A PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER OVER KILOMETER DISTANCES. HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE AND RECENT RESEARCH

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6
Release Decision: 
RIFPUB
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
117
Document Creation Date: 
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date: 
April 4, 2000
Sequence Number: 
1
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
March 1, 1976
Content Type: 
RP
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6.pdf4.61 MB
Body: 
0 m, reim:Fn won [HI a A PERCEPTUAL CHANNEL FOR INFORMATION TRANSFER OVER KILOMETER DISTANCES: HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE AND RECENT RESEARCH by Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory Stanford Research Institute 333 Ravenswood Avenue, Menlo Park, California 94025 To be published in the PROCEEDINGS OF THE IEEE March, 1976 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 For more than one hundred years, scientists have attempted to deter- mine the truth or falsity of claims for the existence of a perceptual channel whereby certain individuals are able to perceive and describe remote data not presented to any known sense. This paper presents an outline of the history of scientific inquiry into such so-called para- normal perception and surveys the current state of the art in parapsycho- logical research in the United States and abroad. The nature of this perceptual channel is examined in a series of experiments carried out in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of Stanford Research Insti- tute. The perceptual modality that we have investigated most extensively is the ability of both experienced subjects and inexperienced volunteers to view, by innate mental processes, remote geographical or technical targets including buildings, roads, and laboratory apparatus. Our ac- cumulated data indicate that the phenomenon is not a sensitive function of distance, and Faraday cage shielding does not in any apparent way degrade the quality and accuracy of perception. On the basis of this research, some areas of physics are suggested from which a description or explanation of the phenomenon could be forthcoming. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Vii LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ix ACKNOWLEDGMENT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . xi I INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . I II BACKGROUND . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 III SRI INVESTIGATIONS OF REMOTE VIEWING . . . . . . . . . . . . 25 A. Subject Si: Experienced . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 B. Subject S4: Learner . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 C. Subjects S2 and S3: Experienced . . . . . . . . . . . . 40 D. Subjects S5 and S6: Learners . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 E. Normal and Paranormal: Use of Unselected Subjects in Remote Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 F. Technology Series: Short-Range Remote Viewing . . . . . 51 G. Summary of Remote Viewing Results . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 1 . Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 59 2. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 67 IV CONSIDERATIONS CONCERNING TIME . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 V DISCUSSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83 VI CONCLUSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 APPENDICES A SIGNAL ENHANCEMENT IN A PARANORMAL COMMUNICATION CHANNEL BY APPLICATION OF REDUNDANCY CODING . . . . . . 91 B REMOTE VIEWING TRANSCRIPT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101 REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 I Airport in San Andres, Colombia, Used as Remote Viewing Target Along with Sketch Produced by Subject in California . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 2 Occipital EEG Frequency Spectra, 0-20 Hz, of One Subject (H.H.) Acting as Receiver Showing Amplitude Changes in the 9-11-Hz Band as a Function of Strobe Frequency . . . . . 24 3 Swimming Pool Complex as Remote Viewing Target (a) City Map of Target Location (b) Drawing by Price (Si) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29 4 Subject Hammid (S4) Drawing Described as "Some Kind of Diagonal Trough up in the Air" . ... . . . . . . . . . . . 36 5 Subject Hammid (S4) Response to Bicycle Shed Target Described as an Open "Barn-Like Building" with "Slats on the Sides" and a "Pitched Roof" . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 6 Subject Elgin (S2) Drawings in Response to Tennis Court Target . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 7 Subject Swann (S3) Response to City Hall Target . . . . . . . 44 8 Subject (S6) Drawing of White Plaza, Stanford University . . 48 9 Subject (V1) Drawing of Merry-Go-Round Target . . . . . . . . 50 10 Drawings of a Typewriter Target by Two Subjects . . . . . . . 54 11 Drawings by Three Subjects (S2, S3, and V3) for Xerox Machine Target When subject (V3) was asked to described the square at upper left, the subject said, "There was this predominant light source which might have been a window, and a working surface which might have been the sill, or a working surface or desk." Earlier the subject had said, "I have the feeling that there is something silhouetted against the window." . . 55 12 Drawing by Two Subjects of a Video Monitor Target . . . . . . 56 13 Subject (S4) Drawing of Drill Press Showing Belt Drive, Stool, and a Graph that Goes Up and Down . . . . . . . . . . 60 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 14 Subject Hammid (S4) Described "Some Kind of Congealing Tar, or Maybe an Area of Condensed Lava .... That Has Oozed Out to Fill up Some Kind of Boundaries" . . . . . . . . 76 15 Subject (S4) Described a Formal Garden "Very Well Manicured" Behind a Double Colonnade . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 16 Subject (S4) Saw a "Black Iron Triangle That Hal Had Somehow Walked Into" and Heard a "Squeak, Squeak, About Once a Second" . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 17 Subject (S4) Described a Very Tall Structure Located Among City Streets and Covered With "Tiffany-Like Glass" . . 80 A-1 Enhancement: of Signal-to-Noise Ratio by Sequential Sampling Procedure (p0 = 0.4, pl = 0.6, a = 0.01, 8 = 0.01) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 95 A-2 Reliability Curve for Sequential Sampling Procedure (p0 0.4, p1 = 0.6, a = 0.01, a = 0.01) . . . . . . . . . . 98 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 I Critical Values of Sums of Ranks for Preferential Matching . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32 2 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Transcripts Associated with Each Target Location for Experienced Subject Price (Si) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 3 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Transcripts Associated with Each Target Location for Learner Subject Hammid (S4) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 4 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Transcripts Associated with Each Target Location for Experienced Subjects Elgin (S2) and Swann (S3) . . . . . . . 45 5 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Transcripts Associated with Each Target Location for Subjects S5 and S6 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 6 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Transcripts Associated with Each Target Location for Visitor Subjects Vl and V2 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 7 Distribution of Rankings Assigned to Subject Drawings Associated with Each Target Location . . . . . . . . . . . . 