STANDARD REMOTE-VIEWING PROTOCOL (LOCAL TARGETS). SRI. HAROLD E. PUTHOFF AND RUSSELL TARG.
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Release Decision:
RIPPUB
Original Classification:
K
Document Page Count:
16
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
April 4, 2000
Sequence Number:
1
Case Number:
Publication Date:
November 1, 1978
Content Type:
REPORT
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4.pdf | 498.87 KB |
Body:
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-0
SG1I
STANDARD REMOTE-VIEWING PROTOCOL
(LOCAL TARGETS)
by
Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ,
SRI International
333 Ravenswood Ave. ? Menlo Park, CA 94025 ? (415) 326-6200 ? Cable: SRI INTL MNP ? TWX: 910-373-1246
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
? Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
STANDARD REMOTE-VIEWING PROTOCOL
(LOCAL TARGETS)
The basic outline of our standard remote-viewing protocol is as
given in our tutorial paper, "A Perceptual Channel for Information
Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent
Research," H. Puthoff and R. Targ, Proc. IEEE, pp. 329-354, March 1976.1
The elements of the protocol, each of which is addressed below, consist
of (1) target pool selection; (2) subject orientation; (3) outbound
experimenter behavior; (4) inbound experimenter behavior; (5) post-
experiment feedback; (6) judging procedure.
1. Target Pool Selection
To carry out an experimental series of, say, n trials with a
subject, a list of targets >> n should be prepared in advance by an
experimenter who will not interact with the subject after that. The
targets should be chosen to be distinctive, but not necessarily distinct
from each other; that is, rather than just a collection of nondescript
street corners one should select bridges, towers, fountains, gardens,
plazas, etc., so that a judge could in principle recognize targets on
the basis of correct but sketchy descriptions. On the other hand, once
having chosen a fountain-type target, there should be several fountain
targets; for a bridge target, several bridge targets, etc., in order to
avoid the possible subject strategy of "I had a bridge yesterday, so it
can't be a bridge today." The subject should be told explicitly that
there are similar as well as different types of targets.
When the target list is made, each target location should be
written on a card and placed in an envelope, the envelopes randomized
and numbered. These should then be stored in a secure safe or similar
container.
With regard to whether a target is replaced in the pool after use,
the preferable procedure, from a methodological standpoint, is to
replace it. (A problem with actual replacement is that the subject,
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
upon becoming aware of a mental image of a previous target, might be
biased to reject it as memory. An acceptable alternative is to replace
a used target by a new one of similar type--e.g., one fountain by
another.)
2. Subject Orientation
Before the experiment, the subject should be shown some previous
remote-viewing results with one goal in mind--to get across the idea
that one should, as nearly as possible, report raw perception rather
than analysis, since the former tends to be correct and the latter is
almost always wrong. A subject needs to understand that a rounded piece
of blue metal is just that, and that he should not initially try to
determine what it is. Remind the subject that imagination constitutes
noise in the channel, and therefore the closer he can get to raw uninter-
preted imagery, the better. To have success in the above, the best
guideline we have found is to choose as subjects individuals who are
self-confident, uninhibited, successful, and not afraid to be wrong.
No psychological test we have investigated is as reliable as the above
subjective assessment in choosing subjects.
3. Outbound Experimenter Behavior
At the start of an experimental session, the inbound and outbound
experimenters and subject should rendevous for a relaxed informal dis-
cussion in the laboratory setting. (The outbound experimenter or
experimenters must not know the target at this time.) Together they
agree on a time for the subject description to start (e.g., 30 minutes
hence--the length of time required to reach the furthest target in the
pool; this time is then an invariant for all experiments.) The outbound
experimenter then leaves the laboratory, uses a random-number generating
procedure to obtain a number from 1 - n (number of targets in pool),
*Figures 3 and 4 in the IEEE pape' are good examples. In Figure 4 the
subject had absolutely no concept of a pedestrian overpass, but simply
saw a pattern of receding squares; in Figure 3 correctly-dimensioned
pools of water were misinterpreted as purification plant pools rather
than recreational swimming pools.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
obtains the so-numbered envelope from the target pool, and leaves the
premises. (We use a Texas Instruments SR-51 hand calculator, which has
a random-number function.) After driving away from the laboratory, he
opens the envelope to determine the target, and then proceeds to that
location. He should arrange to park and then come upon the target
location at exactly the starting time so that his view of it is fresh
at the beginning of the experiment. He then simply pays attention to
the environment and does not let his mind wander (especially to another
target). It does not appear to matter how many people comprise the out-
bound team, provided they do not (1) pay attention only to each other,
or (2) scatter about. At the end of the agreed-upon target viewing time
(usually 15 minutes) they return to the lab.
