PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, THE PERSISTENT PARADOX OF PSYCHIC PHENOMENA: AN ENGINEERING PERSPECTIVE
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
U
Document Page Count:
37
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
June 18, 1998
Sequence Number:
4
Case Number:
Publication Date:
February 1, 1982
Content Type:
RP
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6.pdf | 5.28 MB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
February 1982 Vol. 70 No. 2
NE
Scanning the Issue
Adjustable Speed AC Drives-A Technology Status Review
(Invited Paper), Bose, page 116-After nearly a decade and a
half of intensive development, adjustable speed ac drives have
assumed a position of great importance in a large variety of
industrial applications. This has led to a pressing need in the
industrial and academic communities for an understanding of
the basics and state of the art of this growing technology. This
invited paper, believed to be the first of its kind on this subject,
is a tutorial-review of ac drives intended to be useful both to
the reader who has a general interest in industrial control using
ac drives and to the specialist who wishes to extend and expand
his or her knowledge.
The ac machines which constitute the principal subsystem of
these drives are first concisely but thoroughly reviewed in the
paper. Following this, the technical features of different classes
of drives with converters, such as voltage-fed converters, cur-
rent-fed converters, cycloconverters, static Kramer, and static
Scherbius drives, are discussed. The paper then describes
modeling and simulation of ac drives, and finally the subject
of microcomputer control is reviewed in some detail. Through-
out the paper, the current trends of development of power
semiconductor devices, machines, converters, and controls are
indicated wherever possible.
The Persistent Paradox of Psychic Phenomena: An Engineering
Perspective (Invited Paper), Jahn, page 136-The persistent,
fragmentary indication of the possible existence of psychic
phenomena requires us, in the interests of scientific integrity
and intellectual honesty, to apply the same spirit of objective
inquiry that we have focused on the more traditional and
accepted branches of science and technology.
It was nearly six years ago-in March 1976, to be exact-that
the PROCEEDINGS took the unusual step of publishing a paper
on the psychic phenomenon called remote viewing. That paper
was received with great interest, considerable skepticism, some
hostility, and even a bit of shock. In the intervening years, no
breakthroughs have occurred to prove or disprove the existence
of this and other psychic phenomena, but interest and research
have continued unabated. Where does research on these topics
n
stand now? It was with this question in mind that the paper in
this issue was invited.
This paper reviews the history, nomenclature, conceptual
organization, and status of the generic field of psychic phe-
nomena; presents a few detailed examples of contemporary
research that could have ultimate technological implications;
and attempts to offer a balanced view of the viability and value
of continued study of this fascinating and controversial field.
The paper is believed to represent the first attempt at a
comprehensive engineering survey of the field. Most readers
will likely approach it with a healthy blend of curiosity and
skepticism. It is hoped all will find it informative and thought
provoking.
Performance Evaluation of Data Communication Systems
(Invited Paper), Reiser, page 171-Great strides have been
taken in recent years in developing the architecture of message
or packet-based communication networks. Much has been
learned about organizing, controlling, and operating such net-
works, but performance and design principles were developed
in parallel, and networks were built before a complete under-
standing of their performance was achieved. Major progress
has been made in evaluating the performance of such commu-
nication systems, and this invited paper presents a tutorial
survey of the state of the art of this field. The majority of the
literature on performance evaluation emphasizes protocol
evaluation, and the author systematically develops this ap-
proach.
Following a general introduction to performance evaluation,
the paper gives considerable attention to the mathematical
tools involved, using the language of the communication engi-
neer. The major mathematical techniques are Markov chains
applied to discrete time systems and queueing theory. Through-
out emphasis is given to how these tools are applied in practice.
The mathematical methods do not map on a one-to-one basis
to the system, and the leading modeling ideas are highlighted.
The embedding environments in which protocols are analyzed
are discussed in some detail, and the paper follows the frame-
work of layered architecture. Separate sections are devoted
to the performance analysis of data-link protocols, path-con-
trol protocols, and end-to-end protocols.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
1196 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
11321 M. Schwartz, "Performance analysis of the SNA virtual route
pacing control," IBM Res. Rep. RC 8490, Thomas J. Watson
Research Center, Yorktown Heights, NY, Sept. 1980.
(133] F. Tobagi and L. Kleinrock, "Packet switching in radio channels:
Part IV-Stability considerations and dynamic control in carrier
sense multiple access," IEEE Trans. Commun., vol. COM-25,
pp. 1103-1120, Oct. 1977.
1134] F. A. Tobagi, M. Gerla, R. W. Peebles, and E. G. Manning,
"Modeling and measurement techniques in packet communica-
tion networks," Proc. IEEE, vol. 66, no. 11, pp. 1423-1447,
Nov. 1978.
11351 F. A. Tobagi and V. B. Hunt, "Performance analysis of carrier
sense multiple access with collision detection," Comput. Net.,
vol. 4, pp. 245-259, 1980.
(1361 V. L. Wallace, "Algebraic techniques for the numerical solution
of queueing networks," in Mathematical Methods in Queuing
Theory, A. B. Clarke, Ed. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 1974,
pp. 295-305.
Contributors
Bimal K. Bose (S'59-M'60-SM'78) received the
B.E. degree from Calcutta University, Calcutta,
India, in 1956, the M.S. degree from the Uni-
versity of Wisconsin, Madison, in 1960, and the
Ph.D. degree from Calcutta University, in 1966.
He was a Member of Faculty at Calcutta Uni-
versity where he was awarded the Premchand
Roychand scholarship and the Mouat gold
medal for research contributions. In 1971, he
joined Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, Troy,
NY, as an Associate Professor of Electrical
Engineering. He was responsible for organizing undergraduate and
graduate power electronics programs, including an advanced course on
adjustable speed ac drives. He was a consultant for several industries
including General Electric R&D Center, Bendix Corporation, Lutron
Electronics, and PCI Ozone Corporation. Since 1976 he has been with
General Electric, Corporate Research and Development, Schenectady,
NY. His research interests are power conversion systems and micro-
computer-based performance optimization of ac drives.
Dr. Bose has published 42 research papers and holds a number of U.S.
patents. Recently, he edited the IEEE press book Adjustable Speed AC
Drives. He is Chairman of IEEE Transactions Review of Static Power
Converter Committee, a member of the Power Electronics and Micro-
computer Control Committee, the IEEE Industrial Electronics and
Control Instrumentation Group, the Scientific Committee of the Inter-
national Conference on Digital Control of Electrical Machine, and of
the Program Committee of the International Static Power Converter
Conference, a member of the Steering Committee for the Workshop on
Automotive Applications of Microprocessors, a member of the Fellow
Nominating Committee for the IEEE Static Power Converter Committee,
and a member of the Program Committee of the Tokyo International
Power Electronics Conference.
Robert G. Jahn received the B.S. degree in
mechanical engineering in 1951, the M.A. de-
gree in physics in 1953, and the Ph.D. degree in
physics in 1955, all from Princeton University,
Princeton, NJ.
He has taught at Lehigh University, Bethle-
hem, PA, at the California Institute of Tech-
nology, Pasadena, and, since 1962, at Princeton
University, where he is Dean of the School of
Engineering and Applied Science and also serves
as Professor of Aerospace Sciences. His principal
(137] C. H. West, "General technique for communications protocol
validation," IBM J. Res. Devel., vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 393-404,
July 1978.
(138] J. W. Wong and M. S. Unsoy, "Analysis of flow control in
switched data networks," in Proc. IFIP Conf. on Information
Processing. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland, 1977,
pp.315-320.
[139] J. W. Wong, "Distribution of end-to-end delay in message switched
networks," Comput. Net., vol. 2, pp. 4 -49,1978
[140] J. W. Wong, J.A.B. Moura, and J. A. Field, "Hierarchical model-
ing of local area computer networks," In Proc. Nat. Telecom-
mun. Conf. '80 (Houston, TX, 1980).
1141] J. W. Wong and S. S. Lam, "Queueing network models of packet-
switching networks, Part 1: Open networks," Performance
Evaluation, to be published.
1142) P. Zaflropulo, C. H. West, H. Rudin, D. D. Cowan, and D. Brand,
"Towards analyzing and synthesizing protocols," IEEE Trans.
Commun., vol. COM-28, pp. 651-66 1, Apr. 1980.
areas of scholarship include high temperature gasdynamics, ionized gas
kinetics, plasmadynamics and plasma propulsion, and his book, The
Physics of Electric Propulsion remains the standard text in its field. His
interest in psychic phenomena traces to his supervision of a student
project begun in 1977, and although his present program in this area
remains a very minor part of his professional activity, it has achieved
international prominence in the field.
Dr. Jahn received the Curtis W. McGraw Research Award of the ASEE,
and he is a Fellow of both the American Physical Society and the
American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics. He has served on
several NASA Advisory Committees, and is a member and past-chairman
of the Board of Trustees of Associated Universities, Inc.
Martin Reiser was born in Switzerland in 1943.
He received his education from the Swiss
Federal Institute of Technology (ETH) where
he obtained the Ph.D. degree in 1971.
One year later, he switched from the IBM
Zurich Research Laboratory, Riischlikon, Swit-
zerland, to the IBM Thomas J. Watson Research
Center, Yorktown Heights, NY, where he
joined the Performance Evaluation group. He
specialized in the numerical solution of queueing
networks, and made contributions to the theory
of multichain networks and their numerical solution. Together with
J. Buzen and H. Kobayashi, he laid the foundation for today's fast
algorithms in queueing networks and created one of the first modeling
software packages, known as QNET 4. He is author or co-author of
numerous publications in the field. After spending about 1 year at the
IBM Research Laboratory in San Jose, CA, he returned to Yorktown
Heights where he was soon appointed Manager of Performance Evalua-
tion. He led efforts in analysis, hierarchical modeling, and system
measurement. In late 1977, he started a new group in the area of
Computer Communication Network Modeling and Architecture. This
activity led to a new assignment in 1978 as Senior Manager of Com-
munications and Computer Science back at the IBM Zurich Research
Laboratory, a position he still holds. His latest technical interests are in
mean-value analysis, a theory he co-founded with S. S. Lavenberg and
which he applied to computer networks.
Dr. Reiser is a member of ACM, IFIP WG.7.3, and IFIP WG 6.4.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
136 PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
The Persistent Paradox of Psychic Phenomena:
An Engineering Perspective
ROBERT G. JAHN
Invited Paper
Abstract-Although a variety of so-called psychic phenomena have at-
tracted man's attention throughout recorded history, organized scholarly
effort to comprehend such effects is just one century old, and systematic
academic research roughly half that age. Over recent years, a sizeable
spectrum of evidence has been brought forth from reputable laboratories
in several disciplines to suggest that at times human consciousness can
acquire information inaccessible by any known physical mechanism
(ESP), and can influence the behavior of physical systems or processes
(PK), but even the most rigorous and sophisticated of these studies
display a characteristic dilemma: The experimental results are rarely rep-
licable in the strict scientific sense, but the anomalous yields are well
beyond chance expectations and a number of common features thread
through the broad range of reported effects. Various attempts at theoret-
ical modeling have so far shown little functional value in explicating
experimental results, but have served to stimulate fundamental re-exam-
ination of the role of consciousness in the determination of physical
reality. Further careful study of this formidable field seems justified,
but only within the context of very well conceived and technically im-
peccable experiments of Inge data-base capability, with disciplined at-
tention to the pertinent aesthetic factors, and with more constructive
involvement of the critical community.
