SUMMARY AND CRITICAL EVALUATION OF RESEARCH IN REMOTE VIEWING (PART 2)
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
S
Document Page Count:
119
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
June 11, 1998
Sequence Number:
3
Case Number:
Publication Date:
June 1, 1979
Content Type:
SUMMARY
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2.pdf | 5.19 MB |
Body:
3-2
SECRET
SUMMARY AND CRITICAL EVALUATION
OF
RESEARCH IN REMOTE VIEWING
by
Report RV-79-1
Copy Number r
(Part 2)
C)ER1VA a mVE GL V _ 1 r
D, :fivLu FF O I
SECRET
595temetric5
Approved Fg sfeme?ras~~fn ., o i e id r i fl 9996V0260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Page
PART 1
1. INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Background . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Purpose . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
Approach . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
II. SUMMARY OF SRI STUDIES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
A. Project SCANATE: Long Distance Remote Viewing by
Geographical Coordinates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
Virginia Site . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23
Urals Site . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
Island Site . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
Project Atlas Site . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27
Sylvania Laser Laboratory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Berkeley Laboratory Bevatron . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
Utah and China Lake . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
USSR Sites . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
Critical Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
B. Long Distance Remote Viewing by Target Person Cueing . . . 38
Costa Rica . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39
Continental U . S . 40
C. Local Targets with Target Person Cueing_ . . . . . . . . . 43
Training with Feedback . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
Demonstration of Ability . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Precognitive Experiments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51
Technology Targets . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
Critical Evaluation, Local Targets . . . . . . . . . . 55
Critical Evaluation, Long Distance Targets . . . . . . 138
PART 2 Critical Evaluation, Remaining Long Distance Targets
with Coordinates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167
D. Alphabet Letter Experiments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 172
E.. Random Stimulus Generator Experiments Critical
Evaluation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 174
F. EEG Experiments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 179
G. Summary of SRI Studies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 180
Positive Characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . 180
Negative Characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
III. DESCRIPTION AND EVALUATION OF NON-SRI.REMOTE-VIEWING
EXPERIMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ... . . . . . . 182
A. Allen, Green, Rucker, Cohen, Goolsby, and Morris (1976) 182
B. Bisaha and Dunne (1977a) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
C. Bisaha and Dunne (1977b) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 197
D. Dunne and Bisaha (1978) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 202
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ?IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787ROO0100260003-2
TABLE OF CONTENTS (continued)
Page
E. Hastings and.Hurt (1976) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 205
F. Jahn (1978) . . . . 210
G. Karnes and Susman (1978) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 213
H. Rauscher, Weissmann, Sarfatti, and Sirag (1976) . . . . . 224
I. Solfvin, Roll, and Krieger (1978) . . . . . . . . . . . . 228
J. Tart (1977) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 232
K. 'Vallee, Hastings, and Askevold . . . . . . . . . . . . . 237
L. Whitson, Bogart, Palmer, and Tart . . . . . . . . . . 245
IV. EVALUATION OF POTENTIAL CRITICISMS. . . . . . . . . . . . . . 253
V. RECOMMENDED RESEARCH IMPROVEMENTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 270
APPENDIX A. SUMMARY AND EVALUATION OF MORRIS' (1972) FREE-
RESPONSE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE . . . . . . . . . . . . 274
APPENDIX B. REMOTE VIEWING PROTOCOL . . . . . . . . . . . 282
Approved For Release 2000/08/161. CIA-RDP96-00787ROO0100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787R000100260003-2
In Rauscher we seem to have another case (like Hastings) of an
experimenter/target person/consultant/author. It is difficult to maintain
acceptance of a "double-blind" protocol under these circumstances.
As with the series of experiments dealing with local targets, there is
a large number of methodological questions raised about the long-distance
with-target-person experiments.
Critical Evaluation: Remaining Long Distance Targets with Coordinates
The remote-viewing experiments involving coordinates rather than a
target person will be discussed under two categories, U.S. and foreign.
