a beginning to end of the session. The definable "relaxation state" was very c auds hypothesized that a physiologically- ducive to psi in the receiving organism. y suggested: lowered frequency and In addition to reduced muscle activity, increased amplitude of EEG; lower hear activity; increased basal skin resistance; lower blood lactate level. This overall schema is als e, blood pressure, and vasomotor ygen consumption; and reduced compatible with the results of ow strong free response ESP W'hite's (1964a) finding that people purported to ability consistently described themselves as enterir. mind-clearing at the onset. period of relaxation and Too little work has been done to evaluate the accur y or generality of the Brauds' "relaxation syndrome." Certainly the negative orrelations between alpha abundance and psi performance in two studies, plus t findings of Stan- ford that an increased alpha frequency is correlated with psi o ertain psi tasks, are at odds with the Brauds' ideas. Woodruff and Dale's and Ot _a results with GSR and BSR are also contrary to the implications of this syndrod, . However, the Brauds did not state that relaxation was the only condition fo strong psi performance. Perhaps there are complex interactive relationships am g state- trait subject variables, the nature of the psi task, and the optimum physi ogical processing of psi information. There is evidence that physiological variables are correlated in a variety of ways with psi performance and are therefore in some way involved. Manifesta? tions of psi are often complex, confused, and probably misprocessed within the Approved For Release 2000108107: CIA-RDP96-00788R001100200009-0 I Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 organism at some level. Perhaps therefore we should look directly at changes in psych ophsi ologi cal aspects themselves as indicators of the presence of a psi mes- sage. By so doing, we may be able to look crudely at the psi information during relatively early stages of its processing within the organism. Although the data will lack the richness of experience, they may be more consistent and may addi- tionally eventually tell us a great deal about the processing elements themselves. Targ and Puthoff (1974) flashed a strobe in the eyes of a sender and observed the response of the receiver's occipital EEG. One agent-receiver pair was selected for more extensive work, on the basis of preliminary success plus the monochromatic EEG spectrum of the receiver. During strobe periods, the aver- age power and peak power of the receiver's alpha rhythms significantly de- creased, indicating partial alpha blockage to remote visual information. At the same time, the subject was unable to guess with any accuracy above chance which periods were strobe periods and which were control. Tart (1963) found that subjects in a soundproof room showed a faster and more complex EEG pattern plus more active GSR and plethysmograph responses when a distant agent was receiving strong shocks than during control times. At the same time, receivers showed no behavioral evidence of responsiveness to these distant events, in terms of frequency of key presses made during shock vs. control times. Kelly and Lenz (1976) employed a similar procedure with a re- ceiver selected for monochromatic EEG, but without an agent. The receiver re- laxed, eyes closed, and simply tried to visualize the target area and whether or not the strobe was on. No attempt was made to guess when the strobe was on or off. Using a variety of preliminary procedures, they obtained suggestive evi- dence that the EEG responded differentially to stimulus vs. control conditions and that the nature of the response may be dependent upon such parameters as intertrial interval, body position, and so on. Duane and Behrendt (1965) found some suggestion that increasing alpha abundance in one twin led to increased alpha in a remote identical twin, but the overall results were not significant. The Research Committee of the A.S.P.R. (1959) found no significant EEG changes in receivers during times in which agents were being emotionally stimulated. Lloyd (1973) employed an averaged cortical evoked potential as a measure of responsiveness to the sudden onset of a distant stimulus. An agent was instruc- ted to send a visual image each time a light flashed. During a run, 60 such flashes were entered on the EEG record in such a way that the EEG output be- fore, during, and after the flash onset could be averaged to see if a coherent sig- nal emerged in response to the onset of the remote stimulus. By visual inspec- tion, such a cortical response seemed to be present. Lack of a control condition prevented statistical analysis, however. Millar (1976) repeated this procedure using control periods and found no evidence for psi. An important variable in such studies is the recording site from which the EEG is taken. The best record- psi information, we have no a priori reason to assume one site ,yore important than another. To make evoked potential studies work for psi messages, explora- tion of a variety of potential sites would seem to be mandatory before the ef- fectiveness of evoked potentials as psi responses can be assessed. Another EEG measure that could be used as an indicator of psi processing is the contingent negative variation (CNV), a