Prologue
The world of psychic phenomena might be likened to a vast,
fog-shrouded swamp, wherein are reported to dwell a bewilder-
ing array of bizarre phenomenological creatures, all foreign to
our normal perceptual and analytical catalogs. Some scholars
who have explored this clouded domain have returned to an-
nounce categorically that all such life is illusory-mere sunken
stumps and swirling subsurface shadows, inviting misperception
by the gullible and misrepresentation by the purveyors. But
others of comparable conviction have described in minute de-
tail their observations of a variety of extraordinary beings of
awesome dimensions and capability. Some of these are claimed
to appear unexpectedly, erupting from the roily depths to
flash momentarily in the sunlight of human experience, only
to disappear again before any systematic calibration of their
characteristics can be taken. Others are reportedly enticed to
more replicable and controlled behavior, but only by persons
of special talent or extensive training. Much invalid, even
fraudulent evidence of such activity has been touted by ex-
ploiters of these mysteries, thereby casting deep suspicion on
all other testimony. When fully sifted, only a very few legiti-
Manuscript received July 15, 1981; revised October 26, 1981. This
work was supported in part by the McDonnell Foundation, Inc., and
by the John E. Fetzer Foundation, Inc., The Explorers Club, the Insti-
tute of Noetic Sciences, and the Little River Foundation.
The author is Dean of the School of Engineering/Applied Science,
Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544.
mate specimens seem to have been captured, by tediously de-
liberate trolling of the brackish domain, or by more incisive
invasion of its turbid interior, and even these have proven so
incomprehensible and so delicate to exposure, and the imposed
criteria for their credibility have been so severe, that they have
not been fully persuasive. Yet the goal remains alluring, and
the search continues.
INTRODUCTION
ITH THIS unlikely bit of allegorical musing, I venture
to begin the most extraordinary writing task I have yet
attempted: to respond to the request of the Editors
of this journal for a critical review of the status and prognosis
of scientific research into so-called psychic phenomena. I do
so with some trepidation, first because the topic is far from
my principal line of scholarship and my involvement with it
has been brief and tightly circumscribed, and second, because
of the intensity of reactions any commentary on this subject
tends to call forth from many quarters.
For these reasons, it may be well at the outset to specify my
perspective on the field and the purpose that I hope this article
will serve. My formal training is that of an engineer and applied
physicist, and the bulk of my research has concerned a sequence
of topics in the broad domain of the aerospace sciences: Fluid
mechanics, ionized gases, plasmadynamics, and electric propul-
sion. In my present position as Dean of the School of Engineer-
ing and Applied Science of Princeton University, I have occa-
sion to be involved with an even broader selection of topics
selected for undergraduate independent projects, and it was
in that context some four years ago that I was requested by
one. of our very best students to supervise a study of psychic
phenomena. More specifically, this young lady proposed to
bring her talents and background in electrical engineering and
computer science to bear on some experiments in controlled,
low-level psychokinesis. Although I had no previous experience,
professional or personal, with this subject, for a variety of
pedagogical reasons I agreed, and together we mapped a tenta-
tive scholarly path, involving a literature search, visits to appro-
priate laboratories and professional meetings, and the design,
construction, and operation of simple experiments. My initial
oversight role in this project led to a degree of personal involve-
ment with it, and that to a growing intellectual bemusement,
to the extent that by the time this student graduated, I was
persuaded that this was a legitimate field for a high technologist
to study and that I would enjoy continuing to do so.
Approved For Release 2000/0$/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
de-
sive
i so
>sed
!ave
and
Lure
yet
tors
osis
do
'om
h it
use
ject
my
icle
fled
nce
uid
)ul-
,er-
ca-
)ics
was
by
chic
to
rnd
ed,
ice,
of
ita-
'ro-
gn,
tial
ive-
+nt,
was
.fist
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
JAHN:rPSY IC PHENOMENA: ENGINEERING PERSPECTIVE
I have since assembled a small professional staff, secured the
requisite funding from a few private sources, and undertaken
a modest experimental program in selected aspects of the field
that could ultimately have some engineering implications. I
should emphasize that my fractional involvement with this
program remains quite minor in comparison to my other re-
sponsibilities, and that the work is still very preliminary and
tentative, but it provides the base of cognizance for my broader
observations on the field as a whole.
The intention of this article is to provide some balanced per-
spective on the modem status of this conceptually and logis-
tically difficult subject. Certainly no field of scholarly endeavor
has proven more frustrating; nor has been more abused and
misunderstood, than the study of psychic phenomena. Dealing
as it does as much with impressionistic and aesthetic evidence
as with analytical substance, and carrying by its nature strongly
subjective and numenistic overtones, it has been incessantly
prostituted by charlatans, lunatics, and sensationalists, catego-
rically rejected by most of the scientific establishment, and
widely misunderstood by the public at large. Interspersed
with this, and greatly encumbered by it, a pattern of legitimate
effort to comprehend and utilize the purported phenomena
has evolved to a point where some dispassionate assessment of
its accomplishments can be attempted. The questions addressed
by this review are whether, once the overburdens of illegitimate
activity and irresponsible criticism are removed, there remains
sufficient residue of valid evidence to justify continued research
and, if so, how this research might most effectively be styled,
facilitated, and evaluated.
Before addressing these issues directly, it may be helpful to
review briefly the historical evolution of the field, its contem-
porary nomenclature and conceptual organization, and the
dimensions of current activity. This can then be followed by
a general overview and critique of the modem research, and
that in turn by more detailed description of a few specific
efforts, drawn primarily from our own work. Toward the
close, we shall attempt to survey several theoretical approaches
to modeling of psychic processes and comment briefly on po-
tential implications and applications of the phenomena. In all
of this, no tone of advocacy is intended, other than for objec-
tive assessment of the evidence in hand.
HISTORY
In a sense, the study of psychic phenomena is one of the
oldest of human endeavors [11-M. As far back as can be
traced, mortal man has pondered the supernatural in one form
or another. Cave drawings at Lascaux and Altamira, circa
20 000 B.C., reflect this preoccupation, and the religious
rites of early societies of both the eastern and western worlds
were heavily loaded with psychic formalisms. The classic
civilizations of Egypt, Greece, and Rome dealt extensively in
psychic process. The Delphic Oracle was politically important
from the earliest Hellenic times to the age of Alexander the
Great, and was consulted on problems as diverse as the proper
measures to stop a plague, the constitutions of Greek city-states,
and the best locations for new colonies. Even Aristotle, one of
the most empirical of the classical philosophers, examined the
causal links in prophetic dreams.
Virtually every form of organized religion practiced by man
has been thoroughly laced with various forms of psychic mech-,
anism. The Bible, like most other basic theological texts, treats
Psychic process as a central ingredient, in a tone so matter-of-
fact that one is inclined to believe. that people of those times
accepted such events rather routinely. Indeed, the Bible is an
excellent catalog of psychic phenomena; virtually every cate-
gory of effect identified today is illustrated there in one form
or another.
Christian writers and philosophers, from Augustine to the
Reformation, recount many purported instances of psychic
phenomena, usually attributed to visitations of divine grace
or demonic posession. Secular medieval writing also abounds
with supernatural and mystical reference, and even in the
Renaissance period it is still difficult to separate psychic allu-
sion from religious dogma, although both were then translated
into more organized forms in art and literature. Early in the
16th century the celebrated Swiss physician and philosopher
Paracelsus wrote extensively on psychic capabilities and po-
tentialities. In his words:
The mind of man is the microcosmic counterpart of the uni-
versal mind ..... ...One man may communicate his thoughts
to another with whom he is in sympathy, at any distance however
great it may be, or he may act upon the spirit of another person
in such a manner as to influence his actions .... [8].
Perhaps the first major scientific commentaries on the topic
were offered near the turn of the 17th century by Sir Francis Ba-
con, widely regarded as the originator of the scientific method.
In The Advancement of Learning he suggested that "supersti-
tions and the like" should not be excluded from scientific
study, and in his posthumous book, Sylva Sylvarum, he
proposed deliberate investigation of telepathic dreams, psychic
healing, and the influence of "imagination" on the casting of
dice [9). Some years later, a group of British intellectuals
including Henry More and Joseph Glanvill met regularly to
discuss paranormal topics, and in 1681 Glanvill published
the substance of these studies in a book entitled Saducismus
Triumphatus [10].
Meanwhile, some four centuries of public and church hysteria
over sorcery and witchcraft, as manifested in a sequence of
trials, tortures, and executions, had begun to subside, and by
the mid-18th century, the Roman Church authorized Prospero
Lambertini, who later became Pope Benedict XIV, to carry
out a scholarly investigation of reports of psychic events. His
conclusions, recorded in De Canonizatione [ 11 ], were surpris-
ingly unecclesiastical: namely, that 1) psychic experiences
were not necessarily divine miracles, but could occur to "fools,
idiots, melancholy persons, and brute beasts"; 2) apparitions
had little to do with sanctity or demonic entities; 3) prophesy
occurs more often in sleep than in waking; 4) it is difficult for
a prophet to distinguish his own thoughts from extrasensory
messages; and 5) predictions frequently take. symbolic forms.
In all of these, he presaged to some degree modern thoughts
on these topics.
At roughly the same time, Anton Mesmer's discovery of hyp-
nosis opened an alternative route to demonstration and study
of unconscious psychic process that has continued to this day.
Early reports of hypnotized subjects performing telepathic or
clairvoyant tasks were common [ 12] [ 13 1, and although much
of this evidence might now be discounted on the basis of in-
adequate experimental control, interest in hypnosis specifically}
and in various altered states of consciousness generally, as
facilitators of psychic experience persists into some of the
modern experimentation.
Also in this mid-18th century period, a spiritualist move-
ment focused on extrasensory contact with the dead, possibly
influenced by the work of Emanuel Swedenborg [ 14 J, [IS],
germinated in this country as well as in England, and by the
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
138 - PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
19th century had reached the dimensions of an organized
religion. Symbolic of the popular preoccupation with the-topic,
Mary Todd Lincoln was-reported to have held seances in the
White House in the early 1860's [ 161 . A classic two-volume
work by F.W.H. Myers, entitled Human Personality and the
Survival of Bodily Death 117), brought the topic to its acme
of sophistication, but eventually the fanaticism the movement
attracted and its fraudulent exploitation created, a negative
attitude in the scholarly community which prevails yet today.
Despite these millennia of human concern with the paranor-
mal, orderly and organized scholarly search for verification and
understanding of psychic phenomena began only a century
ago, with the establishment in London in 1882 of the Society
for Psychical Research, in whose Proceedings appeared the first
formal publication of controlled experiments in telepathy and
clairvoyance [ 131, [181, [ 191. Three years later the counter-
part organization in this country, the American Society for
Psychical Research, was founded in Boston by several distin-
guished scientists and philosophers. Because of financial
difficulty, this shortly merged with the British group, but re-
emerged in 1905 as a separate entity with its own professional
journal, and has continued as such to the present 1201.
Although the SPR attracted a barrage of criticism from the
scientific and intellectual communities, it also attracted
significant participation of eminent scholars from established
fields. Numbered among its presidents are three Nobel Lau-
reates, ten Fellows of the Royal Society, one Prime Minister,
and a substantial list of physicists and philosophers, including
Henry Sidgwick, Frederic W. H. Myers, Lord Rayleigh, Sir J. J.
Thomson, William McDougall, Edmund Gurney, Sir William
Crookes, Sir William Barrett, Henri Bergson, Arthur, Earl of
Balfour, Gardner Murphy, G.N.M. Tyrell, Charles Richet,
Gilbert Murray, and one of the most articulate contributors to
the evolution of critical thought on this topic in this period,
the Harvard psychologist and philosopher, William James.