58 8 Summary: Remote Viewing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61 9 Experimental Protocol: Precognitive Remote Viewing . . . . . 73 A-1 5-Bit Code for Alphanumeric Characters . . . . . . . . . . . 92 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 ACKNOWLEDGMENT This work was supported by the Foundation for Parasensory Investi- gation and the Parapsychology Foundation of New York City, the Institute of Noetic Sciences, Palo Alto, California, and the National Aeronautics and Space Administration, NAS 7-100. We wish to express our gratitude to our principal subjects, Mrs. Hella Hammid, Mr. Pat Price, and Mr. Ingo Swann, who showed us patience and forbearance in addition to their en- thusiasm and outstanding perceptual abilities. We note with sadness the death of one of our subjects, Mr. Price. We express our sincere thanks also to Mr. Earle Jones, Mr. Bonnar Cox, and Dr. Arthur Hastings, of SRI, and Mrs. Judith Skutch and Mr. Richard Bach, without whose encouragement and support this work could not have taken place. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 A Perceptual Channel for Information Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical Perspective and Recent Research Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ I INTRODUCTION "It is the province of natural science to investigate nature, impar- tially and without prejudice [1]." Nowhere in scientific inquiry has this dictum met as great a challenge as in the area of so-called extrasensory perception (ESP), the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by the usual sensory processes. Such phenomena, although under scientific con- sideration for over a century, have historically been fraught with unre- liability and controversy, and validation of the phenomena by accepted scientific methodology has been slow in coming. Even so, a recent sur- vey conducted by the British publication New Scientist revealed that 67 percent of nearly 1500 responding readers (the majority of whom are working scientists and technologists) considered ESP to be an established fact or a likely possibility, and 88 percent held the investigation of ESP to be a legitimate scientific undertaking [2]. A review of the literature reveals that although experiments by reputable researchers yielding positive results were begun over a century ago (e.g., Sir William Crookes' study of D. D. Home, 1860s) [3], many con- sider the study of these phenomena as only recently emerging from the realm Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 of quasi-science. One reason for this is that, despite experimental re- sui'its, no satisfactory theoretical construct had been advanced to corre- late data or to predict new experimental outcomes. Consequently, the area in question remained for a long time in the recipe stage reminiscent of electrodynamics before the unification brought about by the work of Ampere, Faraday, and Maxwell. Since the early work, however, we have seen the development of information theory, quantum theory, and neuro- physiological research, and these disciplines provide powerful conceptual tools that appear to bear directly on the issue. In fact, several physicists (Section V) are now of the opinion that these phenomena are not at all inconsistent with the framework of modern physics: the often-held view that observations of this type are a priori incom- patible with known laws is erroneous in that such a concept is based on the naive realism prevalent before the development of quantum theory. In the emerging view, it is accepted that research in this area can be conducted so as to uncover not just a catalog of interesting events, but rather patterns of cause-effect relationships of the type that lend themselves to analysis and hypothesis in the forms with which we are familiar in the physical sciences. One hypothesis is that information transfer under conditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely low frequency (ELF) electromagnetic waves, a proposal that does not seem to be ruled out by any obvious physical or biological facts. Further, the development of information theory makes it possible to characterize Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 and quantify the performance of a communications channel regardless of the underlying mechanism. For the past three years, we have had a program in the Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory of SRI to investigate those facets of human perception that appear to fall outside the range of well-understood perceptual/processing capabilities. Of particular interest is a human information-accessing capability that we call "remote viewing." This phenomenon pertains to the ability of certain individuals to access and describe, by means of mental processes, information sources blocked from ordinary perception, and generally accepted as secure against such access. In particular, the phenomenon we have investigated most extensively is the ability of a subject to view remote geographical locations up to several thousand km distant from his physical location (given only a known person on whom to target).` We have carried out more than 50 experiments under controlled laboratory conditions with several individuals whose re- mote perceptual abilities have been developed sufficiently to allow them at times to describe correctly--often in great detail--geographical or technical material such as buildings, roads, laboratory apparatus, and the like. Our initial work in this area was reported in Nature 252, October 18, 1974 [4], and reprinted in the IEEE Communications Society 13, January 1975. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 As observed in the laboratory, the basic phenomenon appears to cover a range of subjective experiences variously referred to in the literature as autoscopy (in the medical literature); exteriorization or disassocia- tion (psychological literature); simple clairvoyance, traveling clairvoy- ance, or out-of-body experience (parapsychological literature); or astral projection (occult literature). We choose the term "remote viewing" as a neutral, descriptive term free from prior associations and bias as to mechanisms. The development at SRI of a successful experimental procedure to elicit this capability has evolved to the point where: persons such as visiting government scientists and contract monitors, without any previous exposure to such concepts have leared to perform well; and sub- jects who have trained over a one-year period have performed excellently under a variety of experimental conditions. Our accumulated data thus indicate that both specially selected and unselected persons can be as- sisted in developing remote perceptual abilities up to a level of useful information transfer. In experiments of this type, we have three principal findings. First, we have established that it is possible to obtain significant amounts of accurate descriptive information about remote locations. Second, an in- crease in the distance from a few meters up to 4000 km separating the sub- ject from the scene to be perceived does not in any apparent way degrade the quality or accuracy of perception. Finally, the use of Faraday cage electrical shielding does not prevent high-quality descriptions from being obtained. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 To build a coherent theory for the explanation of these phenomena, it is necessary to have a clear understanding of what constitutes the phenomena. In this paper, we first briefly summarize previous efforts in this field in Section II. We then present in Sections III and IV the results of a series of more than 50 experiments with nine subjects carried out in our own laboratory, which represent a sufficiently stable data base to permit testing of various hypotheses concerning the functioning of this channel. Finally, in Section V we indicate those areas of physics and information theory that appear to be relevant to an understanding of cer- tain aspects of the phenomena. First, however, we present an illustrative example generated in an early pilot experiment. As will be clear from our later discussion, this is not a "best-ever" example, but rather a typical sample of the level of proficiency that can be reached and that we have come to expect in our research. Three subjects participated in a long-distance experiment focusing on a series of targets in Costa Rica. These subjects said they had never been to Costa Rica. In this experiment, one of the experimenters (Dr. Puthoff) spent ten days traveling through Costa Rica on a combination business/pleasure trip. This information was all that was known to the subjects about the traveler's itinerary. The experiment called for Dr. Puthoff to keep a detailed record of his location and activities, includ- ing photographs of each of seven target days at 1330 PDT. A total of twelve Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 daily descriptions were collected before the traveler's return: six re- sponses from one subject, five from another, and one from a third. The third subject who submitted the single response supplied a draw- ing for a day in the middle of the series. (The subject's response, to- gether with the photographs taken at the site, are shown in Figure 1.) Although Costa Rica is a mountainous country, the subject unexpectedly perceived the traveler at a beach and ocean setting. With some misgiving, he described an airport on a sandy beach and an airstrip with the ocean at the end (correct). An airport building also was drawn, and shown to have a large rectangular overhang (correct). The traveler had taken a one-day, unplanned side-trip to an offshore island and at the time of the experiment had just disembarked from a plane at a small island airport as described by the subject 4000 km away. The sole discrepancy was that the subject's drawing showed a Quonset-hut type of building in place.of the rectangular structure. The above description was chosen as an example to illustrate a major point observed a number of times throughout the program to be described. Contrary to what may be expected, a subject's description does not neces- sarily portray what may reasonably be expected to be correct (an educated or "safe" guess), but often runs counter even to the subject's, own expec- tations. We wish to stress again that a result such as the above is not unusual. The remaining submissions in this experiment provided further examples of Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 AIRPORT IN SAN ANDRES, COLOMBIA, USED AS REMOTE VIEWING TARGET FIGURE 1 AIRPORT IN SAN ANDRES, COLOMBIA, USED AS REMOTE VIEWING TARGET ALONG WITH SKETCH PRODUCED BY SUBJECT IN CALIFORNIA Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 excellent correspondences between target and response. (A target period of poolside relaxation was identified; a drive through a tropical forest at the base of a truncated volcano was described as a drive through a jungle below a large bare table mountain; a hotel room target description, including such details as rug color, was correct; and so on.) So as to determine whether such matches were simply fortuitous--that is, could reasonably be expected on the basis of chance alone, Dr. Puthoff was asked after he had returned to blind match the twelve descriptions to his seven target locations. On the basis of this conservative evaluation procedure, which vastly underestimates the statistical significance of the individual descriptions, five correct matches were obtained. This number of matches is significant at p = 0.02 by exact binomial calculation.* The observation of such unexpectedly high-quality descriptions early in our program.led to a large-scale study of the phenomenon at SRI under secure, double-blind conditions (i.e., target unknown to experimenters as well as subjects), with independent random target selection and blind judg- ing. The results, presented in Sections III and IV, provide strong evi- dence for the robustness of this phenomenon whereby a human perceptual modality of extreme sensitivity can detect complex remote stimuli. The probability of a correct daily match by chance for any given tran- script is p = 1/7. Therefore, the probability of at least five correct matches by chance out of twelve tries can be calculated from 12-i) 12 12: ( p = E 0.02 7 i=5 is(12-i)! (0'_ Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 II BACKGROUND Although we are approaching the study of these phenomena as physicists, it is not yet possible to separate ourselves entirely from the language of the nineteenth century when the laboratory study of the paranormal was begun. Consequently, we continue to use terms such as "paranormal, "telep- athy," and the like. However, we intend only to indicate a process of information transfer under conditions generally accepted as secure against such transfer and with no prejudice or occult assumptions as to the mecha- nisms involved. As in any other scientific pursuit, the purpose is to collect the observables that result from experiments and to try to deter- mine the functional relationships between these observables and the laws of physics as they are currently understood. Organized research into so-called psychic functioning began roughly in the time of J. J. Thomson, Sir Oliver Lodge, and Sir William Crookes, all of whom took part in the founding of the Society for Psychical Research (SPR) in 1882 in England. Crookes, for example, carried out his principal investigations with D. D. Home, a Scotsman who grew up in America and re- turned to England in 1855 [3]. According to the notebooks and published reports of Crookes, Home had demonstrated the ability to cause objects to move without touching them. We should note in passing that, Home, unlike most subjects, worked only in the light and spoke out in the strongest possible terms against the darkened seance rooms popular at the time [5]. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Sir William Crookes was a pioneer in the study of electrical discharge in gases and in the development of vacuum tubes, some types of which still bear his name. Although everything Crookes said about electron beams and plasmas was accepted, nothing he said about the achievements of D. D. Home ever achieved that status. Many of his colleagues, who had not observed the experiments with Home, stated publicly that they thought Crookes had been deceived, to which Crookes angrily responded; Will not my critics give me credit for some amount of common sense'? Do they not imagine that the obvious precautions, which occur to them as soon as they sit down to pick holes in my ex- periments, have occurred to me also in the course of my prolonged and patient investigation? The answer to this, as to all other objections is, prove it to be an error, by showing where the error lies, or if a trick, by showing how the trick is performed. Try the experiment fully and fairly. If then fraud be found, expose it; if it be a truth, proclaim it. This is the only scientific procedure, and it is that I propose steadily to pursue [3]. In the United States, scientific interest in the paranormal was cen- tered in the universities. In 1912 John Coover [6] was established in the endowed