4. Inbound Experimenter Behavior
During the period that the outbound experimenters spend en route
to the target, the inbound experimenter and subject have a period to
relax and discuss the protocols. (Inbound it is best not to have addi-
tional observers.) The goal of the inbound experimenter during this
period is to make it "safe" for the subject to experience remote viewing.
For the initial orientation of a new subject, this typically includes a
low-key pep talk as to how remote viewing appears to be a natural, not
abnormal, function, that many people appear to have done it successfully,
even their first time, and always including the reminder to eschew
analysis and simply render raw impressions.
Since we think that remote viewing is a difficult task, like per-
ceiving a subliminal stimulus, we think it takes the full attentive
powers of the subject. Therefore, the environment, procedures, etc.,
should be as natural and comfortable as possible to minimize the
attention on anything other than the job at hand. No hypnosis, strobe
lights, or sensory-deprivation procedures are ever used, since in our
view these (novel) environmental factors take away some of the subject's
much-needed attention. We are in this sense proponents of a "naturalist
school." If the subject feels more comfortable smoking, or drinking a
cup of coffee, that is permitted. These should be arranged ahead of
time, however, so that neither subject nor experimenter leave the
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
experimental room while waiting for the outbound experimenter to reach
his target.
The experimenter should have arranged ahead of time to have pen and
paper available for drawing, and a tape recorder. When the agreed-upon
experiment time arrives, the inbound experimenter simply asks the subject
to "describe what impressions come to mind with regard to where the
outbound experimenter is." Most subjects prefer to close their eyes,
but they should simply do what comes naturally. The room lighting is
preferably subdued to prevent after-image highlights, shadows on eyelids,
etc. It is best that the inbound experimenter not pressure the subject
to say a lot; he should act as if there is all the time in the world.
Otherwise, a subject may tend to embroider descriptions just to be
saying something to please the experimenter. If the subject tends
toward being analytical ("I see Macy's") the experimenter must gently
lead the subject into description, not analysis. ("You don't have to
tell me where it is, just describe what you see.") This is the most
important and difficult task of the inbound experimenter.
It is also useful for the inbound experimenter to "surprise" the
subject with new viewpoints. ("Go above the scene and look down--what
do you see? If you look to the left, what do you see?") The subject's
viewpoint appears to shift rapidly with a question like this, and the
data come through before the subject's defenses activate to block it
out. The shifting of viewpoint also obviates the problem of the subject
spending the entire time giving meticulous detail on a trivial item,
such as a flower, which, even if true, will be of no help to a judge.
Once a subject feels he sees something, he tends to hang on to this
perception rather than commit himself to a new viewpoint.
The subject must be encouraged to sketch what he sees, even over
his objections that he is not an artist, can't sketch, etc. He may do
so throughout, or wait until the last five minutes if intermittent
drawing would distract his concentration. Since drawings tend to be
more accurate than verbalizations, this is an extremely important
factor for good results.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
5. Post-Experiment Feedback
When the outbound experimenter returns, the inbound and outbound
experimenters and subject should proceed directly to the target for
feedback. This helps to develop the subject's sense of which parts of
his mental imaging are correct, versus incorrect. It completes the
experiment for him, so that when he does a following experiment, his
mind is not still involved with wondering how he did on the previous
one. Only a very experienced subject can function well time after time
without feedback, so this must be done for each experiment to ensure
success.
6. Judging Procedure
In a sense, the most critical part of the remote-viewing procedure
is the judging. Any single experiment in remote viewing, even if per-
fect, can in principle be dismissed as possibly coincidence. Further,
any result less than perfect can be dismissed as a generalized "grass
is green, sky is blue" transcript that fits every target. Only blind
differential discrimination across a series of targets can put these
interpretations to rest.