One of the founders of the ASPR, James wrote extensively
and eloquently on behalf of objective and disciplined study of
psychic phenomena [211-[251:
Any one with a healthy sense for evidence, a sense not methodi-
cally blunted by the sectarianism of `science,' ought now, it
seems to me, to feel that exalted sensibilities and memories,
veridical phantasms, haunted houses, trances with supernormal
faculty, and even experimental thought-transference, are natural
kinds of (phenomena) which ought, just like other natural events,
to be followed up with scientific curiosity [ 25 1.
Entering the 20th century, a new perspective on psychic phe-
nomena was provided by the emergence of psychology .as a
scholarly discipline, and especially by the early efforts in clinical
psychology and psychoanalytic therapy. The patriarch of this
evolution, Sigmund Freud, was a member of the SPR and con-
tributed, albeit somewhat reluctantly, to its publications [261,
[271. His recognition and exploration of the unconscious
mind and of the function of dreams prompted Myers to suggest
a possible explication of various psychic effects which is still
of theoretical value [ 17 1. Freud's interest in parapsychology
increased toward the end of his life, and he is reported to have
conceded informally that were he to begin his career anew, he
would focus on this topic.
Freud's former protege, Carl Jung, who had written his Ph.D.
thesis on the psychology of occult phenomena, pursued ex-
ploration of the unconscious to deeper dimensions of para-
normal experience, publishing widely on such subjects as telep-
athy, mediumship, synchronicity, the collective unconscious,
and theoretical models of psychic process. [281-[30]. In
Memories, Dreams, Reflections, he stated:
. the relationship between doctor and patient, especially
when a transference on the part of the patient occurs, or a more
or less unconscious identification of doctor and patient, can lead
to parapsychological phenomena. I have frequently run into this
[30].
Jung's collaboration with the eminent physicist Wolfgang Pauli
on the topic of synchronicity clearly influenced the subsequent
evolution of both careers and of fundamental concepts in both
disciplines [ 311. Although much of the established psychologi-
cal community has since rejected parapsychology as a valid
discipline, some interest has been retained by a few clinical
practitioners, presumably because of the demonstrated con-
comitance and similarities of apparent psychic experiences
with certain psychological processes [ 32 ], [ 331.
It was also early in this century that the first organized
academic studies of psychic phenomena were mounted. One
of the more visible of these devolved from gifts and a bequest
from Thomas W. Stanford, brother of the founder of Stanford
University, to endow psychic research at that institution, and
to this day the university provides support of a "psychic research
fellow" and retains a collection of so-called "apports" indicative
of the donor's long personal involvement with the field. Modest
research programs were also undertaken at Harvard and a few
European universities in the first decades of this century, as
evidenced by occasional publications in various established
journals.
The benchmark academic effort, however, germinated at
Duke University in the late 1920's, when William McDougall,
who had been James' successor at Harvard, arrived to chair the
department of psychology and appointed J. B. Rhine and
Louisa Rhine, "to study the claims to scientific value of the
field known as psychical research." Their early tentative
efforts in the study of postmortem survival gradually evolved
into a laboratory for controlled research in "extrasensory per-
ception," as they first termed the process. In this laboratory
were established many of the basic concepts and protocols of
modem psychic research, as well as the first extensive and sys-
tematic data bases of several types of psychic experimentation.
The professional and personal history of the Rhines and their
laboratory is a fascinating saga in its own right, but would take
us too far afield here 1341-[371. A few excerpts from a 1967
address of J. B. Rhine to the American Psychological Associa-
tion, in which he attempted to summarize his first two decades
of intensive study, give hint of the inherent attractions and
frustrations of this field, and of the man's optimistic vision:
The phenomena that were being studied began to show lawful
interrelations and even a degree of unity. One by one the major
claims, based originally only upon spontaneous human experi-
ences, were subjected to laboratory test and experimentally veri-
fied.... Certain general characteristics of the psi process became
clear during this period. The most revealing of these is the sub-
ject's lack of conscious control over any type of psi ability, a
characteristic which accounts for its elusive nature. It was new
methodological ground, even for psychology.... Also, we were
surprised to find that psi ability is widespread, probably even a
specific human capacity rather than a capability possessed by a
few rare individuals as had been the popular belief. Evidence
that psi is not linked with illness or abnormality was another
welcome advance.... By 1951... a healthy young science was
emerging [381.
In 1937, the Rhines began publication of the Journal of
Parapsychology, which remains a leading journal in the field
today. A professional organization calling itself the Parapsy
chological Association was formed in 1957, and in 1969 was
. Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
JAHN: PSYCHIC Pro phi' ?#vfOrE~ &rQ$6/08/07: CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6139
accepted as an affiliate by the American Association for the
Advancement of Science.
At the present time, there are eight English language publica-
tions covering this field [39], supplemented by numerous less
formal magazines and countless books of widely varying quality
and relevance. Research activity is reported from some twenty
U.S. universities and colleges and at least as many institutions
in Western Europe [40], but in most cases it is of very small
scale. There are very few academic programs of study, although
some fifty M.A. and Ph.D. theses have been accepted on psychic
topics at reputable universities over the past forty years [41 ] .
Some ten research institutes and private corporations in the
United States have also authorized publications and reports in
the field [42] . The extent of Eastern Bloc and Oriental efforts
[43]-[541 and of classified research in this country are matters
of considerable speculation on which I cannot comment with
authority.
Further review of contemporary programs will be attempted
in subsequent sections, following an outline of modern nomen-
clature and conceptual organization of the topic. In closing this
historical overview, we might simply observe that in many re-
spects the growth pattern of this field resembles that of the
natural sciences in their earliest days, or perhaps even more
the incubation of classical psychology, in terms of the ab-
sence of replicable basic experiments and useful theoretical
models, the low level of financial support and internal profes-
sional coordination, and the low credibility in the academic
establishment and public sectors. Also like those fields, the
survival and early growth of psychic research can largely be
attributed to the efforts of a few scholars of sufficient convic-
tion, stature, and courage to withstand the rejection of the
orthodox communities.
NOMENCLATURE AND CONCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION
Before turning to an assessment of contemporary research,
it may be useful to specify some notation and delineation of
the field, to an extent consistent with the present limited
comprehension of the phenomena. First, let us agree to a
global definition of "psychic phenomena" (frequently denoted
by "psi" or "0") to include all processes of information and/or
energy exchange which involve animate consciousness in a
manner not currently explicable in terms of known science.
Similarly, let "psychic research" imply any scholarly study of
such phenomena employing scientific methodology, as opposed
to any dogmatic, ritualistic, or theological approaches. Within
these definitions, the field may then be roughly divided into
two major categories: extrasensory perception (ESP) and
psychokinesis (PK).
ESP refers generally to the acquisition of information from
sources blocked from ordinary perception. Under this category
are included such subdivisions as telepathy, which refers to
detection of another person's thoughts; clairvoyance, which
refers to contemporary perception of remote physical objects
or events; precognition and retrocognition, which refer to
perception of future events and events in the past not accessible
by normal recollection; and animal ESP, which encompasses a
variety of seemingly inexplicable capabilities, such as homing,
psi-trailing, collective behavior, communication, etc.
PK (occasionally termed telekinesis, or psychoenergetics) re-
fers to a palpable influence of consciousness on a physical or
biological system. The interaction may be deliberate or spon-
taneous, and the energy transfer involved may range from
microscopic disturbance of atomic-level processes, through
macroscopic distortion or levitation of objects, up to some
very drastic "poltergeist" effects. Psychic healing and man-
plant interactions would be two examples of PK in biological
systems.
Note that in its major subdivision into ESP and PK, the field
conforms to two of the main categories of present-day science
and high technology, i.e., that encompassing the extraction, con-
version, transmission, storage, and utilization of information,
and that comprising the same sequence of processing of energy.
Other domains of psychic research can be identified which do
not fit into these major categories of ESP and PK and with which
we shall not be further concerned in this article. Examples
would include research into survival of death, and the family
of "out-of-body experiences (OBE)," including astral projec-
tion, autoscopy, and bilocation. The following table attempts
to summarize the subdivisions in a concise form.
Categories of Psychic Phenomena
1. Extrasensory Perception (ESP)
A. Telepathy
B. Clairvoyance
C. Precognition/Retrocognition
D. Animal ESP
II. Psychokinesis (PK)
A. Physical Systems
B. Biological Systems
III. Survival
A. Reincarnation
B. Apparitions
C. Mediumship
IV. Out-of-Body Experiences (OBE).
Clearly this particular arrangement is neither unique nor
orthogonal. Elements of one category frequently appear in
the context of another, e.g., precognitive clairvoyance; tele-
pathic healing, etc., and occasionally an assignment is ambigu-
ous, e.g., a particular effect may be regarded as precipitated
by PK, or simply to be forecast by precognition. Notwith-
standing, the table may aid in keeping the subsequent illustra-
tions in some order.
PATTERN OF CONTEMPORARY RESEARCH
By its nature and heritage, modern psychic research remains
rather diffuse and lightly structured, making any attempt to
catalog the work by institution or laboratory, or by tracing
developments of given lines of effort, rather ineffective and pre-
mature. Instead, it may be more useful to comment on the
pattern of attention to this field by academic disciplines, noting
the variations in emphasis, methodology, representation, and
interpretation brought to bear, using specific projects only as
illustrations with no implications. of hierarchy or attempt at
completeness. Even in this format, no recitation of specific
research results or conclusions will be attempted, since these
can be misleading or incomprehensible when extracted from
the detailed context of their experimental arrangements and
protocols. In later sections, an effort will be made to follow a
few sample experiments through to their particular results and
conclusions.
By far the most sustained and broadcast attention to this
field has been given by a cadre of scholars with professional
backgrounds in classical psychology, comprising a controversial
subdiscipline termed "parapsychology." This group has tended
to approach the field with the traditional psychological proto-
cols and vocabulary, and to interpret results in the context of
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
140
their clinical, cognitive, and behavioral psychological heritages,
with the natural consequence that they have concentrated
mainly on the ESP category of psychic tasks, although some
PK work dots their recent literature. Perhaps the most exten-
sive class of parapsychological research has attempted to
correlate psychic performance with personality variables. The
age, sex, creativity, openness, hostility, extroversion, motiva-
tion, and intelligence of the participants as indices of ability to
perform ESP tasks have been explored at length, and some sig-
nificant correlations, most notably with positive a priori atti-
tudes toward the tasks and with outgoing, creative personalities,
have been reported from several laboratories. Other studies
have searched for connections between psychic performance
and dream recall, learning and response strategies, memory,
and feedback [ 5 51- [ 611.
A more aggressive style of parapsychological research has in-
voked a variety of altered states of consciousness in attempts
to enhance psychic process. These have included various natu-
ral and traditional practices, such as sleep, meditation, and pro-
gressive relaxation [62]-[67] ; more mechanical sensory in-
hibition strategies such as hypnosis, isolation and "ganzfeld"
[66], [681-[701; and a few controversial efforts with drug-
induced states [ 711. Physiological correlates have also been
sought, using conventional EEG, GSR, and plethysmographic
equipment to monitor neurological, cardiovascular, and mus-
cular response to psy is effort [351, [72]-[75'. The diffi-
culty of obtaining successful replications of previously positive
results and an observed common tendency of participant per-
formance to deteriorate over time ("decline effect") have led
to systematic study of the role of the experimenter in eliciting
results, i.e., to consideration of the possible influence of the
experimenter's personal attitudes, expectations, and style of
interaction with his subjects, as well as the overall environmental
ambience of his laboratory, on the experimental yield [ 761 -
[81].