Chair of Psychical Research at Stanford University. In the 1920s, Harvard University set up research programs with George Estabrooks and L. T. Troland [7],[8]. It was in this framework that in 1930 William McDougall invited Drs. J. B. and Louisa Rhine to join the Psychology Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Department at Duke University [9]. For more than thirty years, significant work was carried out at Rhine's Duke University Laboratory. To examine the existence of paranormal perception, he used the now-famous ESP cards containing a boldly printed picture of a star, cross, square, circle, or wavy lines. Subjects were asked to name the order of these cards in a freshly shuffled deck of twenty-five such cards. To test for telepathy, an experimenter would look at the cards one at a time, and a subject suit- ably separated from the sender would attempt to determine which card was being viewed. Dr. Rhine together with Dr. J. G. Pratt carried out thousands of ex- periments of this type under widely varying conditions [10]. The statis- tical results from these experiments indicated that some individuals did indeed possess a paranormal perceptual ability in that it was possible to obtain an arbitrarily high degree of improbability by continued testing of a gifted subject. The work of Rhine has been challenged on many grounds, however, includ- ing accusations of improper handling of statistics, error, and fraud. With regard to the statistics, the general consensus of statisticians today is that if fault is to be found in Rhine's work, it would have to be on other than statistical grounds [11]. With regard to the accusations of fraud, the most celebrated case of criticism of Rhine's work, that of G. R. Price [12], ended 17 years after it began when the accusation of fraud was re- tracted by its author in an article entitled "Apology to Rhine and Soal," Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 published in the same journal in which it was first put forward [13]. It should also be noted that parapsychological researchers themselves recently exposed fraud in their own laboratory when they encountered it [14]. At the end of the 1940s, Professor S. G. Soal, an English mathemati- cian working with the SPR, had carried out hundreds of card guessing exper- iments involving tens of thousands of calls [15]. Many of these experi- ments were carried out over extended distances. One of the most notable experiments was conducted with Mrs. Gloria Stewart between London and Antwerp. This experiment gave results whose probability of occurring by chance were less than 10-8. With the publication of Modern Experiments in Telepathy by Soal and Bateman (both of whom were statisticians), it appeared that card guessing experiments produced significant results, on the average.* The most severe criticism of all this work, a criticism difficult to defend against in principle, is that leveled by the well-known British parapsychological critic C.E.M. Hansel [17], who began his examination of the ESP hypothesis with the stated assumption, "In view of the a priori arguments against it we know in advance that telepathy, etc., cannot oc- cur.'", Therefore, based on the "a priori unlikelihood" of ESP, Hansel's Recently, some of the early Soal experiments have been criticized [16]. However, his long-distance experiments cited here were judged in a double- blind fashion of the type that escaped the criticism of the early experi- ments. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 examination of the literature centered primarily on the possibility of fraud, by subjects or investigators. He reviewed in depth four experi- ments which he regarded as providing the best evidence of ESP: the Pearce- Pratt distance series [18]; the Pratt-Woodruff [19] series, both conducted at Duke; and Soal's work with Mrs. Stewart and Basil Shackleton [15], as well as a more recent series by Soal and Bowden [20]. Hansel showed, in each case, how fraud could have been committed (by the experimenters in the Pratt-Woodruff and Soal-Bateman series, or by the subjects in the Pearce- Pratt and Soal-Bowden experiments). He gave no direct evidence that fraud was committed in these experiments, but said, "If the result could have arisen through a trick, the experiment must be considered unsatisfactory proof of ESP, whether or not it is finally decided that such a trick was in fact used" [17, p. 18]. As discussed by Honorton in a review of the field [21], Hansel's conclusion after 241 pages of careful scrutiny there- fore was that these experiments were not "fraud-proof" and therefore in principle could not serve as conclusive proof of ESP. Even among the supporters of ESP research and its results, there re- mained the consistent problem that many successful subjects eventually lost their ability and their scores gradually drifted toward chance results. This decline effect in no way erased their previous astronomical success; but it was a disappointment since if paranormal perception is a natural ability, one would like to see subjects improving with practice rather than getting worse. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 One of the first successful attempts to overcome the decline effect was in Czechoslovakia in the work of Dr. Milan Ryzl, a chemist with the Institute of Biology of the Czechoslovakian Academy of Science and also an amateur hypnotist [22]. Through the use of hypnosis, together with feedback and reinforcement, he developed several outstanding subjects, one of whom, Pavel Stepanek, has worked with experimenters around the world for more than ten years. Ryzl's pioneering work came as an answer to the questions raised by the 11956 CIBA Foundation conference on extrasensory perception. The CIBA Chemical Company has annual meetings on topics of biological and chemical interest, and that. same year they assembled several prominent para- psychologists to have a state-of-the-art conference on ESP [23]. The con- ference concluded that little progress would be made in parapsychology re- search until a repeatable experiment could be found; namely, an experiment that different experimenters could repeat at will and that would reliably yield a statistically significant result. Ryzl had by 1962 accomplished that goal. His primary contribution was a decision to interact with the subject as a person, to try to build up his confidence and ability. His protocol depended on "working with" rather than "running" his subjects. Ryzl's star subject, Pavel Stepanek, has produced highly significant results with many contemporary researchers [24]'-[29]. In these experiments, he was able to tell with sixty percent reliability whether a hidden card was green side or white side up, yield- ing'statistics of a million to one with only a thousand trials. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 As significant as such results are statistically, the information channel is imperfect, containing noise along with the signal. When con- sidering how best to use such a channel, one is led to the communication theory concept of the introduction of redundancy as a means of coding a message to combat the effects of a noisy channel [30]. A prototype experi- ment by Ryzl using such techniques has proved to be successful. Ryzl had an assistant select randomly five groups of three digits each. These fif- teen digits were then encoded into binary form and translated into a se- quence of green and white cards in sealed envelopes. By means of repeated calling and an elaborate majority vote protocol, Ryzl was able after 19,350 calls by Stepanek (averaging nine seconds per call) to correctly identify all fifteen numbers, a result significant at p = 10-15. The hit rate for individual calls was 61.9 percent, 11,978 hits, and 7,372 misses [31].