To prepare the transcripts for judging, an experimenter not involved
in judging must read the transcripts and delete from them any reference
to dates or previously used targets, so that a judge could not order
the transcripts chronologically or otherwise obtain a priori information
useful in matching.
Two judging procedures can then be used: Direct Matching, and
Rank Ordering. Both procedures assume that n experiments have been
carried out and n responses obtained. The judge must then try to
determine which of the n responses goes with which of the n targets.
a. Direct-Matching Procedure
The n responses (transcripts with associated drawings) are
numbered in random order and given to the judge along with the list of
n targets, also in a (different) random order. The key is known by an
experimenter, but not the judge. The judge then visits the target sites
and constructs a one-to-one correspondence list between targets and
responses without replacement; that is, no target or response is used twice.
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP596-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
With the correspondence list and the aid of the key, the
experimenter then consults the statistical table for Direct Matching
(Table 1) to determine whether the result is statistically significant.
For example, if there were 5 correct matches out of 9 responses, the
table indicates that the probability of obtaining such a result by chance
is p = 0.003125, or roughly 3 times out of a thousand. Since the accepted
standard in behavioral research is that a result can be considered signi-
ficant if one obtains the value p s 0.05, such a result would be
considered significant--that is, indicative of a nonchance correspondence.
The Direct Matching procedure is the simplest to carry out, but
will give no credit for a fairly good description if a judge has
difficulty in choosing between two possibilities and chooses the wrong
one. This procedure is thus overly conservative. The more difficult
Rank Ordering procedure, described next, gives partial credit in such a
case, and is therefore a more precise statistical tool for analysis of
medium-grade results.
b. Rank-Ordering Procedure
In the use of the Rank-Ordering procedure, the experimenter
randomizes the targets and transcripts as before. Now, however, each of
n judges is given a set of the n transcripts but only one of the target
sites to investigate. Each judge's task is to visit his assigned target
site, read through all the transcripts, and order them best-to-worst
match (1 through 5, say, if there are five targets and five transcripts).
With the aid of the key, the experimenter then adds up the
rank-ordering numbers assigned to each target's associated transcript.
For example, if the actual response to a target was given a first place
when a judge was looking at the target, then it gets a 1. If the actual
response to a target was given a third place match when a judge was
looking at that target, then it gets a 3, etc. The addition of these
numbers 1 + 3 + ... then yields a number called the sum of ranks. One
then consults the rank-ordering table (Table 2) for the statistic of
interest. For example, if there were 5 experiments (5 targets and 5
transcripts) and the sum of ranks was 9, the table for 5 x 5 gives a
6
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
1
(f
P
N
?
r
N
Pli
P
1?? ?I
O
CP
a
0,
n
0
B'
??~
N O
'
0
0 ?
0
?N
IP
PII
.?
'??
A C
0
O ?,
O
A
fa
O',
n
C
an
c c
c
O
C
.-f
1?
ti
PI
?'?
PI
O
O O.
O
O I
O
'0
p
#i
?D
?`?
O
?O
a O
O
IC,
O
?1
?
?PI
I~?I
?I
I
C
?
C
?
O
?O
?
O O'
? ?
O
?
O
O
?
?
fn
' .
F
1
-I
on
? R7
?
? O
0
O'
? '
A
r
ey
r,
10:
Q
f
? C
~?
40
? O
0 CD
.+
fa
0
0'
n
O'
in
O O+
O
O'
.r
P.
A
Pl'
.?
fn!
e
O O :
O
O
p
0
0
4
0'
O
O O
0
O
P1
P
.r.
O
C
C :%
O
O
O
0
O
.?
O
O
~?
O N
?
O
I?.
11-
1
v
PI
on,
N
??'
'0
?
O
c
10
N
P
in
01
In
O
,
?
0
O
o
o b O
w1
fr
e-
N
-?:
P
0
A
0
C
0:
UI,
o p o
P- !O ?
wl
0
0-
s
N
4
N
'O P ?
o
0
0
a
ff1
..
? O ?