These experiments are limited in number and are discussed in one
publication. (Puthoff, et al., 1977a) Two of the three experiments, those
of the Sylvania Laser Lab, Mt. View, California, and the Lawrence Berkeley
Laboratory Bevatron, Berkeley, California, could easily involve cueing.
Targ, who selected the coordinates, had also worked for Sylvania. After
five years of research effort together, it is difficult to believe that
some sort of information did not flow between the two experimenters, and
therefore, this target can hardly be considered "double-blind." No statement
is made as to whether the subject, Il, an experienced remote viewer and
likely one of the six early remote-viewers, co-researcher, and experimenter
has ever been to the target.
The second target, the Lawrence Berkeley Laboratory Bevatron, is also
a choice related to one of the experimenters, Elizabeth Rauscher, who well
may have knowledge of this target. It is stated that the experimenter
with subject, H1, did have prior knowledge of the target. The experimenter
knew the name and general function of the building and although supposedly
167
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
taking precautions to prevent cueing, leading questions have been used
before and could have been used here also. The choice of experimenters is
obviously poor since, besides Rauscher, there are at least four more involved
in the research: Hurt, Targ, Puthoff, and May. Cole may also still be
involved.
The description of the third target, that of the Minuteman and Poseidon
Static Test Firings includes statements such as "'... drawn-out muffled roars'
which 'raised dust clouds' and involved 'glowing melted materials'
...." (p. 75)
could also apply to what could be observed from a distant viewing.
With as
many people involved in the project and with knowledge of the coordinates,
it would not be difficult to have an observer near the spot to report the
actual time of the firings. Two subjects are involved in this experiment,
I1 in Menlo Park who participated in all five experiments and H2 in L.A. who
participated in two. Why did H2 only participate in two?
Previously, more than one reading has been allowed in both the local
and long-distance experiments and the same questions apply to these experiments,
such as: Was there any form of feedback given to the subject at any time
during the experiment? What time period elapsed between first and second
readings?
The foreign targets are an R&D test facility and some Russian sites.
Of the foreign targets, the R&D test facility also appears to have had
many readings. This target, however, is identified to the experimenters not
only by coordinates but also as being an R&D test facility. Two clues are
already available to the experimenters. It is, difficult to estimate how
much could be guessed (or cued to the subject) knowing the sponsor.
168
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
VN'Inrr
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : -00787R000100260003-2
Price is the subject for this experiment. "Figure 1 shows the level
of detail for a sample early effort at drawing a gantry crane he observed ...."
(SRI Progress Report, August 1974, p. 3) Price seems to have been a fairly
decent artist if his sketch of the swimming pool complex is any indication
of his ability. (Mind-Reach, p. 54) Why then the need, in an early drawing,
for such a precise rule-drawn sketch? Was the drawing really his first?
The remaining experiments are Russian-based targets and appear to be
grouped around Moscow. This in itself is an advantage since only this area
would need to be covered with finely detailed maps. Two subjects participate:
I1 and Rl, a .new subject apparently since this letter designation has not
been.used earlier.
The experimenters passed the coordinates to I1, which are of a population
center. The subject, however, described a dam and an airfield. When these
descriptions do not match the actual coordinates, I1 requests an overview of
the area. which now does include the airfield, thus making his description
appear correct. In effect, attention is drawn away from the non-corresponding
elements and drawn to those that do correspond much as a magician does in
his routine. The use of pictures, although for the reader's sake, and the
use of standing at specific spots for judging, quoting only certain elements
of a transcript, are also ways in which this same diversion-of-attention
procedure seems to be used. That this type of behavior is used is not
unremarkable considering that Targ was an accomplished amateur magician.
"Magic tricks fall into two general categories which I will call 'sleights'
and 'illusions.' Sleight-of-hand tricks are simply misdirections of the
observer's attention by. the performer. In magic, as in psychic research,
attention is everything .... However, attention will not help you keep
169
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10,CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
track of the other form of trick, an illusion .... An illusion, ..., is
any trick in which the observer is invited to greatly underestimate the
amount of preparation necessary to create the 'effect.' ... This preparation
may have begun the moment the magician learned that on a particular day in
the next month he will have a meeting with a given person who just might ask
to be shown some magic. And with a month to prepare for a meeting, a
magician can do anything. A magician will spend. that kind of time to prepare
a single trick,-because that's what he does for a living." (Mind-Reach,
pp. 139-:L40)
In the same experiment, E1 is told to search the general coordinate area,
specifically for an airfield. If these instructions were given, they must
have been given following I1's request for an overview of the area or both
were cued to look for such.