* The characteristics of such a channel can be specified in accordance with the precepts of communication theory. The bit rate associated with the information channel is calculated from [30] R = H(x) - H (x) y (1) where H(x) is the uncertainty of the source message containing symbols with a priori probability p, i x Note added in proof. It has just come to our attention that a similar procedure was used to transmit without error the word "peace" in Inter- national Morse Code: J. C. Carpenter, "Toward the Effective Utilization of Enhanced Weak-Signal ESP Effects," presented at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Advancement of Science, New York, N.Y. (January 27, 1975). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 H(x) = Pi log2 Pi i=1 (2) and H (x) is the conditional entropy based on the a posteriori probabil- y ities that a received signal was actually transmitted, 2 H y (x) L P(i,j) log2 Pi (j) i, j=1 (3) For Stepanek's run, with pi = 1/2, pj(j) = 0.619, and an average time of nine seconds per choice, we have a source uncertainty H(x) = 1 bit and a calculated bit rate R': 0.041 bits/symbol or R/T ti 0.0046 bits/s [Since the 15-digit number (49.8 bits) actually was transmitted at the rate of 2.9 x 10-CE bits/s, an increase in bit rate by a factor of about 20 could be expected on the basis of a coding scheme more optimum than that used in the experiments. (See, for example, Appendix A.)] Dr. Charles Tart at the University of California has written exten- sively on the so-called decline effect. He considers that having subjects attempt to guess cards, or perform any other repetitious task for which they receive no feedback, follows the classical technique for decondition- ing any response. He thus considers card guessing "a technique for extin- guishing psychic functioning in the laboratory" [321. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Tart's injunctions of the mid-sixties were being heeded at Maimonides Hospital, Brooklyn, New York, by a team of researchers that included Dr. Montague Ullman, who was director of research for the hospital; Dr. Stanley Krippner; and, later, Charles Honorton. These three worked together for several years on experiments on the occurrence of telepathy in dreams. In the course of a half-dozen experimental series, they found in their week-long sessions a number of subjects who had dreams that consistently were highly descriptive of pictorial material that a remote sender was looking at throughout the night. This work is described in detail in the experimenters' book Dream Telepathy [33]. Honorton is continuing work of this free-response type in which the subject has no preconceived idea'as to what the target may be. In his more recent work with subjects in the waking state, Honorton is providing homogeneous stimulation to the subject who is to describe color slides viewed by another person in a remote room. In this new work, the subject listens to white noise via earphones and views an homogeneous visual field imposed through the use of Ping-Pong ball halves to cover the subject's eyes in conjunction with diffuse ambient illumination. In this so-called Ganzfeld setting, subjects are again able, now in the waking state, to give correct and often highly accurate descriptions of the ma- terial being viewed by the sender [34]. In Honorton's work and elsewhere it apparently has been the step away from the repetitive, forced-choice experiment that has opened the way for Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 a wide variety of ordinary people to demonstrate significant functioning in, the laboratory, without being bored into a decline effect. This survey would be incomplete if we did not indicate certain aspects of the current state of research in the USSR. It is clear from trans- lated documents and other sources that many laboratories in the USSR are engaged in paranormal research [35]. Since the 1930s in the laboratory of L. Vasiliev (Leningrad Insti- tute for Brain Research), there has been an interest in the use of tel- ep4thy as a method of influencing the behavior of a person at a distance. In Vasiliev's book Experiments in Mental Suggestion, he makes is very clear that the bulk of his laboratory's experiments were aimed at long- distance communication combined with a form of behavior modification; for example, putting people at a distance to sleep through hypnosis [36]. Similar behavior modification types of experiments have been carried out in recent times by I.M. Kogan, Chairman of the Bioinformation Section of the Moscow Board of the Popov Society. He is a Soviet engineer who un- til 1969 published extensively on the theory of telepathic communication [36]-[40]. He was concerned with three principal kinds of experiments: mental suggestion, without hypnosis over short distances, in which the per- cipient attempts to identify an object; mental awakening over short distances, in which a subject is awakened from a hypnotic sleep at the "beamed" sug- gestion from the hypnotist; and long-range (intercity) telepathic communica- tion. Kogan's main interest has been to quantify the channel capacity of Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 the paranormal channel. He finds that the bit rate decreases from 0.1 bits/s for laboratory experiments to 0.005 bits/s for his 1000-km inter- city experiments. In the USSR, serious consideration is given to the hypothesis that telepathy is mediated by extremely low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic propagation. (The pros and cons of this hypothesis are discussed in Section V of this paper.) In general, the entire field of paranormal research in the USSR is part of a larger one concerned with the interac- tion between electromagnetic fields and living organisms [41],[42]. At the First International Congress on Parapsychology and Psychotronics in Prague, Czechoslovakia in 1973, for example, Kholodov spoke at length about the susceptibility of living systems to extremely low-level ac and do fields. He described conditioning effects on the behavior of fish resulting from the application of 10 to 100 ?W of RF to their tank [43]. The USSR take these data seriously in that the Soviet safety requirements for steady- state microwave exposure set limits at 10 4W/cm2, whereas the United States has set a steady-state limit of 10 mW/cm2 [44]. Kholodov spoke also about the nonthermal effects of microwaves on animals' central nervous systems. His experiments were very carefully carried out and are characteristic of a new dimension in paranormal research. The increasing importance of this area in Soviet research was indi- cated recently when the Soviet Psychological Association issued an unprece- dented position paper calling on the Soviet Academy of Sciences to step up Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 efforts in this area [45]. They recommended that the newly formed Psycho- logical Institute within the Soviet Academy of Sciences and the Psycholog- ical Institute of the Academy of Pedagogical Sciences review the area and consider the creation of a new laboratory within one of the institutes to study persons with unusual abilities. They also recommended a comprehen- siveevaluation of experiments and theory by the Academy of Sciences' Institute of Biophysics and Institute for the Problems of Information Transmission. The Soviet research, along with other behavioristically oriented work, suggests that in addition to obtaining overt responses such as verbalizations or key presses from a subject, it should be possible to obtain