Despite its present recalcitrance toward more systematic
study of psychic phenomena, the richly diverse, rapidly matur-
ing parent field of psychology continues to offer an expanding
array of modern methodologies and models which could be
brought to bear on increasingly sophisticated study of this sub-
ject. Computer-assisted linguistic analyses; psychoneurological
studies of attention, perception, and concept formation; social
learning theory and similar approaches to human interactions;
and the emerging formulations of transpersonal and humanistic
aspects of human consciousness, all bear possible relevance to
comprehension of various aspects of this ultra-difficult step-
child, but at the moment, the low level of financial support,
and negative professional peer pressures have discouraged such
enterprise.
The involvement of physicists in psychic research, while con-
siderably less extensive, has been no less dedicated and no less
controversial. Since the days of Sir Francis Bacon, a number
of noted physicists have made excursions into this field,
usually to the bemusement and ridicule of their colleagues of
the day. One of the most notable of these was Sir William
Crookes, discoverer of the element thallium and pioneer in
the physics of low pressure discharges, whose broadside pro-
fessional and personal battles with the scientific establishment
over this issue make entertaining, and possibly enlightening,
reading [821. Sir Isaac Newton was intensely involved in the
study of alchemy, including some of its more metaphysical
aspects [831, and as already mentioned, Lord Rayleigh and
J. J. Thomson were active members of the S.P.R.
PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
In more recent years the attention of physicists has influenced
development of the field in at least three ways. First, their in-
terests have focused more on the PK.category of problems, i.e.,
the interaction of human consciousness with physical systems,
to balance the predominant ESP interests of the parapsycholo-
gists. Second, more sophisticated experimental equipment
than has typically been available to the psychological com-
munity has been brought to bear on the identification and
correlation of very low-level physical effects. Third, the tradi-
tional theoretical physics formalisms have been directed to the
proposition of various models of psychic phenomena, from
whence has arisen some hope of establishing the traditional
dialogue between critical experimentation and theoretical
hypothesis essential to any ultimate comprehension and appli-
cation of such phenomena.
Typical of the modern physicist's specific contributions to
the field have been the development and application of a variety
of electronic random event generators (REG) for the purpose
of identifying and correlating PK abilities in human subjects
[841-[931, and similar application of magnetometers [941,
torsional pendula [ 95 1, lasers [ 96 ], interferometers [ 97 1, and
electronic strain gauges [98], [99] to a variety of other PK
tasks. On the theoretical side, a number of applications of quan-
turn mechanics, statistical thermodynamics, electromagnetic
theory, and other formalisms to the representation of psychic
process have been proposed [1001-[1131, and attempts at
some philosophical correlation of the phenomena with other
previously or presently obscure physical processes have been
suggested [1141. Again, despite the open identification of
a few distinguished personalities with such efforts, a more
broadly held categorical rejection of the field has inhibited
much collaborative or systematic attention to it.
Up to this time, the involvement of engineers with psychic
research has been very recent, very sparse, and very much along
the lines of the experimental physicists. Beyond our own
program, which will be outlined in some detail below, I am
aware of only a very few engineering laboratories addressing
any aspects of the field in any substantial and deliberate way
[115]. These have so far tended to concentrate on applied
physics types of experimentation and on aspects of informa-
tion processing, rather than on more empirical technological
applications.
Another community of scholars to influence the pattern of
psychic research comprises the statisticians and other applied
mathematicians and logicians who have been concerned with
the proper evaluation and interpretation of the research data.
In the absence of any experiments displaying rigid causal
replicability, all of the inferences and hypotheses about psy.
chic phenomena have necessarily been based on either anec
dotal or statistical evidence. The former defy any systematic
representation; the latter are vulnerable to alternative inter
pretations and hence to impressionistic bias and argument.
Early in the emergence of mathematical statistics as at
integral discipline, S. S. Wilks found himself involved in
controversy over the validity of the statistical procedures o
early psychic researchers, and published some recommenda
tions for methods that could be applied to telepathy experi
ments [ 1161. Since that time, much of the commentary fron
the critical community has addressed perceived flaws in th
statistical methodology underlying the experimental evident
[1171, [1181, and the advocate community has reacted b;
paying disproportionate attention to this aspect of their logic
Most of the encyclopaedic references in the field contai
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
MEN,: Ps`t'CHICA11pg90ovesdaR R S2OOO 8/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6 141
substantial components on the statistical methods [119],
and the leading journals regularly display intense dialogues on
specific statistical issues [1201-[124]. At least one of these
journals routinely refers all articles submitted for publication to
a consultant statistician as a part of their review process. A
few illustrations of the statistical questions that can arise in
psychic experiments appear in the detailed examples presented
below.
A number of other disciplines have played roles in the evolu-
tion of the study of psychic process and continue to contribute,
albeit somewhat more peripherally. A succession of philoso-
phers, from Aristotle through James and Bergson to C. D. Broad
in the present era, have mused on these topics [211-[251,
11251-[1291. The intersections of the field with anthropology,
theology, and history have been approached from many per-
spectives ranging from aesthetic to analytic, and from dogmatic
to scholarly [1301-[1341. Its relevance to the study and
practice of medicine has been an enduring and intense debate,
focusing in the present day on the propriety and efficacy of
holistic health strategies and psychic healing. Isolated instances
of interest on the part of chemists, biologists, geologists, and
archaeologists can be found, and the application of psychic
techniques in criminology and law enforcement, while less
rigorous than most of the academic efforts, contributes further
anecdotal evidence to the overall data base. In the arts and
humanities, the topic continues to provide stimuli for a variety
of creative compositions.
Still other areas could be cited, but these become progressively
more satellite than central to the task of this paper. Rather
than pursuing this disciplinary survey further, it seems prefer-
able next to focus in greater detail on a few contemporary
studies that can serve to illustrate more specifically the bizarre
phenomena, the awkward and tedious protocols, and the un-
conventional theoretical concepts that arise in this class of
research. Although these will provide a better sense of the
status of the field than any attempt to summarize results from
the diffuse multidisciplinary pattern of effort sketched above,
one general assessment may be useful at this point. Namely,
throughout all of the work just skimmed, and that sharpened
somewhat below, I am aware of no reputable investigator who
has claimed, let alone demonstrated, any psychic experiment
approaching classical scientific replicability. What have been
put forward are a varied assortment of observations, currently
inexplicable in terms of established science, which display cer-
tain common phenomenological and psychological features,
and which could have substantial implications for basic physical
theory and ultimate practical applications. The following ex-
amples are presented in that spirit.
PSYCHOKINESIS
The first group of experiments selected as more detailed illus-
trations of contemporary psychic research are drawn from the
general subdomain of PK. As defined above, this broad cate-
gory of purported psychic phenomena encompasses the possible
influence of human consciousness on the behavior of physical
or biological systems or processes, and comprises several loosely
related classes of effect characterized by different scales of
energy, forms of manifestation, replicability, and statistical be-
havior. Confining attention to interactions with physical objects
or systems, the most popularly publicized class features the de-
formation, levitation, or other macroscopic disturbance of ob-
jects, as commonly propounded by professional performers,
mediums, and various Eastern practitioners [135]-[137]. Al-
though a number of serious efforts have been made to submit
such demonstrations to rigorous scientific testing, these have
tended to yield only equivocal confirmations, fodder for the
critical community, and some embarrassment and frustration
to the investigators.
Of a yet more bizarre nature are the family of very rare and
spectacular "poltergeist" effects, more technically termed
"recurrent spontaneous psychokinesis" (RSPK), wherein are
reported specific major events of levitation, vibration, telepor-
tation, and breakage of a wide range of objects, a variety of
acoustical and electromagnetic phenomena, and various optical
aberrations [138], [139]. , For years these phenomena were
naively attributed to manifestations of the spirit world, or return
of the dead to "haunted" houses, and inspired countless horror
movies and pulp-magazine articles. Recently, some order has
been brought to this weird business by systematic surveys of
documented poltergeist cases undertaken by A. R. G. Owen,
W. G. Roll, J. G. Pratt, and others [138]-[142]. In one of
these surveys, 116 cases of reported poltefgeist activity, rang-
ing back to the year 1612, were re-examined. Of these, 92
were found to be associated with particular individuals living
in the affected dwelling, most of whom were adolescents, and
most of whom were affected by some neurological/emotional
ailment, most commonly epilepsy. Often a precipitating
traumatic event could be identified which seemed to initiate
the activity, after which the general pattern involved a period
of relatively mild precursor events, a sequence of major dis-
turbances, and a period of "after shocks," extending as much
as several weeks beyond the main events. Controlled experimen-
tation on poltergeist phenomena has proven virtually impossible
because of their infrequent and unpredictable occurrence
pattern and because of the delicate physiological and emotional
situations prevailing in most cases, which have taken precedence
over the technical investigations. Nevertheless, these processes
have retained some fascination because of the magnitude of
the energy transfer involved and because of their apparent
correlation with specific types of individuals and neurological
disorders.
The most systematic and persuasive studies of PK, however,
have dealt with much more modest scales of physical distur-
bance, in some cases reaching down to the atomic level. This
somewhat more viable domain has been addressed by numerous
investigators in various ways, but basically one of two strategies
is followed. In one approach, relatively simple physical systems
are employed-mechanical, electrical, optical, thermal, etc.,-
each of which involves a particular component or process that
is ultra-sensitive to disturbance. The experiment is arranged to
signify such disturbance by a relatively large change in some dis-
play which provides feedback indication to the operator, much
in the spirit of a biofeedback instrument, and simultaneously to
provide some form of permanent data record. Examples of this
class of experiment would include the use of magnetometers,
torsional pendula, optical interferometers, electronic strain
gauges, glow discharges [ 143 1, and sensitive thermistors [ 144 ] .
In the second approach, attempt is made to distort the nor-
mal statistical patterns of various random physical processes on
either a microscopic or macroscopic scale. In a sense, these ex-
periments deal with energy rearrangement within the systems,
i.e., with their information content or entropy, rather than with
energy transfer to the system per se. The earliest versions of
this class employed dice, or other simple mechanical implements
of well-known statistical behavior [37], [92], [145]-[148],
but more recent studies have tended to employ more sophisti-
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
, Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
FRINGES ON SCREEN
RAMP DRIVE
cated apparatus, such as electronic REG's like those described
in detail below.
Although the first two categories of PK feature more dramatic
effects and thus have enjoyed greater popular attention, the
smaller scale phenomena seem more amenable to controlled
experimentation and theoretical interpretation, and the re-
mainder of this section deals only with such. Perhaps the most
efficient means of elaborating on this type of research would be
to review briefly the spectrum of such experiments in progress
in our own laboratory, and then to display and attempt to
interpret data from one of them. In so doing, we intend no
neglect of other work noted in the references, but simply deal
from greater familiarity.
As examples of the first class of low-level PK experiment
mentioned above, we have in operation a Fabry-Perot optical
interferometer, a dual-thermistor bridge, and a photoelastic
strain detector. In the interferometer experiment, shown in
the photograph and schematic of Fig. 1, a Coherent Optics
Instrument #360-370, using a diffuse sodium lamp as source,
is adjusted to produce circular fringes on a screen visible to the
operator (Fig. 2). Small changes in the separation of the inter-
ferometer plates cause the fringes to migrate radially inward or
outward. By visual observation of the fringe movement, plate
motions of less than 0.1 wavelength can be readily detected.