negative shift in cortical potential re- corded by surface electrodes from the frontal portion of the brain. Also called the expectancy wave, it is generally regarded as a sign that the organism is im- minently expecting some specific form of stimulation to which it must respond. Levin and Kennedy (1975) employed a reaction-time procedure to see whether or not the presence of a CNV could serve as evidence for anticipation of a yet-to- be determined event. Subjects were told to press a key when a green light ap- peared but not when a red light appeared. Which light appeared was determined by an RNG immediately before the light came on. In a preliminary study, sub- jects' CNV's showed significantly more evidence of expectancy just before the RNG selected green, the color to which the subject was to respond, than before red. A confirmatory study produced chance results, however. This procedure is very important, nevertheless, because the CNV represents a time-locked, precise event in central nervous system information processing. Several other studies have employed psychophysiological measures other than the EEG. Tart's GSR and plethysmograph results have already been mentioned. Dean (1965), using a dream telepathy paradigm, found that active sending on the part of an agent significantly influenced the abundance of rapid eye move- ments during dream periods, even on occasions in which the subject's dream de- scriptions were unrelated to the target. Beloff, Cowles, and Bate (1970) found no evidence that subjects' galvanic skin responses (GSR) were affected by mildly emotionally interesting messages sent by a remote agent. nor did Barron and Mordkoff (1968), Dean (1969), or Sanjar (1969). Rice (1966) found strong GSR deflections in receivers when the agents were exposed to startling stimuli, e.g., sudden immersion of feet in cold water, or hearing a blank cartridge fired. Hettinger (1952) claims that a group of pre- selected sensitives showed increased GSR activity when agents several miles away were stimulated or made to exercise, but does not provide sufficient details. Figar (1959) measured peripheral vasomotor activity with a plethysmograph and found some indication that a receiver's vasomotor activity increased when a remote agent performed mental arithmetic. Unfortunately, no real attempt was made to analyze the data blind, nor was any precise statistical evaluation carried out. Esser, Etter, and Chamberlain (1967) found some indication that receivers' vasomotor activity increased when agents attended to sentences or names of emotional importance to the receivers, as opposed to control sentences, but the authors did not attempt any statistical analysis. Dean (e.g., Dean, 1962, 1969; ing sites are well known for various kinds Of ordinary sensory sApproved'-For Release12060/08/07 : CIAo-unu ~~f ity increased when agents attended to names of emotional importance either to the agent or receiver. Sanjar (1969) found no relationshipl%lap1a1i(t i1FAr Rele vasomotor activity as measured by plethysmograph and agent arousal by loud noises or by being put through a psychiatric interview. These studies are of considerable potential importance and should be pursued in more detail. Of especial interest is the fact that physiological responses were often correlated with the onset of the target event, whereas the cognitively pro- cessed verbal report or behavioral output of the subject was not. Perhaps such indices do give us a more direct access to less processed (and therefore less dis- torted) psi messages. There is not much that can be said in summarizing this material, other than that a few scattered promising beginnings have been made in developing the metho- dology, for relating psi functioning to our present knowledge of biological com- munication. There is some evidence that psi communication is not restricted to humans. Before more specific speculation on the evolution of psi and its eco- logical significance can be seriously considered, we need much more data on more species. We also need to find functional relationships between anpsi strength and other relevant variables such as level of arousal, need strength, and so on to feel comfortable that we are of just dealing with experimenter psi effects. Our knowledge of psi information processing in humans is little better off. There is fairly strong evidence, however. that psi expression does interact with detectable physiological events, some of which may serve as more direct indica- tions of psi than our cognitively elaborated responses. In conclusion, we must greatly expand our data base before we can truly assess the extent to which psi communication interacts with our presently known bio- logical communication channels, either at a cellular or population level or some- where in between. American Society for Psychical Research, Research Committee. Report of the Research Committee for 1958. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 1959,53, 69-71. Backster, C. Evidence of a primary perception in plant life. International Journal of Para- psychology, 1968, 10, 329-348. Barnothy, M. F. (Ed-). Biological Effects of Magnetic Fields. New York: Plenum Press, 1964. Vols. I and 2. Barron, F., and Mordkoff, A. M. An attempt to relate creativity to possible extrasensory empathy as measured by physiological arousal in identical twins. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 1968, 62, 73-79. 4tolbifl b ' r ~hT. Alk IINk~Y&~SID ,5 JQok3 F.JIIOi 70 00Qu1s01ournal of e Parapsychology, 1949,13,166-176. Bel off, J., Cowles, M., and Bate, D. Autonomic reactions to emotive stimuli under sensory and extrasensory conditions of presentation. Journal of the Arerican Society for Psy- chical Research, 1970,64, 313-319. Bestall, C. M. An experiment in precognition in the laboratory mouse. Journal of Parapsy- chology, 1962, 26, 269. Braud, L. W., and Braud, W. G. Further studies of relaxation as a psi-conducive state. Journal of the American Society for Psychical Research, 1974, 68, 229-245. Brenner, D., Williamson, S. J., and Kaufman, L. Visually evoked magnetic fields of the human brain. Science, 1975, 190, 480-482. Broughton, R., and Millar, B. An attempted confirmation of the rodent ESP findings with positive reinforcement. In J. D. Morris. W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Eds.), Research in Parapsychology 1974, pp. 73-75. Metuchen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press, 1,975. Bullock, T., H., and Cowles, R. B. Physiology of an infrared receptor: The facial pit of vipers. Science, 1952, 115, 541-543. Cadoret, R. J. An exploratory experiment: Continuous EEG recording during clairvoyant card tests. Journal of Parapsychology, 1964, 28, 226. Callahan, P. S. Insects tuned in to infrared rays. New Scientist, 1964, 23, 137-138. Christopher, M. Mediums, Mystics and the Occult. New York: Crowell, 1975. Cohen, D. Magnetic fields of the human body. Physics Today, Aug. 1975, 34-4 3. Craig, J. G. The effect of contingency on precognition in the rat. In W. G. Roll, R. L. Morris, and J. D. Morris (Eds.), Research in Parapsychology 1972, pp. 154-156. Metu- chen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press, 1973. Craig, 1. G., and Treurniet, W. Precognition in rats as a function of shock and death. in W. G. Roll, R. L. Morris, and J. D. Morris (Eds.), Research in Parapsychology 1973, pp. 75-78. Metuchen, N.J.: Scarecrow Press, 1974. Dean, E. D. The plethysmograph as an indicator of ESP. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research. 1962, 41, 351-353. Dean, E. D. Proceedings of the Parapsychological Association, 1965, 2, 22-23. Dean, E. D. Long-distance plethysmograph telepathy with agent under water. Proceedings of the Parapsychological Association, 1969, 6, 41-42. Dean, E. D., and Nash, C. B. Coincident plethysmograph results under controlled condi- tions. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 1967, 44, 1-13. Dijgraaf. S. The functioning and significance of the lateral-line organs. Biological Review, 1963,38,51-105. Drbscher, V. The Magic of the Senses. New York: Dutton, 1969. Duane, T., and Behrendt, T. Extrasensory electroencephalographic induction between identical twins. Science, 1965, 150, 367. Duval, P., and Montredon, E. ESP experiments with mice. Journal of Parapsychology, 1968, 32,153-166. (a) Duval, P., and Montredon, E. Further psi experiments with mice. Journal of Parapsy- chology, 1968, 32, 260. (b) Esser, A., Etter, T. L., and Chamberlain, W. B. Preliminary report: Physiological concomi- tants of "communication" between isolated subjects. International Journal of Parapsy- chology,1967,9,53-56. Erickson, R. Personal communication, 1969. Eysenck, H. J. Precognition in rats. Journal of Parapsychology, 1975, 39, 222-227. Figar, S. The application of plethysmography to the objective study of so-called extra- sensory perception. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 1959, 38. 162-171. Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 Approved For Release 2000108107 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100200009-0 (1963) telepathic percipients produced subliminal physiological changes in r sponse to mild electrical stimuli delivered to an agent. Johnson (1974) of the University of Utrecht has studied subliminality in modification of POtzi's tachistoscopic dream experiments. He flashed "threate ing" pictures and measured the degree to which the subject's defensivene against the microtrauma affected his performance in ESP tasks. The results bo! out the hypothesis that a "good" defensive posture was conducive to psi-missim Observations of this order appear to confirm that certain elementary forms r ESP tend to bypass conscious awareness. They are not tied to processing i higher cortical centers and point to subcortical, midbrain, or limbic regions i their substrate. Experimental psychologists described sensory perceptions c this order as subceptions. Eysenck (1961), Burt (1968), Beloff (1974), an others have specifically pointed to their relationship to perceptual defense! while Dixon (1971) and Beloff emphasized the close relationship of both p phenomena and subliminal perception to the reticular formation and the bra: stem. Eysenck (1967) has noted, furthermore, that a high state of arousal c the cortex mediated by the ascending reticular formation is antagonistic to psi. The EEG is particularly suited to telepathic experiments of this kind. Duan and Behrendt (1965) succeeded in inducing electroencephalographic changes identical twins, bypassing conscious perception. Their observations have n: been replicated by others. Earlier attempts by Ehrenwald, Kahn, and