objective evidence of information transfer by direct measurement of physiological parameters of a subject. Kamiya, Lindsley, Pribram, Silverman, Walter, and others brought together to discuss physiological methods to detect ESP functioning, have suggested that a whole range of electroencephalogram (EEG) responses such as evoked potentials (EPs), spontaneous EEG,.and the contingent negative variation (CNV) might be sensitive indicators of the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by usual sensory processes [46]. Early experimentation of this type was carried out by Douglas Dean at the Newark College of Engineering. In his search for physiological correlates of information transfer, he used the plethysmograph to measure changes in the blood volume in a finger, a sensitive indicator of autonomic Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 nervous system functioning [47]. A plethysmographic measurement was made on the finger of a subject during telepathy experiments. A sender looked at randomly selected target cards consisting of names known to the subject, together with names unknown to him (selected at random from a telephone book). The names of the known people were contributed by the subject and were to be of emotional significance to him. Dean found significant changes in the chart recording of finger blood volume when the remote sender was looking at those names known to the subject as compared with those names randomly chosen. Three other experiments using the physiological approach have now been published. The first work by Tart [48], a later work by Lloyd [49], and most recently the work by the authors [4] all follow a similar procedure. Basically, a subject is closeted in an electrically shielded room while his EEG is recorded. Meanwhile, in another laboratory, a second person is stimulated from time to time, and the time of that stimulus is marked on the magnetic tape recording of the subject's EEG. The subject does not know when the remote stimulus periods are as compared with the non- stimulus periods. With regard to choice of stimulus for our own experimentation, we noted that in previous work others had attempted, without success, to detect evoked potential changes in a subject's EEG in response to a single stroboscopic flash stimulus observed by another subject [50]. In a discus- sion of that experiment, Kamiya suggested that because of the unknown Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 temporal characteristics of the information channel, it might be more appropriate to use repetitive bursts of light to increase the probability of detecting information transfer [51]. Therefore, in our study we chose to use a stroboscopic flash train of 10-s duration as the remote stimulus. In the design of the study, we assumed that the application of the remote stimulus would result in responses similar to those obtained under conditions of direct stimulation. For example, when an individual is stim- ulated with a low-frequency (H I- J 0 0 U DECISION 1 Accept "1" as the Bit Being Transmitted DECISION 2 the Bit Being 20 30 40 50 60 NUMBER OF TRIALS FIGURE A-1 ENHANCEMENT OF SIGNAL-TO-NOISE RATIO BY SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING PROCEDURE (p0 = 0.4, pt = 0.6, a = 0.01, a = 0.01) Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 El = d1 + SN, E 0 = -d0 + SN, d 0 log p1 1-P0 log- p0 1-P1 pl 1-P0 log- p0 1-P1 S is the slope N is the number of trials d1 and d0 are the y axis intercepts. A cumulative record of receiver-generated responses to the target bit is compiled until either the upper or lower limit line is reached, at which point a decision is made to accept 0 or 1 as the bit being transmitted. Channel reliability (probability of correctly determining message being transmitted) as a function of operative psi parameter * is plotted Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 in Figure A-2. As observed, the sequential sampling procedure can result in 90 percent or greater reliability with psi parameters on the order of a few percent. Implementation of the sequential sampling procedure requires the transmission of a message coded in binary digits. Therefore, the target space must consist of dichotomous elements such as the white and green cards used in the experiments by Ryz1. In operation, a sequence corresponding to the target bit (0 or 1) is sent and the cumulative entries are made (Figure A-1) until a decision is reached to accept either a 1 or 0 as the bit being transmitted. At a prearranged time, the next sequence is begun and continues as above until the entire message has been received. A useful alternative, which relieves the percipient of the burden of being aware of his self- contradiction from trial to trial, consists of cycling through the entire message repetitively and entering each response on its associated graph until a decision has been reached on all message bits. The authors have used this technique successfully in a pilot study, but a discussion of this would take us beyond the intended scope of this paper. From the results obtained in such experiments, the channel bit rate can be ascertained for the system configuration under consideration. Furthermore, bit rates for other degrees of reliability (i.e., for other p0, pl, cx, and p) can be estimated by construction of other decision Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 ? c 0.8 0 I I I I I I N I 1 1 +0.4 +0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4 ' (psi parameter) FIGURE A-2 RELIABILITY CURVE FOR SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING PROCEDURE (po = 0.4, p, = 0.6, a = 0.01, 0 = 0.01) Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 curves over the same data base and thus provide a measure of the bit rate per degree of reliability. In summary, the procedures described here can provide a specification of the characteristics of a remote sensing channel under well-defined conditions. These procedures also provide for a determination of the feasibility of such a channel for particular applications. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Appendix B REMOTE VIEWING TRANSCRIPT Following is the unedited transcript of the first experiment with an SRI volunteer (S6), a mathematician in the computer science laboratory, with no previous experience in remote viewing. The target, determined by random procedure, was White's Plaza, a plaza with fountain at Stanford University (shown in Figure 8). The capital letters correspond to the experimenter's statements and questions. As is our standard protocol, the experimenter with the subject is kept ignorant of the specific target visited as well as of the contents of the target pool. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 TODAY IS MONDAY, OCTOBER 7TH. IT IS 11:00 AND THIS IS A REMOTE VIEWING EXPERIMENT WITH RUSS TARG, PHYLLIS COLE, AND HAL PUTHOFF. IN THIS EX- PERIMENT HAL WILL DRIVE TO A REMOTE SITE CHOSEN BY A RANDOM PROCESS. PHYLLIS COLE WILL BE THE REMOTE VIEWER, AND RUSS TARG IS THE MONITOR. WE EXPECT THIS EXPERIMENT TO START AT TWENTY MINUTES AFTER ELEVEN AND RUN FOR FIFTEEN MINUTES. IS IS JUST ABOUT TWENTY MINUTES AFTER ELEVEN AND HAL SHOULD BE AT HIS TARGET LOCATION BY NOW. WHY'DON'T YOU TELL ME WHAT KIND OF PICTURES YOU SEE AND WHAT YOU THINK HE MIGHT BE DOING OR EXPERIENCING. The first thing that came to mind was some sort of a large, square kind of a shape. Like Hal was in front of it. It was a ... not a building or something, it was a square. I don't know if it was a window, but some- thing like that so that the bottom line of it was not at the ground. About where his waist was, at least. That's what it seemed to me. It seems outdoors somehow. Tree. DOES HAL SEEM TO BE LOOKING AT THAT SQUARE? I don't know. The first impression was that he wasn't, but I have a sense that whatever it was was something one might look at. I don't know if it would be a sign, but something that one might look at. CAN YOU TELL IF :CT IS ON THE GROUND OR VERTICAL? It seemed vertical. I don't have a sense that it was part of anything particular. It might be on a building or part of a building, but I don't know. There was a Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 tree outside, but I also got the impression of cement. I don't have the impression of very many people or traffic either. I have the sense that he is sort of walking back and forth. I don't have any more explicit picture than that. CAN YOU MOVE INTO WHERE HE IS STANDING AND TRY TO SEE WHAT HE IS LOOKING AT? I picked up he was touching something--something rough. Maybe warm and rough. Something possibly like cement. IT IS TWENTY-FOUR MINUTES AFTER ELEVEN. CAN YOU CHANGE YOUR POINT OF VIEW AND MOVE ABOVE THE SCENE SO YOU CAN GET A BIGGER PICTURE OF WHAT'S THERE? I still see some trees and some sort of pavement or something like that. Might be a courtyard. The thing that came to mind was it might be one of the plazas at Stanford campus or something like that, cement. Some kinds of landscaping. I said Stanford campus when I started to see some things in White Plaza, but I think that is misleading. I have the sense that he's not moving around too much. That it's in a small area. I guess I'll go ahead and say it, but I'm afraid I'm just putting on my impressions from Stanford campus. I had the impression of a fountain. There are two in the plaza, and it seemed that Hal was possibly near the, what they call Mem Claw. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 WHAT IS THAT? It's a fountain that looks rather like a claw. It's a black sculpture. And it has benches around it made of cement. ARE THERE ANY BUILDINGS AT THE PLACE YOU ARE LOOKING AT? ARE THERE ANY BUILDINGS? YOU DESCRIBED A KIND OF A COURTYARD. USUALLY AT SOME PLACES THERE SHOULD BE A BUILDING, LARGE OR SMALL THAT THE COURTYARD IS ABOUT. LOOK AT THE END OR THE SIDES OF THE COURTYARD. IS THERE ANYTHING TO BE SEEN? I have a sense that there are buildings. It's not solid buildings. I mean there are some around the periphery and I have a sense that none of them are very tall. Maybe mostly one story, maybe an occasional two story one. DO YOU HAVE ANY BETTER IDEA OF WHAT YOUR SQUARE WAS THAT YOU SAW AT THE OUT SET? No. I could hazard different kinds of guesses. DOES IT SEEM PART OF THIS SCENE? It ... I think it could be. It could almost be a bulletin board or some- thing with notices on it maybe. Or something that people are expected to look at. Maybe a window with things in it that. people were expected to look at. WHAT KIND OF TREES DO YOU SEE IN THIS PLACE? I don't know what: kind they are. The impression was that they were shade trees and not terribly big. Maybe 12 feet of trunk and then a certain Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 amount of branches above that. So that the branches have maybe a 12 foot diameter, or something. Not real big trees. NEW TREES RATHER THAN OLD TREES? Yeah, maybe 5 or 10 years old, but not real old ones. IS THERE ANYTHING INTERESTING ABOUT THE PAVEMENT? No. It seems to be not terribly new or terribly old. Not very interesting. There seems to be some bits of landscaping around. Little patches of grass around the edges and peripheries. Maybe some flowers. But, not lush. YOU SAW SOME BENCHES. DO YOU WANT TO TELL ME ABOUT THEM? Well, that's my unsure feeling about this fountain. There was some kind of benches of cement. Curved benches, it felt like. They were of rough cement. WHAT DO YOU THINK HAL IS DOING WHILE HE IS THERE? I have a sense that he is looking at things trying to project them. Looking at different things and sort of walking back and forth not cover- ing a whole lot of territory. Sometimes standing still while he looks around. I just had the impression of him talking, and I almost sense that it was being recorded or something. I don't know if he has a tape recorder, but if it's not that, then he is saying something because it needed to be remembered. IT'S 11:33. HE'S JUST PROBABLY GETTING READY TO COME BACK. Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 1. J. R. Smythies, ed., Science and ESP (Routledge and Kegan Paul, London, England, 1967). 2. C. Evans, "parapsychology--what the questionnaire revealed," New Scientist, pp. 209 (January 25, 1973). 3. Alan Gauld, The founders of psychical research (Schocken Books, New York, New York, 1968). See also Wm. Crookes, Researches in the phenomena of spiritualism, J. Burns, London, England, 1874. 4. R. Targ and H. Puthoff, "Information transmission under conditions of sensory shielding," Nature 252, 5476, pp. 602-607 (October 18, 1974). 5. D. D. Home, Lights and shadows of spiritualism (G. W. Carleton, New York, New York, 1877). 6. J. Coover, Experiments in psychical research (Stanford University Press, Palo Alto, California, 1917). 7. G. Estabrooks, Bulletin of the Boston Society for Psychical Research (1927) [See also reference 12, pp. 18-19.]. 8. L. T. Troland, Techniques for the experimental study of telepathy and other alleged clairvoyant processes (Albany, New York, 1928). 9. J. B. Rhine, New frontiers of the mind (Farrar and Rinehart, New York, New York, 1937). 10. J. Pratt and J. B. Rhine, et al., Extra-sensory perception after sixty years (Henry Holt, 1940). 11. C. Scott, "G. Spencer Brown and probability: A critique." J. of the Society for Psychical Research 39, pp. 217-234 (1958). 12. G. R. Price, "Science and the supernatural," Science 122, pp. 359- 367 (1955). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 13. G. R. Price, "Apology to Rhine and Soal," Science 175, p. 359 (1972). 14. J. B. Rhine, "A new case of experimenter unreliability," J. Para- psychology 38, pp. 215-225 (June 1974). 15. S. G. Soal and F. Bateman, Modern experiments in telepathy (Faber and Faber, London, England, 1953). 16. C. Scott and P. Haskell, "'Normal' explanation of the Soal-Goldney experiments in extra-sensory perception," Nature, 245, pp. 52-54 (September 7, 1973). 17. C.E.M. Hansel, ESP--A scientific evaluation (Charles Scribners, New York, New York, 1966). 18. J. B. Rhine and J. G. Pratt, "A review of the Pearce-Pratt distance series of ESP tests," J. of Parapsychology 18, pp. 165-177 (1954). 19. J. G. Pratt and J. L. Woodruff, "Size of stimulus symbols in extra- sensory perception," J. of Parapsychology 3, pp. 121-158 (1939). 20. S. G. Soal and H. T. Bowden, The mind readers: recent experiments in telepathy (Yale University Press, New Haven, Conn., 1954). 21. C. Honorton, "Error Some Place!" J. of Communication 25:1 (The Annenberg School of Communication, Winter 1975). 22. M. Ryzl, "Training the psi faculty by hypnosis," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 41, pp. 234-251 (1962). 23. CIBA foundation symposium on extra sensory perception (Little Brown, Boston, Massachusetts, 1956). 24. M. Ryzl and J. Pratt, "A repeated-calling ESP test with sealed cards," J. Parapsychology, vol. 27, pp. 161-174 (1963). 25. M. Ryzl and J. Pratt, "A further confirmation of stabilized ESP per- formance in a selected subject," J. Parapsychology, vol. 27, pp. 73- 83 (1963). 26. J. Pratt, "Preliminary experiments with a 'borrowed' ESP subject," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 42, pp. 333-345 (1964). 27. J. Pratt and J. Blom, "A confirmatory experiment with 'borrowed' outstanding ESP subject," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 42, pp. 381-388 (1964). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 28. W. G. Roll and J. G. Pratt, "An ESP test with aluminum targets," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 62, pp. 381-387 (1968). 29. J. Pratt, "A decade of research with a selected ESP subject: an overview and reappraisal of the work with Pavel Stepanek," Proc. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 30 (1973). 30. C. Shannon and W. Weaver, The mathematical theory of communication (University of Illinois Press, Urbana, Illinois, 1949). 31. M. Ryzl, "A model for parapsychological communication," J. Para- psychology, vol. 30, 18-31 (March 1966). 32. C. Tart, "Card guessing tests: learning paradigm or extinction paradigm," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 60, p. 46 (1966). 33. M. Ullman, S. Krippner, with A. Vaughan, Dream telepathy (The Mac- millan Company, New York, New York, 1973). 34. C. Honorton, "State of awareness factors in psi activation," J. Amer. Soc. Psych. Res., vol. 68, pp. 246-257 (1974). 35. Proc. 2nd Int'l Congress on Psychotronic Research, Monte Carlo. Int'l Assoc. for Psychotronic Research, Cotati, CA (1975). 36. L. L. Vasiliev, Experiments in mental suggestion (ISMI Publications, Hampshire, England, 1963). 37. I. M. Kogan, "Is telepathy possible?" Radio Engineering, 21, No. 1, p. 75 (January 1966). 38. I. M. Kogan, "Telepathy, hypotheses and observations," Radio Engineering, 22, No. 1, p. 141 (January 1967). 39. I. M. Kogan, "Information theory analysis of telepathic communication experiments," Radio Engineering, 23, No. 3, p. 122 (March 1968). 40. I. M. Kogan, "The information theory aspect of telepathy," RAND publication, p. 4145, Santa Monica, California (July 1969). 41. A. S. Presman, Electromagnetic fields and life (Plenum Press, New York, New York, 1970). 42. Y. A. Kholodov, ed., Influence of magnetic fields on biological objects, JPRS 63038, NTIS, Springfield, Virginia (September 24, 1974). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 43. Y. A. Kholodov, "Investigation of the direct effect of magnetic fields on the central nervous system," Proceedings of the first Conference on Psychotronic Research, JPRS L/5022-1 and 2 (September 6, 1974). 44. D. Mennie, "Consumer electronics," IEEE Spectrum, pp. 34-35 (March 1975). 45. W. P. Zinchenko, A. N. Leontiev, B. M. Lomov, and A. R. Luria, "Parapsychology: fiction or reality?," Questions of philosophy, 9 pp. 128-136 (1973). 46. Roberto Cavanna, ed., Proceedings of an international conference on methodology in psi research (Parapsychology Foundation, New York New York, 1970). 47. E. Douglas Dean, "Plethysmograph recordings as ESP responses," Int'l. J. Neuropsychiatry, vol. 2 (September 1966). 48. C. Tart, "Physiological correlates of psi cognition," Int. J. Para- psychology, No. 4 (1963). 49. D. H. Lloyd, "Objective events in the brain correlating with psychic phenomena," New Horizons, vol. 1, No. 2 (Summer 1973). 50. J. Silverman and M. S. Buchsbaum, "Perceptual correlates of con- sciousness; a conceptual model and its technical implications for psi research," in: R. Cavanna, ed., Psi favorable states of con- sciousness, pp. 143-169 (Parapsychology Foundation, New York, New York, 1970). 51. J. Kamiya, "Comment to Silverman and Buchsbaum," in: R. Cavanna, ed., Psi favorable states of consciousness, pp. 158-159 (Parapsychology Foundation, New York, New York, 1.970). 52. D. Hi:Ll and C. Parr, Electroencephalography. A symposium on its various aspects (The Macmillan Company, New York, New York, 1963). 53. T. D. Duane and T. Behrendt, "Extrasensory electroencephalographic induction between identical twins," Science, 150, 367 (1965). 54. K. Osis, ASPR Newsletter, No. 14 (1972). 55. R. L. Morris, "An exact method for evaluating preferentially matched free-response material," Jour. ASPR, 66, No. 4, p. 401 -(October 1972). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 56. G. R. Schmeidler, "PK effects upon continuously recorded temperatures," J. Am. Soc. Psychical Res., vol. 67, No. 4 (October 1973). 57. W. Scherer, "Spontaneity as a factor in ESP," Jour. ASPR, 12, pp. 126- 147 (1948). - 58. R. Targ, P. Cole, and H. Puthoff, "Techniques to enhance man/machine communication," final report, NASA Contract NAS7-100, SRI, Menlo Park, California (June 1974). 59. R. Ornstein, The nature of human consciousness, Ch. 7 and 8 (W. H. Freeman and Company, San Francisco, California, 1973). 60. R. W. Sperry, "Cerebral organization and behavior," Science, 133, pp. 1749-1757 (1961). - 61. 0. Bilaniuk and E.C.G. Sudarshan, "Particles beyond the light bar- rier," Physics Today, vol. 22 (5 May 1969). 62. W. Pauli and C. G. Jung, eds., The interpretation of nature and the psyche, Bollingen series LI (Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey, 1955). 63. M. A. Persinger, ELF waves and ESP," New horizons transactions of the Toronto society for psychical research, 1, No. 5 (January 1975). 64. M. A. Persinger, The paranormal Part II: mechanisms and models, M.S.S. Information Corp., New York, New York (1974). 65. B. Julesz, Foundations of cyclopean perception (University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, 1971). 66. J. White, ed., Psychic exploration--A challenge for science, See article by H. Puthoff and R. Targ, pp. 522-542 (G. P. Putnam's Sons) New York, New York, 1974). 67. G. Feinberg, "Precognition--a memory of things future?" Proc. Conf. on Quantum Physics and Parapsychology, Geneva, Switzerland (Para- psychology Foundation, New York, New York, 1975). 68. E. P. Wigner, "The problem of measurement," Am. J. Physics, 31. No. 1, p. 6 (1963). 69. J. J. Freedman and J. F. Clauser, '!Experimental test of local hidden variable theories," Phys. Rev. Letters, 28, No. 14, p. 938 (3 April 1972). - Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6 70. J. F. Clauser and M. A. Horne, "Experimental consequences of objec- tive local theories," Phys. Rev. D, 10, No. 2, p. 526 (15 July 1974). 71. D. Bohm and B. Hiley, "On the intuitive understanding of non-locality as implied by quantum theory," Preprint, Birkbeck College, London, England (February 1974). 72. J. S. Bell, "On the problem of hidden variables in quantum theory," Rev. Modern Physics, 38, No. 3, p. 447 (July 1966) 73. H. Stapp, "Theory of reality," Lawrence-Berkeley Laboratory Report No. LBL-3837, University of California, Berkeley, California (April 1975). 74. A. Einstein, B. Podolsky, and N. Rosen, "Can quantum-mechanical description of physical reality be considered complete?," Phys. Rev. 47, p. 777 (15 May 1935). 75. R. H. Dicke and J. P. Wittke, Introduction to quantum mechanics, Ch. 7 (Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., Inc., Reading, Massachusetts, 1960). 76. E. H. Walker, "Foundations of paraphysical and parapsychological phenomena," Proc. Conf. on Quantum Physics and Parapsychology, Geneva, Switzerland (Parapsychology Foundation, New York, New Hork, 1975). 77. 0. Costa de Beauregard, "Time symmetry and interpretation of quantum mechanics," Foundations of Physics (in press), Lecture delivered at the Boston Colloqium for the Philosophy of Science, February 1974. 78. J. A. Stratton, Electromagnetic theory (McGraw-Hill Book Company, New York, New York, 1941). 75. A. Sinkov, Elementary cryptanalysis--a mathematical approach (Random House, New York, New York, 1968). 80. P. Hoel, Introduction to mathematical statistics, 2nd edition, p. 27 John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York, New York, 1954). 81. R. Taetzsch, "Design of a psi communications system," Intern'l Jour. of Parapsychology, 4, No. 1, p. 35 (Winter 1962). Approved For Release 2002/05/17 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000500020001-6