Via a. pinhole in the screen, the brightness of the central fringe
is recorded by a photomultiplier/chart recorder system at an
order of magnitude higher sensitivity, thereby preserving quan-
titative output data while the operator simultaneously sees an
the fringe pattern in a stated direction relative to the normal
baseline drift of the instrument. The protocols involve rigid
control and monitoring of the environment of the instrument
and surrounding laboratory, and the interspersing of baseline
responses with active PK efforts obtained under otherwise
identical conditions, including the position of the operator and
any other personnel relative to the instrument. In pilot studies
with this device, a variety of fractional-fringe responses were
observed, using several different operators and various initial
interferometer settings. A more formal procedure has since
been developed which provides more precise conditions for
an ongoing series of trials. In this protocol, the central fringe
is set initially on a maximum gradient position between a
bright and dark fringe, and its progress monitored for subse-
quent periods of baseline or PK effort. Encouragingly repli-
cable data have been obtained from a number of different
operators, in the form of chart recordings of 5-min PK trials
with interspersed 5-min baseline drifts of the instrument. Using
computerized graphic, regression, and spectral analyses of the
data, it is possible to discern characteristics in the hierarchy
of trace derivatives and the Fourier spectra which, while not
definitive, display certain recurrent features [97] . No physical
interpretation has been attempted other than to acknowledge
that the observed fringe migrations could also be indicative
of slight changes in the index of refraction of the air in the
plate gap or in the wavelength of the light source, as well as of
a displacement of the plates.
The dual-thermistor experiment comprises a much more sen-
attractive optical display of his progress for use as feedback. sitive version of a multiple-thermistor arrangement on which
~l@ latRceleea8iaiQ4O/QZritro4Af RDR9 U7c88RN4 OO S Schmeidler [ 1441.
mal
igid
ent
line
use
and
lies
ere
tial
ce
for
ge
is
se-
?li-
3nt
'als
ng
he
by
lot
:.al
ge
we
he
of
en-
ich
4].
JAIaN: PSAp row"i c. R Ie 29LWtQ J4 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Fig. 2. Interferometer fringe patterns.
As shown in Fig. 3, two Omega Engineering thermistors,
Model UVA 3254, each with its own electronic bridge and volt-
age source, are connected differentially to a Tektronix l A7A
oscilloscope preamplifier and to a visual feedback display. With
suitable ground planes and cable shielding, sensitivities greater
than 0.001 K are obtainable, and by subtracting the two output
signals the major portion of spurious electrical and mechanical
interference is eliminated. The effects of ambient thermal vari-
ations in the laboratory are essentially excluded by enclosing
each of the thermistors in identical Pyrex flasks immersed in
a large liquid reservoir, in which configuration the undisturbed
system exhibits stable baselines over long periods of time.
Using the same interspersed baseline protocol as in the inter-
ferometer experiment, the task of the operator is to achieve an
increase in the reading of one thermistor with respect to the
other or in some more subtle fashion to alter the PK response
relative to the baseline. Some such effects have indeed been ob-
served, but little systematic data have so far been accumulated
on this experiment.
Also in a preliminary stage is an experiment to monitor in-
ternal strain in a solid specimen via photoelastic optical tech-
niques. Several studies have been reported on the PK deforma-
tion of solids, but most of these have employed conventional
engineering strain gauges or microacoustic sensors as detectors
[98], [99], [135], [149], [150], both of which require sub-
stantial interface electronics before a feedback signal reaches
the operator, leaving unclear the role of the sensor in any pos-
sible PK influence. - Although less sensitive than the electronic
methods, photoelastic techniques have the advantage of relating
the operator more directly to the sensitive element of the ex-
periment via an attractive optical fringe pattern much like that
of the interferometer (Fig. 4). This equipment and technique
may also be applied to a sensitive levitation experiment wherein
Fig. 3. Dual-thermistor apparatus.
the object is suspended on a photoelastic lever arm of suitable
dimensions.
Within the second category of low-level PK experiment, we
are employing or are now constructing several devices based on
random physical processes, some macroscopic in scale, others
deriving from atomic-scale processes. The largest of these in-
volves a 6 X 10 ft apparatus, shown in Fig. 5, which drops some
10 000 - in spheres through a "quincunx" array of 336 nylon
pegs in about 12 min. As a consequence of the multitudinous
collisions with the pegs and with each other, the spheres are dis-
persed into a good approximation of a Gaussian distribution
as they fall into 19 collecting compartments at the bottom.
The goal of the operator is to distort the distribution in some
prescribed fashion to a significant degree compared to empirical
baseline experience. Photodiode counters mounted in funnels
at the entrance to each bin provide real-time digital displays of
the bin populations to supplement the more qualitative feedback
of the growing ball stacks seen by the operator and to provide
quantitative data for on-line statistical analysis. Fig. 5 shows
a typical baseline distribution for this device and a distorted
distribution obtained in a particular PK effort. Full statistical
analysis of the significance of any particular achieved pattern
is a challenging problem in its own right, since it must deal
with a combination of 19 bin populations, each of which has
its own empirical baseline mean and standard deviation, all
constrained by total ball count.
A somewhat similar experiment, not yet refined, employs a
device which allows small metallic or dielectric spheres to
bounce on an optically flat, precisely horizontal circular plate
of glass, which is oscillated by a vibration coil at frequencies
from 10 Hz to 20 000 Hz. In the absence of any external dis-
turbance, a sphere started at the center of the plate executes a
random walk toward the outside edge, arriving with equal prob-
ability at any azimuth. Since the sphere may make as many as
I0$ collisions in the process, it is vulnerable to statistical distor-
tion of its trajectory and consequent terminus. The task of the
operator is preferentially to direct the sphere to a prescribed
terminal quadrant.
In an attempt to intervene with a random physical process at
the atomic level, we have constructed a large glow-discharge
device whose luminous patterns are indicative of the mean free
path of the current-carrying electrons against inelastic excita-
tion collisions with the background gas. This device, shown in
Fig. 6, presents a 36-in X 2-in diam cylindrical glow marked by
a sequence of bright and dark zones along its positive column
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788 R001,100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6 ___
i2 " "fit1Lltlt ...:~"~t9Ittl ; .
Fig. 5. Gaussian analog device and distributions,
typical of dc discharges in a given range of gas pressure and
terminal voltage. The number and locations of these striations
are sensitive to the electron inelastic mean free path, which in
turn depends on the gas type and density, the electron tempera
ture, and the local electric field. Striation position is monitorec
by photoelectric detectors, and the goal of the operator is t(
expand or contract the pattern on demand, to a significan
extent compared to the normal background jitter and drifi
Protocols are much the same, output data take the same ger
eral form, and are analyzed by the same algorithms as in th
interferometer and photoelastic experiments.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
1982 1, JAHN: P # " t or. 142Q1Q Wi617 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6 145
uu
ELGENGO
NOISE SOURCE
S 3602A15124
LOW-
PASS
FILTER
SAMPLER
GATE
SAMPLE
PULSE
GENERATOR
Fig. 7. Functional diagram of REG.
II s _ ~~ _ I ~M ~.
(a)
~~ ~ IIII! VIII
(b)
:mpera-
nitored
or is to
riificant
d drift.
me gen-
s in the
Fig. 8. REG waveforms: (a) Filtered noise. (b) Clipped noise.
(c) Sampling pulses.
A number of other atomic-scale random system PK experi-
ments are under consideration, design, or construction, involv-
ing such processes as information storage on a microelectronic
chip, the spontaneous decay of phosphorescent surfaces, lam-
inar to turbulent transition in a fluid stream, atomic and molec-
ular resonators, and resonant acoustical or electrical cavities,
but none of these is far enough advanced to merit description
here. Rather, we shall concentrate for the remainder of this
section on a more detailed presentation of our most serviceable
experiment, and the one on which we have the largest data
base, the electronic REG.-
RANDOM EVENT GENERATOR EXPERIMENTS
REG's have been the most widely used and most productive
facilities for experimentation with low-level PK. Although a
broad variety of such devices exist, most involve four conceptu-
ally and functionally separable components: an electronic noise
source; a sampling system which examines the noise at pre-
scribed intervals and prepares an output pulse train correspond-
ing to the samples thus obtained; a system which analyzes the
pulse train in accordance with preset instructions and prepares
suitable output for a feedback system; and the feedback dis-
play itself, which informs the operator -of the results of the
'sampling process.
The particular version we have employed utilizes a packaged
commercial noise source module based on a solid-state junction
and precision preamplifier (Elgenco Model 3602A15124), but
modules employing radioactive decay units or glow discharges
can be readily substituted. This source produces a random noise
spectrum up to several megahertz, which our logic circuit first
filters to a flat spectrum from 50 to 20 000 Hz, then amplifies
and clips to the flat-topped profile shown in Figs. 7 and 8. This
is then sampled by a regular train of gate pulses, yielding a cor-
responding random succession of positive and negative output
pulses indicative of the sign of the noise at the time of sampling,
and these are then counted. Since the average time between
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000108/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
INTERNAL
NOISE
SOURCE
External Noise Source .
r
SAMPLMIG hit
RATE
GENERATOR
Rote
Control
O
+1
Etternot Pulse
Generator
( S micro sec.Widlh)
Set N
PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
SCALE-OF-N
COUNTER
Reset Signal
To Event
Counters
7 Analog Output
TRIAL COUNTER
AND LED DISPLAY
Fig. 9. Electrical schematic of REG.
zero crossings of the clipped noise waveform is about 30 ?s,
sampling rates to about 15 000/s can be tolerated with statis-
tical independence.
The full functional array is sketched in Fig. 9, and a photo-
graph of the boxed units in Fig. 10. By panel setting the
sampler may be instructed to take "trials" of 100, 200, or 2000
samples, at a frequency of 1, .10, 100, 1000, or 10 000/s. The
Train of
N Puts"
Print Analog
CUMULATIVE Output
Gate OFF on _ _ uainur.
SCALE-OF-SO START
COUNTER CIRCU OL
% A
Manuel Start or
Automatic Repeat
450 Times)
Reset to
Printer
Trial Counter
counting system may be set to count only positive pulses, only
negative pulses, or to alternate positive and negative counting
on successive samples. The alternating positive/negative mode
effectively factors out any systematic bias in the noise source,
and is the mode employed in all the experiments reported here.
The counting results are displayed by LED arrays tracking both
the running count of each trial and the concurrent mean rela-
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788 R001100010004-6
1982 JAHN: PSYCHIC PHENOMENA: ENGINEERING PERSPECTIVE 147
TABLE I
REG 200-SAMPLE DATA SUMMARY
Series
Instr.
No.
Trials
Mean
Std.
Dev.
t-score
Pr
n+/n-
REG I
B.L.
12 000
100.009
6.994
0.144
0.443
5678/5611
PK+
-
4 550
100.264
7.037
2.528
0.006
2230/2056
PK
3 850
99.509
7.063
-4.313
10-5
1716/1926
A PK
8 400
4.890
5 X 10-7
REG II
B.L.
2 500
100.033
6.875
0.239
0.406
1188/1179
PK+
-
1950
100.247
6.849
1.590
0.056
916/919
PK
1800
99.597
6.775
-2.526
0.006
797/902
APK
3750
2.920
0.002
REG III
B.L.