O
P
P
P
PI
-
U1
f1
O 'O
O
,p
,p
e
O
.-f
01
o O
o
w1
P1
'?
e
e
o,
o 0
0
N
1-
0
-
0
? ? In
o
U1
N
-
N
P
P N
fr
0
F
0
p..
O
r
'0
N:
o ? 00
p
o
m
'p
e
o 0
O
C
en
O
O
?+
PI
on,
o
O
in
In
o
o o
!-
0-
0
P1
N
0
'O
,
0
'O
O
fo
PI
P-
O
O
O.
~O
'0
0
O
c
o
WI
In
?
P/
'0
m
'o
P
In
4
4
to
1f1
O
PI
P1
.?
O
O
ti
O
1-
P1
?
f1
!1
O
0
P1
?
th
in
'o
0
'0
P1
'o
r-
'0
0
o'
e1
r1
-
o
0
o
m
0
6n
P1
O
Y
H
PI
U1
U) W
on
O
?
1-
O a
f 5
O
??
'0
C1 ~?
fo
fn
rr1
P1
0
WI
?
p
??
V/r
?
,.
a
O 0
N O ? 0
O ? O
U)
? o
? O
0
IH 'b
z
U 0
0
LL E
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Number of Targets - 4; Number of Transcripts 4
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
4
0.39063E-02
5
0.19531E-01
6
0.58594E-01
7
0.13672E 00
8
0.25781E 00
9
0.41406E 00
10
0.58594E 00
11
0.74219E 00
12
0.86328E 00
13
0.94141E 00
14
0.98047E 00
15
0.99609E 00
16
0.10000E 01
Number of Targets - 5; Number of Transcripts - 5
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
5
0.320 00 E- 03
6
0.19200E-02
7
0.67200E-02
8
0.17920E-01
9
0.403201-01
10
0.79040E-01
11
0.13824E 00
12
0.21984E 00
13
0.32224E 00
14
0.43904E 00
15
0.56096E 00
16
0.67776E 00
17
0.78016E 00
18
0.86176E 00
19
0.92096E 00
20
0.95968E 00
21
0.98208E 00
22
0.99328E 00
23
0.99808E 00
24
0.99968E 00
25
0.10000E 01
*The notation E-02 is to be understood as 10-2; E 01 as 101; etc.
8
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Table 2 (Continued)
Number of Targets - 6;
Number of Transcripts - 6
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
6
0.21433E-04
7
0.15003E-03
8
0.60014E-03
9
O.18004F-02
10
0.45010E-02
11
0.99023E-02
12
0.19676E-01
13
0.35880E-01
14
0.60764E-01
15
0.96472E-01
16
0.14463E 00
17
0.20585E 00
18
0.27939E 00
19
0.36310E 00
20
0.45357E 00
21
0.54642F 00
22
0.63689E 00
23
0.72061E 00
24
0.79415E 00
25
0.85537E 00
26
0.90353E 00
27
0.93923E 00
28
0.96412E 00
29
0.98032E 00
30
0.99010E 00
31
0.99550E 00
32
0.99820E 00
33
0.99940E 00
34
0.99985E 00
35
0.99998E 00
36
0.10000E 01
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Table 2 (Continued)
Number of Targets - 7;
Number of Transcripts - 7
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
7
0.12143E-05
B
0.97141E-05
9
0.43714E-04
10
0.14571E-03
11
0.40071E-03
12
0.96170E-03
13
0.20837E-02
14
0.41589E-02
15
0.77458E-02
16
0.13585E-01
17
0.22595E-01
18
0.35838E-01
19
0.54453E-01
20
0.79544E-01
21
0.11205E 00
22
0.15259E 00
23
0.20137E 00
24
0.25802E 00
25
0.32161E 00
26
0.39065E 00
27
0.46315E 00
28
0.53685E 00
29
0.60935E 00
30
0.67839E 00
31
0.74198E 00
32
0.79863E 00
33
0.84741E 00
34
0.88795E 00
35
0.92045E 00
36
0.94555E 00
37
0.96416E 00
.38
0.97740E 00
39
0.98641E 00
40
0.99225E 00
41
0.99584E 00
42
0.99791E 00
43
0.99903E 00
44
0.99958E 00
45
0.99984E 00
46
0.99995E 00
47
0.99998E 00
48
0.99999E 00
49
0.10000E 01
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Table 2 (Continued)
Number of Targets - 8;
Number of Transcripts - 8
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