Ullmr; along the same lines were abandoned owing to inadequacies of the available it strumentation. In their article in Nature, Targ and Puthoff (1974) found the systematic alteration of ongoing electric activity can be elicited by a remou photic stimulus. Needless to say that the exact determination of the sources of the EEG activit of the brain is still problematical. By and large, the occipital lobes have bee; identified as the sources of origin of alpha rhythm in certain altered -states -,I consciousness. EEG data concerning lateralization of visually evoked potentic ? have been described by neurophysiologists, but no conclusive parapsychologict % findings have as yet been reported. . A striking series of EEG experiments has been reported by Lloyd (1973). Fk work has been sponsored by the New Horizons Research Foundation in Toronte The Lloyd series is aimed at duplicating, by telepathic induction, the "averag evoked EEG response" elicited by an actual auditory stimulus. It is claimed tha the results indicate a direct brain response to a telepathic message, although Lloyi leaves open the question whether PK may have directly affected the monitorial: equipment. We are told that T. H. Lloyd is a pseudonym, but the Editor ofiVer Horizons testifies to the good faith and scientific qualifications of the author Attempts at replicating the "Lloyd effect" elsewhere were unsuccessful. The vast literature dealing with the relationship of ESP and alpha activity, am its reinforcement by diverse biofeedback methods, is described in other sectiou? of this Handbook. as-so ates also noted that hamsters and gerbils were unresponsive to similar use of OB periments. The ex naive literature of psi trailing in cats and dogs, though likewise sug- gestive of p ', is still amenable to rival sensory, in lieu of extrasensory, interpre- tations. So 1 the homing behavior of salmon, pigeons, cats, and dogs. More persuasive are e older observations on dogs, reported by Bechterev (1949) and the more rece findings by White (1964). Wood and Cadoret (1958) tested clairvoyance in a man og relationship by adapting the ESP card-calling technique to a dog called Chris. 1ey found evidence of ESP in the presence of the owner. These few representati samples of animal psi should suffice in the present context. They indicate tha despite the many gaps in the available evidence, the occurrence of psi from the to linet unicellular organisms to higher vertebrates, and especially rodents, cats, an ogs, can be taken for granted. It also appears that the observable psi responses to to increase in complexity as we move from organisms with primitive neural struc res to organisms with more highly differ- entiated central nervous systems. It sho -ld be noted at this point, however, that psi phenomena must not be lumped togeth r in one single undifferentiated set of organismic functions-afferent or efferent, ESP, PK, and precognitive-as the case may be. Generically they may amount t the same thing, ranging over the whole spectrum of organic nature from plants to an. But we must realize that PK responses of paramecia differ from those of a co roach, a hamster, or a dog, much in the same way as an apple and an applesee differ from a full-grown specimen of an apple tree. By and large, evidence of psi etermined behavior in lower animals indicates that it may occur under the auspic of what McLean de- scribed as the archaic reptilian brain. In turn, the growl incidence of psi phenomena associated with the evolutionary increase of cortic tructures would point to the part played by so-called uncommitted brain regions nfield,1975) in the origin of psi functions of a higher order. Yet it is needless to ay that the data of animal psi do not permit a fixed cerebral localization in any ecialized brain region. Further insight into the problem has to be sought by tur g our attention to additional neurophysiological and clinical observations in hum s. PHYSIOLOGICAL VARIABLES It has always been suspected that a vast number of psi incidents, spontaneous and experimental, are taking place below the threshhold of conscious awareness. They are subliminal, preconscious, or unconscious and can only be gleaned from monitoring such variables as blood pressure, pulse rate, electromyogram, EEG, and other indices. A typical example is Dean's (1962) plethysmographic experiments in which an emotionally charged stimulus word sent by an agent elicited measurable changes in the blood supply in the finger of a percipient. In a comparable series, Tart's Approved For Releaser 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP9'";-'6O " ' ' l'cO1dd66''6" 72 The Brauds (1975) have focused attention on the right versus the left hemi- sphere, and their relevance to facilitating psi functions. Their hypothesis that inducing mental states favoring right hemispheric dominance would be psi pro- ductive was not borne out. But the expected induction of left hemispheric dominance did lead to significant psi-missing. Andrew (1975) applied the same procedure in reference to "active" psi, that is, psychokinesis. In this series, the right hemisphere group scored significantly above chance, and the left group showed an extrachance tendency to psi-missing. Results of assessing the activity of the two hemispheres during bilateral EEG recordings are not yet