The second site was one of no special interest containing a town within
a barren area. Reference to an Atlas indicates this quite easily. Puthoff,
Targ, and May (1977) conclude that this experiment "... provided an opportunity
to verify that (1) the subject's output is not simply geared to match the
expectations of the experimenters ...." [Do they really mean the "expectation
of the experimenters" or should they say, what is expected from those
designating the target?]
(2) the subject does not simply conjure up what may reasonably be
expected to be correct ...." [Here, again why should the subject "expect"
anything?] "... and in fact describes the area appropriate to the coordinates
even though it may run counter even to the subject's own expectations."
As has been stated previously, any knowledge of this area would provide
the information that the subject describes. However, the subject may have
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 a7?CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
orrinrT
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 DP96-007878000100260003-2
been very surprised at this set of coordinates.
The remaining experiment is a ten-site scan about which the above comments
can also apply. The one site mentioned is also centered around Moscow and a
general description of the site could be obtained from a map. Since the more
specific elements have not been verified to us, it is difficult to discuss
this series further.
Puthoff,,Targ, and May use four other studies as verification of their
remote-viewing 'studies. In Mind-Reach, they state: "In the fall of 1974,
we had already concluded most of the experiments described thus far. Our
work was going well. Other laboratories were beginning to replicate our
remote viewing experiments." (p. 90) The first study using their protocol
is that of Rauscher, et al., reported in August 1975. As has been stated
earlier, this author becomes a consultant and experimenter for the SRI team.
This study, however, did not show significant results.
In March 1976, IEEE issused a formal call for papers: "We would
encourage others to repeat their experiments and to report their results,
whether they be positive or negative." (p. 291)
As has been shown earlier, Hastings, also involved in the SRI studies,
and Hurt, also involved, run their experiment in early March (See Table 1.),
although these results are not published until October 1976. Whitson, Bogart,
Palmer, and Tart's paper is also received in late March. Tart has also
been involved in the remote viewing studies: "... the authors have benefited
greatly from their many discussions with ... Dr. Charles Tart ...." (Mind-
Reach, p. viii) Vallee, Hastings, and Askevold's paper is received in early
May. Hastings is also involved in this study in addition to being a co
researcher with Tart in another study. (Tart, 1977, p. 165) Of these,
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 -61A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/ ` ` 96-00787R000100260003-2
three are used to "verify" their data. The remaining study is that of Bisaha
and Dunne, and is not reported until August 1976. Their first study actually
deals with precognition. Their second study is an extension of the precognition
work and was reported in September 1977. These experiments are discussed in
the following section so will not be dealt with here. One should note, however,
that they conducted a series of five precognition experiments between the U. S.
and Czechoslovakia and the U.S.S.R. while one of the experimenters was in
Eastern Europe In August 1976, although their other experiments are confined
to the Chicago area.
In conclusion, the comments made in reference to the judging of transcripts
for the local remote viewing series seem applicable here. "Since the judge
did not know a priori which elements of the descriptions were correct or
incorrect, the task was complicated, and transcripts often seemed plausibly
to match more than one target. A confounding factor in these studies is
that some target locations have similarities that seem alike at some level of
perception ... a radio telescope at the top of a hill, the observation deck
of a tower, and a jetty on the edge of a bay all match a transcript description
of 'looking out over a long distance.' ... According to the judge, the most
successful procedure was a careful element-by-element comparison that tested
each transcript against every target ...." (Puthoff and Targ, 1976b, p. 346)
D. Alphabet Letter Experiments
As a result of previous research, Puthoff and Targ (1977) concluded that
"The problem in the forced-choice matching experiments, as opposed to the
free-response task, is that the subject's stored mental images are available
to him from his memory, and constitute an important source of 'noise' in the
172
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
orAULTow
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : LhkA[L7 6-OO787ROOO1OO26OOO3-2
remote viewing channel." This is supported by the following experiments.