+
3 500
99.977
7.013
-0.193
0.424
1658/1655
PK
-
2 400
100.227
6.821
1.634
0.051
1150/1086
PK
2 600
99.736
7.026
-1.918
0.028
1192/1270
A PK
5 000
2.507
0.006
E REG I
B.L.
18 000
100.006
6.981
0.115
0.454
8524/8445
II
PK+
-
8 900
100.250
6.938
3.403
3 x 10-4
4296/4061
III
PK
8 250
99.600
6.989
-5.203
10-7
3705/4098
A PK
17 150
6.107
5 X 10-10
REG la
no B.L.
PK+
2150
100.206
7.091
1.340
0.088
1059/993
PK-
2100
99.945
6.937
-0.365
0.358
954/1019
APK
4250
1.213
0.113
REG Ila
B.L.
5 000
100.186
6.974
1.882
0.030
2367/2337
PK+
-
2000
100.117
7.041
0.746
0.228
955/950
PK
1750
99.941
6.898
-0.360
0.359
803/839
A PK
3 750
0.772
0.220
E REG I
B.L.
23 000
100.045
6.980
0.978
0.164
10891/10782
la
II
PK+
13 050
100.223
6.979
3.644
10-4
6310/6004
IIa
III
PK-
12100
99.709
6.968
-4.596
2 X 10-6
5462/5956
A PK
25 150
5.828
3 x 10-9
only
sting
lode
.rce,
sere.
both
rela-
tive to a preset origin and are permanently recorded on a strip
printer. For most of the experiments described below, an AIM-
65 microprocessor interface is also utilized to insert the trial-
count data on-line into processing routines supported by a
TERAK Model 8510 used as a terminal and PDP 11/45 and
VAX 750 employing a UNIX operating system programmed in
C language. All of the sampling, counting, and display functions
can be simply checked by referring them to an internal or ex-
ternal calibrated pulse train generator.
The device also has a manual/automatic option, whereby it
will either collect its trial samples only when a panel switch
or parallel remote switch is pushed, or it will repeat that process
for 50 trials automatically once activated by the switch. The
operator thus has the option of triggering each trial or of
initiating a repetitive flow of 50 such trials with no further
intervention.
The experiments reported here were performed by a single
operator, seated in front of the device with the remote initia-
tion switch in hand and the LED count indicators and TERAK
terminal display visible. This operator attempted, on instruc-
tion or volition, to distort the trial counts either toward higher
or lower values. The several options of sampling number, sam-
pling frequency, +/- polarity, and manual/automatic sequenc-
ing were variously determined by random instruction, operator
preference, or experimental practicality, and recorded before
the beginning of each trial. Clearly, the full matrix of such pos-
sibilities could not be explored, and for our first sequence of
experiments only 200-sample trials were used, at 100 or 1000
counts/s, all counted in the +/- alternating mode. The auto-
matic/manual and high/low options were more thoroughly
tested, in both the volitional and instructed choice modes.
Fifty trials of the 200-sample units comprised a test run, and
data from these were processed individually and in many concat-
enations via a statistics package in the UNIX system developed
specifically for this task. Calculated were the mean, standard
deviation, range, kurtosis, skew coefficient, z-score, t-score,
X2 goodness-of-fit with both 8 and 16 degrees of freedom, and
the corresponding one-tailed probabilities against chance of the
last four measures. Applied to earlier and ongoing baseline
data, this analysis confirmed that in undisturbed operation this
REG conforms very well to a Gaussian approximation to the
appropriate full binary statistics.
The major portion of the results listed below comprised three
separate experimental series, extending over fifteen months,
labeled REG I, REG II, and REG III, respectively. All other
data acquired under slightly less formal conditions of protocol
during this period, included for completeness, are grcluped
under two other series, labeled REG la and IIa, respectively.
Details of these series protocols, calibration tests, and their in-
dividual results are available in the reference [931. All told,
over 25 000 active PK trials were obtained, corresponding to
more than 5 000 000 binary events.
Table I summarizes all of the baseline and PK data acquired
during these five experimental series. A total of 23 000 base-
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
100
COUNT
P ? 05
J t--------
4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 14000
NUMBER OF TRIALS
6000
5000
4000
3000
2000
1000
0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000
NUMBER OF PAIRS OF TRIALS
Fig. IS. REG cumulative deviations in direction of effort of all 200?
sample data.
2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 14000
NUMBER OF TRIALS
Fig. 16. REG cumulative average deviations of PK* and PK- 200-sample
data.
line trials were taken under a variety of conditions before,
during, and after the active PK trials. Their overall mean was
100.045, and their standard deviation 6.980, compared with
the values of 100.000 and 7.071 predicted by the theoretical
Gaussian approximation to the appropriate binary statistical
distribution. As shown in Fig. 11, the frequency of count dis-
tribution conformed very well with the theoretical curve.
The results of the PK trials are also presented in Table I and
in Figs. 12 and 13. Briefly, the 13 050 high-instruction trials,
denoted PK' , yielded a mean of 100.23 and a standard devia-
tion of 6.979; the 12 100 low-instruction trials, denoted PK , .
yielded a mean of 99.704 and a standard deviation of 6.968.
The one-tailed probability of chance occurrence of the former,
computed from t-score, is about 10-4 ; of the latter, about
2 X 10-6. The combined probability of the split, i.e., of this
total "direction-of-effort" success, denoted APK, is about 3 X
10-9. ( A number of more elaborate statistical measures have
been applied to these data; the results are not qualitatively
changed thereby.)
As is evident from Figs. 12 and 13, and as is verified by the
more detailed statistical tests performed, no significant distor-
tion of the frequency-of-count distributions other than the
shifting of the means has occurred. In other words, the ob-
served effect is to shift the total distributions intact, rather
than to distort any of their higher moments significantly. This
result clearly has felicitous implications for this class of experi-
mentation, since it allows much simpler and faster data collec-
tion and analysis than might otherwise have been anticipated.
It is illustrative to exhibit the overall data behavior via graphs
of the cumulative deviation of the trial score means versus the
accumulated data base. Fig. 14 shows such a representation
for the total data pool plotting PK, PK-, and baseline data
with reference to cumulative 0.05 confidence levels. Fig. 15
uses a similar representation for compounding the PK+ and PK-
data in a "direction of effort" cumulative deviation. (Had REG
Ia and IIa been excluded from these data, the overall slopes
would have been slightly more. severe and uniform.)
Alternatively, the cumulative data may be presented in terms
of the progressions of the average deviations from the theoreti-
cal mean, as shown in Fig. 16, where the stochastic variations
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
BASELINE
---------------
P1- ,05
BASELINE
tl
S!
e;
0
h
b
sl
sl
tl
h
t(
h,
h
t1
gi
tl
S(
lil
al
an
1 N(
tiv
to
istor-
a the
.e ob-
:ather
This
xperi-
;ollec-
ite.d.
;raphs
us the
tation
e data
rig. 15
A PK-
d REG
slopes
i terms
teoreti-
iations
JAHN: P9YCAHICpProedNAOfNGIFEERIRZ`i 990919 07 : CIA-RDP96-00788R001100010004-6
149
of the small data bases are seen to damp out to well-defined this operator, who claimed no special talent for this or any
terminal values after several thousand trials. other psychic task, reported that any conscious variations in
The PK+ and PK- effects also manifest themselves in terms psychological strategy, such as focus of visual attention, or
of the number of trial means recorded above and below the intensity of concentration or desire, did not appear to have
theoretical value. As displayed in Table I, PK"' efforts were any evident effect on the yield. Similarly, differences in the
generally characterized by an excess of trial means above laboratory ambience, such as the lighting level, background
100.00 .and PK- efforts by an excess below. The total con- noise, or peripheral presence of other persons, did not seem
catenations in these terms are significant at a level of 0.003 to influence this operator's performance. When queried about
for PK' and 3 X 10-5 for PK-. any impressionistic sense of the interaction process, the opera-
The ensemble of results acquired in these experiments dis- tor alluded to a "resonance or identification with the system,
play certain instructive general features: leading to a loss of self-awareness similar to that experienced
.1) The importance of accumulating very large data bases in a game, a movie, or some creative occupation." Clearly this
when dealing with such marginal phenoma is emphasized by class of parameter will be the most difficult to specify and cor-
the relative scales of the statistical vagaries and the broader relate, and we are far from any definition of its mechanisms.
systematic trends in Figs. 14-16. Although the trends are With respect to experimental options on the equipment pa-
established early in the data collection sequence, unambigu- rameters, we can make very limited explorations with the
ous departures from the accumulated vagaries of chance be- acquired data base. Briefly, binary correlations of the data for
havior occur only well into the total 25 000 trial, 5 000 000 the 100/1000 counting rate option, for the volitional /instructed
bit, sequence. direction of effort, and for automatic/manual sampling give
2) Over this large a data base, there arises some quantitative little indication of importance of such factors in the overall
statistical regularity in the PK process, epitomized by the mean performance. Each category shows clear and significant separa-
slopes of the cumulative deviations in Figs. 14 and 15 and by tion of the means for the PK+ and PK- efforts, with little to
the terminal values of the average deviations in Fig. 16. Traced choose between the t-scores for the various categories. Thus,
back to the elemental binary samples, these values imply direc- at least for the data base at hand, the process seems insensitive
ted inversions from chance behavior of about one or one and a to these particular experimental parameters.
half bits in every one thousand or, alternatively, of 0.2 or 0.3 We have also attempted correlation in terms of the trial-
bits per trial. number sequence. With cognizance of the ubiquitous "de-
3) The differences between the somewhat larger values for cline effect" which is reported over a broad range of psychic
the PK- deviation and the lesser values for PK+ are only mar- experimentation, we have prepared an algorithm which cross-
ginally significant on this data base, but prevail rather uniformly concatenates from the data base all scores achieved on the
throughout all the test series. The suspicion that these reflect first trials of the experimental run, all achieved on the second
some subtle bias in the REG itself is not supported by the base- trials, etc., up to the fiftieth, and arrays those fifty means in a
line data, which concatenate to a grand mean very slightly graphical form. The results show little systematic profile of
above the theoretical value. yield versus trial number. A similar exercise has been per-
One of the primary goals of such controlled PK studies at formed to cross-concatenate the data by run-number over the
this early phase in the understanding of the phenomena is to various series to search for a decline effect on that larger scale,
develop experiments of sufficient yield and replicability that but again no significant correlation is found within this data
various parametric correlations may be systematically explored, base.
thereby hopefully separating the consequential from the incon- The most extensive parametric exploration attempted to date
sequential factors. The experiments outlined above hold some was motivated by the apparently fundamental question implicit
promise of serviceability for this purpose, but a great deal of in general conclusion 2) above, i.e., whether the magnitude of
data will need be accumulated to establish any such correla- the observed effect correlates with the total number of bits
tions. Four classes of parameters could be considered: those processed, or with the number of trials. To explore this aspect,
associated with the experimental equipment; those associated the same operator has performed a second ensemble of expert-
with the operator's physiological and emotional characteristics; mental series totaling 25 000 trials, all consisting of 2000-sam-
those associated with the operator's technique; and various en- ple bits rather than 200. As before, various combinations of
virQnmental factors not directly associated with either. So far the automatic/manual and volitional /instructed modes were
we have accumulated only small amounts of data from other employed, but to speed data acquisition and reduce the opera-
operators, and given the general indication #1 above regarding tor's tedium, only the 1000/s counting rate was used. This,
the importance of large data bases, we can make no statement coupled with the more elegant data processing capabilities that
about the generality or peculiarity of our principal operator's had evolved over the preceding experiments, allowed this
performance. Similarly, we have attempted no systematic vari- sequence to be completed in less than six months.
ation of external environmental factors, and although test times, The results of this effort, as presented in Table II and Figs.
dates, durations, and laboratory temperature, pressure, and 17 and 18, are curiously ambivalent. As before, there is clear
humidity have been routinely recorded, we cannot comment and' significant separation of the means of the PK+ and PK-
on the importance of this category of parameter. efforts, and the baseline is well behaved. As could be antici-
On the matter of operator technique, it should first be re- pated from the larger standard deviation of the 2000-bit data,
emphasized that the sole formal difference between the PK+ the cumulative traces display larger statistical fluctuations and
and PK-?trials is the specified intention of the operator to in- require a larger number of trials to settle toward well-defined
fluence the device to generate numbers in the assigned direction. terminal values. To the quantitative precision allowed by this
No other variation in protocol is permitted, save those subjec- data base, these terminal values appear not to endorse any
tive differences in psychological attitude the operator chooses simple bit-level hypothesis in that they fail by a factor of 6 or
to invoke. Although no records of such aspects were kept, 7 to achieve the one or one and one-half bits per thousand
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
'15Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RD>REDgiGi8or~THE1T9K, VAL. 70, N062, FEBRUARY 1982
TABLE II
SUMMARY OF REG 2000-SAMPLE DATA
z
0
3750
w
0
0
3750
U
Intr.