available. Whitten (1974) studied the EEG record of a gifted subject, Matthew Manning, while attempting to perform specified psycho. kinetic tasks. In this case, a distinct wave pattern, described as the ramp function, made its appearance. The investigations of Broughton (1976). of the University of Edinburgh, focus- ing on brain hemisphere specialization, were based on the same rationale. Using an ingenious experimental paradigm to separate the two hemispheres in an ESP test, he found that improved performance could be elicited when the right hemi- sphere responses were encouraged while at the same time loading an additional conceptual task on the left hemisphere. He concludes that the right hemisphere of the brain is better at a receptive type of ESP. We shall see that more light on the presumed lateralization of psi phenomena can be shed by clinical observations. In hu, an subjects we have to rely on the occurrence of organic pathology as a substitut or surgical removal or excision techniques used in animal experiments. The frequen cited instance of the dying serving as telepathic percipients or agents can be to n as the closest approximation to states of clinical "decerebra- tion" or organic ", ,nus function" (Ehrenwaid, 1948) in which higher cortical functions are in abeya -e. Similar considerations apply to the death defying feats of some out-of?bo subjects (E' renwald, 1974). Telepathy in sleep, dreams, and the REM state ( man and Krippner, with Vaughan, 19-113) points in the same direction. So do th ountless reports of telepathy in trance condi- tions, in absent-mindedness, and in rse altered states of consciousness. The case of alga K., published by von N reiter (1935), illustrates another facet of the problem. Ilga was a mentally defer e girl of 9 with a severe reading dis- ability. The samples of her handwriting, her eras demeanor, and her photo- graph suggest the diagnosis of mental deficiency. Yet von Neureiter reported that the child could "read" any text if and when he other, seated in another room, was reading the same text silently to herself. l1 a case has been subject td some controversy. Yet the crucial point is that in this ' stance it was her in ing that was responsible for Ilga's telepathic ability. Another point to bear ind is that her "minus function" consisted of a specific deficit on a higher ve level. It was of the nature of an alexia-that is, a cortical deficiency as cogni describ impairm fective bo (1968), in by Ikjerine. Potz1 and others-and was riot due to a peripheral sensory t. A similar picture was described by Drake (1938) in a mentally de- little Bo, and, more recently, by Recordon, Stratton, and Peters and. The latter case is known as that of the Cambridge Boy. Both Little Bo and were telepathica e Cambridge Boy suffered from congenital spastic paralysis and responsive to their mothers only. school psychologist Eloise Shields (1976) has investigated the The Los Angele psi abilities of 25 m defective children, ag against chance under clairvoyant conditions w -21. They yielded an astronomical score of P =.00000057 iepathic conditions. Their performance scores only slightly above chance. Another significant exp of ESP tests of 18 hospitali imental contribution is Schmeidler's (1952) series clinical picture in such cases i d patients suffering from cerebral concussion. The too ill-defined to permit any neurological diagno- sis beyond the conjecture that brain syndrome with some degree amounts to a combination of a diffuse organic psychological overlay. Using a control group without cerebral injury, Schmeidler found a of 11 hospitalized fracture patient' significantly higher scoring pattern (P In such cases it can be argued that .002) in the concussion group. postulated organic brain damage served ing factors for the emergence of psi as one of the predisposing or conditid phenomena. Yet it should also be noted ESP responses of the card-calling type wit hat lumping together forced-choice ESP responses focusing on major relate the two with a supposedly targets of higher complexity, and trying to cd all-encompassing functional deficit, "minus fun of consciousness, is apt to miss the point. We sh semblance of order into the conflicting findings th face between psi phenomena and neuropathology, 1 presently see that to bring a have accrued on the inter- will have to think up a function is supposed to new twist to the conventional question of how a giver' be related to a specific brain region. The new twist proposed here is to rephrase-or turn arour' about the hypothetical cerebral localization of psi phenorne? Bering what neural structures or brain region is responsible some of the time, we have to ask: What is it that prevents th Instead of won- their operation organism from uli all of the being flooded by the influx of both sensory and extrasensory sti time? By the same token, we have to ask: What is it that puts the organism's motor or psychomotor organization, that stops it from over-reacting) to external stimulation, that stops it from exhausting it cting (or store of sm of psychomotor energy in the process-and from spending itself in a parox Approved For Relbase 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100200009-0 goioid,25 brain-damaged, and 25 undifferentiated mentally ion," or assorted altered states rake on the