Subjects H1 and I1 participated in twenty letter-guessing trials, Hl
in her Los Angeles home and I1 in his New York City home. One letter target
was posted each day in the SRI laboratory. Subjects were to attempt to
identify the letter. The results are not given in the 1977 technical report,
although the authors state that the results were "not found to depart from
chance expectation." Since > 2 correct would be required (of 20 trials)
to obtain p < .05, and > 3 at p < .01, we must assume that neither subject
hit on more than one target, assuming further that the 20 targets were
sampled with replacement from a full alphabet of 26. Very incomplete
reporting of this experiment makes an analysis of the results quite tenuous.
In a second alphabet remote viewing experiment, subjects S1 and H1
were asked to draw what they saw posted a few laboratories away. Puthoff
then blindly estimated the letters based on these drawings. S1 was correct
on 2 of 2 trials, while H1 was correct on 1 of 3 trials.
The probability of S1 being correct, by chance, on both trials is
p = .0015; the probability of H1 being correct on 1 of 3 trials is p = .0043;
the combined results of 3 or more correct out of 5 trials is significant at
p = 5.36 x 10-4 by the binomial theorem, as reported by the authors. Thus,
the obtained results clearly exceed chance expectations for each subject,
and for the combined data.
The authors conclude that "This suggests that the way to increase the
analytical capability to include written material is to arrange to separate
the perception from the analysis, to encourage the subject to describe only
his or her perception, and to follow up by having a different person do the
final analysis on a blind basis." It seems that improvement. can be made on
r 173
Approved For Release 2000/D8/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787ROO0100260003-2
C"A @'i YV41 '
Approved For Release 2000/08 IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
this protocol by having the judging done on a real blind (or double-blind)
basis, that is, by having the judge otherwise naive about the experiment.
Puthoff did the judging in this experiment.
E. Random Stimulus Generator Experiments
A four-state random generator was designed to evaluate how well subjects
could learn to guess which of four letters would next appear on a display.
This experiment is discussed in Puthoff and Targ (1977) and in Targ and
Puthoff (1977).
The data for four experiments with subject I1 are shown in Table 10.
The authors state that the "probability of > k successes in n trials is
obtained by table lookup of the probability of a normal distribution value
(k - 4 - 1)/(3n/16)1/2.
(2)
It is perhaps worth noting that the t distribution is not a normal (z)
distribution, and that experiments 3 and 4 had results which did not exceed
chance. While the overall results are clearly significant., the subject's
ability deteriorated over the four experiments. In fact, the high hit rate
on experiments 1 and 2.(Phit = 0.36) was high enough to sustain a significant
result for the total data set even if the subject ran another 700 trials
at exactly chance (P = 0.25) performance. Thus, the decision to stop after
experiment 4 seems to be one of good strategy.
While the conversion from the obtained number of hits to a t value is
unreferenced, one can also verify the results by either a critical. ratio
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-2P96-00787R0001 00260003-2
TABLE 10. RESULTS OF SUBJECT I1 ON RANDOM STIMULUS
GENERATOR EXPERIMENT
TARGET NUMBER OF NUMBER
l
EXPERIMENT LETTERS TRIALS, n OF HITS, H t
1
'ABIO
200
64
2.20
0.014
2
CDGQ
100
44
4.27
1.07 x 10-5
3
EHLT
100,
31
1.27
0.102
100
28
0.58
0.28
4
KWYZ
500
167
4.29
< 10-5
(k'- 4 1)/(3n/16)1/2, for p - 1/4
and k is number of hits on n trials.
Approved For Release 2000/08/101?bIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08WOLMW
P96-007878000100260003-2
equivalent (Siegel, 1956) or by the exact binomial. Recalculation of the
p-values by the binomial for.the four experiments yields 0.010, 1.09 x 10-5,
0'.069, and 0.930, respectively. Thus, experiments 1 and 2 remain statistically
significant, while experiments 3 and 4 remain nonsignificant. By the exact
binomial, the total of 167 hits of 500 trials is also significant, p = 0.00001.