8
0.59605E-07
9
0.53644E-06
10
0.26822E-05
11
0.98348E-05
12
0.29504E-04
13
0.76711E-04
14
0.17899E-03
15
0.38356E-03
16
0.76663E-03
17
0.14447E-02
18
0.25867E-02
19
0.44264E-02
20
0.72724E-02
21
0.11515E-01
22
0.17628E-01
23
0.26157E-01
24
0.37702E-01
25
0.52890E-01
26
0.72328E-01
27
0.96562E-01
28
0.12602E 00
29
0.16095E 00
30
0.20139E 00
31
0.24714E 00
32
0.29772E 00
33
0.35237E 00
34
0.41012E 00
35
0.46982E 00
36
0.53018E 00
37
0.58988E 00
38
0.64763E 00
39
0.70228E 00
40
0.75286E 00
41
0.79860E 00
42
0.83905E 00
43
0.87398E 00
44
0.90344E 00
45
0.92767E 00
46
0.94711E 00
47
0.96229E 00
48
0.97384E 00
49
0.98237E 00
50
0.98849E 00
51
0.99273E 00
52
0.99557E 00
53
0.99741E 00
11
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Table 2 (Continued)
Number of Targets - 9; Number of Transcripts - 9
SUM OF RANKS
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
P-VALUE
0.25812E-08
0.25812E-07
0.14196E-06
0.56786E-06
0.18455E-05
0.51675E-05
0.12919F-04
0.29529E-04
0.62748E-04
0.12547E-03
0.23821E-03
0.43226E-03
0.75357E-03
0.12673E-02
0.20628E-02
0.32586E-02
0.50075E-02
0.75003E-02
0.10968E-01
0.15683E-01
0.21954E-01
0.30122E-01
0.40548E-01
0.53601E-01
0.69639E-01
0.88989E-01
0.11192F 00
0.13864E 00
0.16924E 00
0.20370E 00
0.24189E 00
0.28353E 00
0.32821E 00
0.37540E 00
0.42447E 00
0.47469E 00
0.52531E 00
0.57553E 00
0.62460E 00
0.67179E 00
0.71647E 00
0.75811E 00
0.79630E 00
0.83076E 00
0.86136E 00
0.88807E 00
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Table 2 (Concluded)
Number of Targets - 10;
Number of Transcripts - 10
SUM OF RANKS
P-VALUE
10
0.10000E-09
11
0.11000E-08
12
0.66000E-08
13
0.28600E-07
14
0.10010E-06
15
0.30030E-06
16
0.80080E-06
17
0.19448E-05
18
0.43758E-05
19
0.92378E-05
20
0.18475E-04
21
0.35261E-04
22
0.64559E-04
23
0.11412E-03
24
0.19512E-03
25
0.32387E-03
26
0.52317E-03
27
0.82418E-03
28
0.12686E-02
29
0.19106E-02
30
0.28197E-02
31
0.40825E-02
32
0.58049E-02
33
0.81133E-02
34
0.11156E-01
35
0.15103E-01
36
0.20143E-01
37
0.26484E-01
38
0.34347E-01
39
0.43960E-01
40
0.55552E-01
41
0.69345E-01
42
0.85541E-01
43
0.10432F 00
44
0.12581E 00
45
0.15011E 00
46
0.17725E 00
47
0.207211 00
48
0.23987E 00
49
0.27506E 00
50
0.31255E 00
51
0.35202E 00
52
0.39311E 00
53
0.43538E 00
54
0.47838E 00
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
probability of obtaining such a rank ordering result by chance of
0.0403..., which is significant. A more complete set of tables is given
in Solfvin et al.2
14
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4
1. H.E. Puthoff and R. Targ, "A Perceptual Channel for Information
Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and
Recent Research," Proc. IEEE, Vol. 64, pp. 329-354 (March 1976).
2. G. Solfvin et al., "Some New Methods of Analysis for Preferential-
Ranking Data," J. Am. Soc. for Psychical Research, Vol. 72, No. 2
(April 1978).
15
Approved For Release 2000/08/07 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500400001-4