No..
Trials
Mean
Std.
Dev,
t-score
Pr
B.L.
12
500
1000.016
21.879
0.079
0.468
6157/6088
PK+
12
200
1000.380
21.906
1.914
0.028
6092/5897
PK-
12
800
999.569
22.005
-2.216
0.013
6218/6351
A PK
25
000
-
-
2.920
0.002
-JS-J11/ pi-.05
BASELINE
----/-pt 05
- 780 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000 14000
NUMBER OF TRIALS
Fig. 17. REG cumulative deviations of PK+and PK - 2000-sample data.
inversion accomplished in the 200-bit trials. However, the
new values are larger on a per-trial basis by a factor of about
1.7, which is not negligible in this context. Again, much more
data of this sort will be required to come to grips with this class
of correlation.
In addition to continuing study of this sample-size parameter,
our next generation of experiments employs a number of other
operators to explore the variation of yield with operator type
and technique, and a number of alternate noise sources, includ-
ing pseudo-random sources, in an attempt to localize the effect
somewhat and thereby to narrow the range of future experi-
ments and models.
The results outlined above are by no means the only conse-
quential REG data available for contemplation. Of particular
interest are a variety of experiments reported by Schmidt,
some employing pseudo-random as well as physically random
sources, and others using taped source outputs recorded well
in advance of their presentation to the operator [87]-[90].
In another approach, May has recently reported an REG study
using electronic gear specifically designed to preclude very
subtle artifacts that might confound the effects of interest,
and includes in his paper a thorough search of the modern
REG literature [ 911.
In addition, considerable research in the parapsychological
community has been performed using REG devices as drivers
for various forced-choice video games employed in both the
PK and clairvoyance modes [ 1511. Many of these claim sig-
nificant yields, but rarely are the data-bases sufficiently large
to present quantitative trends, or to allow much parameteric
correlation.
Regardless of their particular implementation, any potential
vulnerability of random electronic noise sources to incidental
or intentional distortion by the means under study here would
seem to be of some interest to a number of engineering com-
munities, given the proliferate application of such devices in
various functional and computational capacities.
REMOTE PERCEPTION
As a second example of contemporary psychic research that
has displayed some substantial yield and interlaboratory repli-
0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10000 12000
NUMBER OF TRIALS
Fig. 18. REG cumulative average deviations of PK' and PK- 2000-
sample data compared to 200-sample data.
cability, we select a topic which has come to be called "remote
perception" or "remote viewing." The basic concept of this
process is far from new; in the early 16th century, Paracelsus
stated it unequivocally:
Man also possesses a power by which he may see his friends and
the circumstances by which they are surrounded, although such
persons may be a thousand miles away from him at that time [ 8 ] .
In its modern form, the experimental protocol requires a
"percipient" to describe, by free-response oral or written
narrative or drawing, a remote, unknown target location at
which is stationed an "agent," with whom there is no normal
sensory mode of communication during the course of the
experiment. The targets are usually selected by some prescribed
random process from a previously prepared pool of targets,
which is unknown to any of the active participants. The
quality of the perception is assessed by various impressionistic
or analytical judging methods described below.
Historically, this experiment has evolved from several genera-
tions of free-response clairvoyance and telepathy experiments,
which were found to have certain advantages over the more
traditional "forced-choice" ESP tasks, such as the Xener card
identifications of the early Rhine laboratory [341-[37], in
displaying less tendency for percipient stagnation and "decline-
effects" over extended testing, and in maintaining some of.the
spontaneity of anecdotal clairvoyance experiences. One of
the earliest detailed reports of such free-response studies appears
as a book by Upton Sinclair entitled Mental Radio, which
features an equivocal foreword by Albert Einstein [1521.
More modern work of this class was performed at the Mai-
monides Medical Center by Ullman and Krippner in the 1960's,
and reported in their book Dream Telepathy [64]. From this
work emerged the so-called "ganzfeld" or sensory inhibition
perception studies of Honorton and many others which pro-
pounded the desirability of emotionally stimulating tasks to
which the subjects could relate in a personal and spontaneous
fashion [66], [67].
The contemporary version of the remote perception protocol
was introduced in a sequence of publications by Targ and Put-
hoff [94], [1531-[156], which prompted a substantial num-
ber of attempted replications [1571-[1741, and considerable
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
JAHNi PSYCHIC Apt Q4rF EEW@9M 08/07: CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6 151
Fig. 19, Remote perception target: Picnic area, Feathered Pipe Ranch
near Helena, MT; 12:00 N MDT, Sept. 5, 1978.
Percipient transcript: Princeton, NJ; 8:30 A EDT, Sept. 5, 1978:
"Outdoors... open landscape-large areas of trees-pines? Inter-
spersed with open fields. Single road. High overcast, cool, breezy.
(Agent) in dark jacket talking to someone near road-possibly turn-
out area or picnic area. Assembly of stones-possibly pylon or, marker
or wall. Large sign somewhere."
critical comment. The most extensive of the replications, con-
ducted by Dunne and Bisaha in the Chicago area over the period
1976 to 1979, comprised 40 formal trials to which were applied
157 independent transcript judgings, 84 of which assigned first-
place rank to the proper targets [ 1611, [ 1621.
The type of data which can be acquired in such studies is
illustrated in the sequence of Figs. 19-22. In each case is
shown a photograph of a particular target, selected by some
random process, which was visited by an agent on the date
and time indicated. Below each figure is a portion of the cor-
responding percipient transcript, with the time and location of
the perception effort also noted. The examples shown are
drawn from a variety of experimental series conducted under
somewhat different protocols, but serve to display some of the
characteristics which commonly appear in the more successful
efforts:
1) The overall ambience of the scene is accurately perceived.
2) Certain details are accurately identified; others are mis-
construed or totally ignored.
3) A feature which is impressive to the agent is not necessarily
so to the percipient, and vice versa.
4) The composition of the scene may be distorted by errors
in scale, relative positions of key objects, or total right
left inversions. -
5) The aesthetic aspects, such as colors, general shapes, degree
of activity, noise level, climate, and other ambient features
tend to be more accurately perceived than more analytical
details such as number, size, or relative positions.
6) The perception is not necessarily centered on the defined
target, and may even provide accurate information on ad-
jacent areas external to the target, unnoticed by the agent.
7) The fidelity of the perception seems to be independent of
the remoteness of the target, up to distances of several
thousand miles.
8) The time of the perception effort need not coincide with
the time the agent is at the target. Perceptions obtained
several hours, or even days, prior to the agent's visit to the
target, or even prior to selection of the target, display at
least as high a yield as those performed in real time.
Fig. 20. Remote perception target: Woodrow Wilson School, Princeton,
NJ; 2:15 P EDT, Aug. 28, 1980.
Percipient transcript: Princeton, NJ; 12:15 P EDT, Aug. 28, 1980:
"Some kind of courtyard, enclosed by buildings on two sides. Paths
or walks around periphery, a statue or monument of some kind in the
middle surrounded by grass. Could be a fountain; I have the feeling
of water. Trees or tall hedges on one side. Fairly quiet, but some
people walking around. Not sure of sound, the idea of a fountain sug-
gests sound of water but I'm not sure I really hear it or not."
The philosophical and practical implications of items 7 and 8
are clearly substantial. If the data are valid, the most parsimo-?
nious explications would require access of the percipient's con-
sciousness to other portions of the space-time grid than that in
which it is currently immersed, or that it can reach by normal
processes of communication or memory. These same items
also seriously delimit the potential physical mechanisms for
such access.
Rigorous evaluation of the data from experiments such as
these is. confounded by the psychological components of the
process, by the impressionistic nature of the information in-
volved, and by the inevitable subjective biases of all those par-
ticipating in the experiment. Doubtless the earliest and most
primitive assessments were informal a posteriori exchanges of
impressions about the target between agent and percipient
which, although possibly informative and stimulating to them,
lacked any quantitative basis and held little scientific credibility.
In a somewhat less vulnerable strategy invoked more recently,
the percipient, after completing his transcript, visited several
possible targets drawn from the pool and attempted to identify
the one he perceived, or to rank-order each of them in terms
of conformity to his earlier perception. Statistical arguments
could then be applied to these ranks to estimate the likelihood
that information about the target had been acquired by means
other than chance [ 1751- [1771.
In an attempt to separate the possible ESP functioning of the
percipient during the visitation and ranking process from his
original perception effort, the protocol subsequently evolved to
invoke independent judges who were provided copies of the
various transcripts and taken to the target sites to perform their
preferential rankings. Even in this form, the technique has
been criticized for possible sensory cuing of the judges [ 1781
and has tended in turn to be replaced by a protocol wherein
the judges perform their ranking on the basis of photographs
of the targets, usually taken by the agent at the time of the
trial. In one such version, the judges, who have not been in-
volved in any earlier portion of the experiment, are asked to
compare a single percipient transcript with agent-generated
descriptions and photographs of a number of alternative targets,
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
Fig. 21. Remote perception target: Rockefeller Chapel, Chicago, IL; 2.15 P CDT, June 10, 1977.
Percipient transcript: Mundelein College, Chicago, IL; 1:00 P CDT, June 10, 1977:
"I'm seeing a heavy wooden door with a black bolt on it rounded at the top in a dome fashion. I have a feeling of opening the doors and
looking in and it's dark inside. My feeling at the moment is that it's a building like a church. And I can see the pews. There is some light but
them
I feel basically a kind of darkness in there and a quietness. I'm seeing little turrets, very elaborate-looking little turrets, a whole series s offthem ima like across the entire top of the building and there's a straight line and then up to a trianglI.m hgetting ave a demo star nag glass wand wl s -type that statue
arch
marble, flowing robes. I see the door again and I see some stairs. I think it's very hig
building
the
ornamentation
the
of it
ecture
blue.
ark
d took
shape and hey
quite 1 k there'sta sect onion the top with t e turrets and then be elow that there are some other kinds of d signs but mo e l nearadesigns. and it looks t tha "I again have a vision of the doors and then maybe a ledged area or an which iareah f this uil air or at prof u es with s mreend of ai desig and
there maybe a couple ofthose before you get to the top part There , le filigree
turrets s or r something. And within in the building there Sesort oof a continuation of arches, ns It looks like it's ached, but possibly they meet columns or something like
Ap~~re~lt i"1~' Pg X011 1O? r -F~DP96-00788RO01100010004-6
and
but
hem
,tue,
arch
)oks
and
igree
like
, JAHN: pS-Appre EFerEkt#aa?eG209M8 7 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6 153
Fig. 22. Remote perception target: Danube River, Bratislava, Czecho-
slovakia; 3:00 P European Standard Time, Aug. 24, 1976.