The authors conclude (Puthoff and Targ, 1977, p. 33) that "... the
results obtained in the remote-viewing and machine approaches to reading
remote alphabet, characters do indicate a potential for developing acceptable
levels of reliability in reading text for operational purposes. Further
study is required to determine whether this reliability can be achieved with
a reasonable effort."
In a previous experiment with the random stimulus generator, the six
remote viewing subjects (S1 through S6, presumably) were tested. Only Elgin
(S2) showed performance greater than chance (Targ and Puthoff, 1977, p. 129).
In another, NASA-funded, study, 145 volunteer subjects were tested, 79
adults and 66 children. Of these, six subjects had a significantly positive
slope (p < .01) over an undisclosed set of trials. (Targ and Cole, 1975,
p. 28)
Two other subjects had significantly beyond chance scores over 1400
and 2800 trials, respectively, with mean percent hits of .305 (p = 2 x 10-6)
and .2957 (p < 10-6). "These results indicated that the ESP machine can
serve as a suitable screening device for those with ESP ability." (Targ and
Cole, 1975, p. 29) Unfortunately, the above results are predicated upon a
true a priori probability of 0.25; i.e., the generator is indeed random.
As will be discussed later, this assumption is questionable.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10~:76IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 DP96-00787R000100260003-2
ULURILI
Critical Evaluation
The authors conducted a reasonable evaluation of the statistical random-
ness of the four-state generator. Without question, the zero and first-order
probabilities of occurrence are within random chance fluctuation.
A separate analysis was conducted by the sponsor, and reported in an
internal memorandum. The results of this analysis are quite interesting.
Table 11 gives the numbers of nonidentical transition frequencies, i.e.,
the next-to-be-generated color, following the present color. These sequences
of pairs of nonidentical colors, both forward and backward, are shown in
the lower half of Table 11.
The forward and backward pair frequencies are all within random bound
expectations. However, one would also expect no correlation between the
forward and backward pair frequencies. That is, the likelihood of a given
color following any other color should be unrelated to the reverse. order of
occurrences of those pairs. Such is not the case, for the correlation between
the two columns of pairwise frequencies in this table is r = 0.93, p < .01.
The mean forward frequency (788) across all pairs does not differ significantly
from the mean backward frequency (798).
Combining the forward and backward frequencies, the differences among
the six transition frequencies are statistically significant (X2 = 13.34,
df = 5, p = 0.02). Thus, a successful strategy that could be adopted to
exceed chance is: "When green press red, when red press green, and otherwise
use 'pass' button as much as possible." This strategy will increase the
hit score and, given enough trials, yield a significant result.
There is no evidence that Elgin, or any other successful subject
followed this strategy. Neither is there any evidence they did not. A
Approved For Release 2000/08/101?tlA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1A' P96-00787R000100260003-2
TABLE 11. NONIDENTICAL TRANSITION FREQUENCIES IN FOUR-STATE GENERATOR
CURRENT STATE YELLOW
GREEN BLUE
RED
Yellow (Y) -
Green (G). 777
Blue (B) 776
Red (R) 787
764 765
- 773
796 -
852 803
790
863
773
-
TRANSITION PAIR FREQUENCIES
FORWARD
BACKWARD
Y/G
764
777
Y/B
765
776
Y/R
790
G/B
773
G/R
863
852
B/R
773
Approved For. Release 2000/08/1017~. IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787R000100260003-2
careful analysis on successful subject's sequences would be interesting. In
any event, the lack of accessibility (and time to analyze) the actual data,
coupled with the above critical analysis, appears to be ample justification
to question the validity of the reported positive results.