Percipient transcript: Minoqua, WI; 8:30 A CDT, Aug. 23, 1976:
"I have the feeling that the agent is somewhere near water. I seem to
have the sensation of a very large expanse of water. There might be
boats. Several vertical lines, sort of like poles. They're narrow, not
heavy. Maybe lamp posts or flag poles. Some kind of circular shape.
It's round on its side, like a disc, it's like a round thing flat on the
ground, but it seems to have height as well. Maybe with poles. Could
possibly come to a point on top.. Seeing vertical lines again. Seems to
be a strong impression, these vertical lines. Predominant colors seem
to be blue and green. Water again., Some very quick impression of a
fence, a low fence. The steps seem to go up to some kind of fence.
It's a dark fence and it's along like a walk sort of at the top of the
steps. The steps sort of lead up to like a path or walkway. Like a
boardwalk. And there's a fence along it. There's people walking
along it, and there's vertical lines along that walkway."
including the proper target, and to rate or rank-order them by
some prescribed criteria. Again, statistical assessment of the
significance of the rankings follows.
The bulk of the remote perception data reported in the litera-
ture has been evaluated by some form of these independent
judging processes, and displays sufficiently high yield to encour-
age further refinement of the protocol and analysis before
attempting categorical judgment on the validity and viability
of the phenomena. ' Beyond minor tightening of the target
selection, agent maneuver, and perception acquisition and re-
cording phases of the experiment, the major potential improve-
ments would still seem to pertain to the judging process, which
remains potentially vulnerable to subtle cues in the transcripts,
to vagaries in the judges' capability, to their subjective biases
toward the individual experiments and to the topic as a whole,
and to possible psychic input of their own [ 1611, 1621.
In an effort to improve the judging process further, our labora-
tory has explored. the applications of various information theo-
retic methods to the quantification of the data, and an analytic
technique has been devised which is based on a limited binary
alphabet of target/perception descriptors [1791. While less
sensitive to the Gestalt impressionism and symbolic represen-
tations which a human judge might capture, this method does
provide a rudimentary framework for evaluation of signal-to-
noise ratio in the information transfer, and an assessment
standardization less dependent on subjective interpretation.
In essence, the strategy is to replace impressionistic assessment
of the quality of a perception by the identification of specified
elements of information therein, after which a mechanical
scoring and ranking procedure takes over. In the hope of con-
veying a bit more substantive flavor of the data acquired in
remote perception experiments and the processing thereof,
permit us to describe this analysis in a little detail.
The heart of the method is the establishment of a code, or
alphabet, of simple descriptive queries which may be addressed
to all targets and all perceptions, responses to which serve to
distinguish them and to permit quantification of the informa-
tion acquired in the perception process. In one version, these
"descriptors," thirty in number, are posed in binary form and
range over a spectrum from quite factual discriminations, e.g.,
whether the scene is indoors or outdoors, whether trees are
present, or whether there are automobiles, to much more im-
pressionistic aspects, such as whether the ambience is noisy or
quiet, confined or expansive, hectic or tranquil. The particular
ensemble of descriptors has evolved in part through personal
experience and intuition, and in part through trial-and-error
application to various pilot data. The goal has been a balanced
alphabet whose elements are a) relatively unambiguous; b) com-
monly perceived by a broad selection of percipients; c) individu-
ally instructive in defining the scene; d) complementary to one
another; and e) sufficient in number to permit reasonable syn-
thesis of the scene, but not so numerous to burden the data
collection or computation excessively.
Given this descriptor alphabet, each target in the pool is then
represented in terms of 30 binary bits, corresponding to the ap-
propriate YES/NO responses to the queries. This encoding is
normally performed by the agent at the time of target visita-
tion, although reference may be made to the target selector's
judgment or to photographs of the target for verification.
Each perception is similarly rendered into a corresponding
sequence of binary digits, but only after the percipient has been
allowed to form a free-response impression of the target. Vari-
ous scoring recipes are then invoked for quantitative comparison
of the perceptions with the targets, using for computation the
UNIX operating system of a PDP 11 /45 or VAX 750.
The simplest recipe merely counts the number of correct
responses to the 30 descriptors, i.e., the positive correlations
between the target and descriptor matrices. This does not nor-
mally provide a particularly accurate index of the quality of the
individual perceptions, since the a priori probabilities of the
various descriptors are widely different. For example, a given
pool may have more outdoor than indoor targets, and hence
a correct identification of an indoor context should be given
higher credit than identification of an outdoor context. To
facilitate such weighting, a step is included in the computational
program to provide the a priori probabilities of all descriptors
in the prevailing target pool, on the basis of which more elabo-
rate scoring recipes may be invoked.
Since the various targets have substantially different charac-
teristics and hence different capacities for achieved scores, a
variety of normalization procedures also have been developed,
using as denominators the total number of descriptors, the per-
fect score, i.e., the score that would be achieved for a given
target if all descriptors were identified correctly, and various
"chance" scores for the target, defined by some random or
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE, VOL. 70, NO. 2, FEBRUARY 1982
TABLE III
PRECOGNITIVE REMOTE PERCEPTION 24 X 24 "CHICAGO" SERIES
PROPER TARGET RANKS
Human Jud
es
E
Avg.
Avg./24
g
Mean Rankb
1
3/4
7
2
5.9
0.25
2.7/8 =
0.34
2
1/2
2
1
1.9
0.08
1.0/8 =
0.13
3
1
1
1
1.0
0.04
1.5/8 =
0.19
4
2/8
7/2
2/2
4/2
2/2
4.5
0.19
2.7/8 =
0.34
5
7/3
9
11
4
8
8.0
0.33
1.7/8 =
0.21
6
9/5
12/2
7/2
16/2
7/2
11.0
0.46
3.5/8 =
0.44
7
13/2
11
14
10
13
12.3
0.51
2.3/8 =
0.29
8
20/3
22
20
14
19
19.2
0.80
1.818 =
0.23
9
4/2
4
8
1
5
4.5
0.19
2.6/7 =
0.37
10
10/7
13
13
9
5
10.6
0.44
1.4/7 =
0.20
11
9/4
9
11
6
12
9.7
0.40
3.6/7 =
0.51
12
1/3
2
2
2
5
2.6
0.11
1.8/7 =
0.26
13
1/3
1
3
1
3
2.0
0.08
2.2/7 =
0.31
14
1
2
2
1
1
1.4
0.06
1.4/7 =
0.20
15
1/2
2
2
1
1
1.5
0.06
1.0/7 =
0.14
16
2
2
2
1
2
1.8
0.08
1.0/10 =
0.10
17
1/4
9
1
9
1
4.5
0.19
1.0/5 =
0.20
18
2/2
10
5
1
2
4.1
0.17
1.0/5 =
0.20
19
14/3
14
14
19
17
15.8
0.66
unjudged
20
7/6
.11
11
8
10
9.9
0.41
5.0/6 =
0.83
21
1/2
2
3
4
1
2.3
0.10
2.0/6 =
0.33
22
5/6
7
12
2
7
7.1
0.30
3.0/6 =
0.50
23
16/4
23
11
15
9
15.1
0.63
3.0/6 =
0.50
24
3
3
4
3
4
3.4
0.14
2.0/6 =
0.33
5.79
6.67
0.28
0.31
a Computed rank/number of ties for that rank.
bAssigned rank/number of possible ranks.
arbitrary process of descriptor response. A "selective" scoring/
normalization process has also been applied which effectively
allows the percipient to reject any descriptor on which he feels
unqualified to comment, and thence to be scored only on the
reduced descriptor set.
The statistical significances of these various normalized per-
ception scores are assessed by a collective ranking process
reminiscent of the traditional human judging techniques, but
having the advantages that the ranking proceeds on a much
more standardized and analytical basis, and that many more
alternative targets can be ranked by the machine than by a
human judge. Specifically, the program scores each transcript
not only against its proper target, but against every other tar-
get in the pool, and then ranks these targets in order of descend-
ing score and specifies the rank of the match with the proper
target. This process is repeated for every scoring method, and
the results displayed in corresponding matrix arrays.
Table III displays typical results' of these analytical ranking
procedures as applied to a group of 24 perceptions of 24 targets
in the Chicago area. Tabulated are the ranks of the proper tar-
gets compared with all other targets for each of the perception
efforts, as computed by five of the scoring methods we have
found to be most instructive, namely, A) number of correct
descriptors/total number of descriptors; B) weighted full de-
scriptor score/perfect score; C) weighted lull descriptor score/
number of descriptors; D) weighted selective descriptor score/
perfect score; and E) weighted selective descriptor score/chance
score. Also included in the table are the mean ranks assigned by
independent human judges subjectively comparing these per-
ceptions with a much smaller number of alternative targets.
Although the bases of comparison are quite different, it
appears that in the majority of these cases the analytical and
impressionistic evaluations concur at least roughly in their
estimate of the quality of the perceptions, particularly for those
which consistently obtain low rank assignments. If the analyt-
ical computation is carried through using as target pool only
those alternative targets available to the human judges, the
agreement in mean ranks is found to be somewhat closer, per-
haps fortuitously so, given the categorically different bases of
assessment implicit in the two methods.
To this analytically scored and ranked data it is possible to
apply a variety of statistical assessments of widely ranging
sophistication and complexity. Consistent with the rather
broad mesh of the descriptor code and the elementary scoring
recipes invoked in this version of the concept, we confine our-
selves to correspondingly simple statistical measures which
provide at least semi-quantitative indication of the yield beyond
chance. Specifically, we address only the distribution of proper
target ranks achieved in the series of perceptions, such as sum-
marized in columns 2-6 of Table III. Using the common z-
score method for a discrete distribution, the probability of
achieving the mean rank of any of these columns by chance
may be directly computed. Table IV displays the results of
such calculation for the same 24 X 24 "Chicago" series. Note
that, whereas all of the methods suggest significant departures
of the computed mean ranks from chance, there is relatively
little disparity among them, indicating that the specific method
of scoring and normalization is not a sensitive element in the
overall evaluation of the perception series.
The departure of the shape of the proper target rank distribu-
tion from chance is also displayed in Table IV in terms of the
number of perceptions achieving first-place ranks, the number
rank
its a
T1
num
den(
in w
pool
cipie
Nori
Be
illusi
metl
Eurc
utilh
The
perci
local
roug
tion
resul
previ
consi
To
some
score
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
Y 1982
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00788RO01100010004-6
JAHN: P Y HIC PHENOMENA: ENGINEERING PERSPECTIVE
TABLE IV
PRECOGNITIVE REMOTE PERCEPTION. 24 X 24 "CHICAGO" SERIES
SCORE SUMMARY
Method'
Mean
Rank
No. 1st (2nd)
Ranks
No. Ranks
Below Mean
Z
Pz
X2(4)
PX
Chance
expectation
12.5
1.0
(1.0)
12
A
6.73
4.4
(4.0)
19
-4.08
2 x 10-5
18.5