F. EEG Experiments
Targ and Puthoff (1974) described an experiment "undertaken to determine
whether a physiological measure such as EEG activity could be used as an
indicator of information transmission between an isolated subject and a
remote stimulus." (p. 606)
Sender and receiver pairs were used, with visual and electromagnetic
shielding between them. A photostimulator was activated for 10 s duration
in front of the sender; the receiver's EEG was recorded at occipital location
0 with a bandpass of 1 to 120 Hz. The photostimulator operated at three
z
frequencies: 0 (no flashes), 6 flashes per second, and 16 flashes per second,
randomly intermixed.
No EEG driving was noted in any of the six volunteer subjects; however,
one subject (Hammid) showed a consistent alpha frequency blocking effect.
Data from seven sets of 36 trials each were then collected from this subject
on three separate days.
Fourier analysis of the spectra showed reduced power at 16 flashes/s
in the 9-11 Hz region, as compared to the 0 flash/s (control.) condition.
The 6 flashes/s power was between the 0 and 16 flashes/s levels. The
average power and peak power were both significantly less at 16 flashes/s
than at 0 flash/s (p < 0.04 and p < 0.03, respectively). The 6 flashes/s
power values, while in the "right" direction, did not differ significantly
01
Approved For Release 2000/08/179CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
4
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : P96-00787R000100260003-2
3. The demonstrated remote viewing ability appears to be insensitive
to time and distance.
4. Real-time, movement-containing activities can apparently be seen
through this ability.
5. Untrained subjects can demonstrate this ability and improve with
practice, often providing information as valid as that of known "sensitives."
6. The remote viewing channel is quite noisy. Concepts of information
theory pertinent to S/N improvement appear to apply to this channel as well.
Negative Characteristics
1. Research reports are of behavioral data, yet are not presented with
sufficient, rigorous experimental detail appropriate to behavioral science
publications and acceptable to behavioral scientists.
2. Conflicting, inaccurate reporting of experimental "facts" detracts
from the acceptability of the results.
3. Methodological weaknesses in the local target and long-distance U.S.
target procedures provide alternate (i.e., nonparanormal) possibilities of
explanation.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? tilA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :P96-00787R000100260003-2
OLUMT-
III. DESCRIPTION AND CRITICAL EVALUATION OF NON-SRI REMOTE-VIEWING
EXPERIMENTS
In this section of the report we describe the non-SRI published (and in
one case unpublished) remote-viewing experiments. Although the reporting
format differs greatly among these experiments, an effort is made to present
each experiment in the same format to permit direct comparison of experimental
methods and results.
Following-the description of each experiment, a critical evaluation of
the methodology is presented. Features of the experiment which are considered
to be of good scientific technique are pointed out. Similarly, weaknesses
of the methodology are also indicated, Following the philosophy that such
research, to be universally accepted, must be totally free of critical
methodological flaws (or possibilities thereof), emphasis is deliberately
placed upon known or potential "soft" spots in the methodologies.
A. Allen, S., Green, P., Rucker, K., Cohen, R., Goolsby, C., and
Morris, R. L. A remote viewing study using a modified version
of the SRI procedure. In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L.
Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 2975. Metuchen, N.J.:
The Scarecrow Press, 1976, 46-48.
Purpose. The purpose of this experiment is to repeat the general SRI
procedure, with modifications considered appropriate by the authors.
Experimental Design. Twelve remote-viewing sessions were run by a
three-person group. Each member of the group was subject (receiver) for
four sessions, target person for four sessions, and experimenter for four
sessions. Twelve targets were used, without sampling replacement.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :j$kA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
SrCnrT
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ~96-00787RO00100260003-2
Subjects. All three subjects were students in a research methods
class. The subjects had been conducting critical assessments of a variety
of (presumably related) experimental procedures.
Experimenters. Same as subjects, see experimental design.
Target Persons. Same as subjects, see experimental design.
Judges. Indicated to be "blind"; no additional information given.
Equipment. Subject performed in a University of California, Santa
Barbara experimental room and recorded all verbal responses on a tape
recorder. No additional information is given on the room or the tape recorder.
Targets. Twelve targets "were drawn from a population of 30 varied
locations (indoors and outdoors) within a half-hour drive of the UCSB campus.
No location was used twice." No additional information is given on any of
the targets, such as what they were, how similar or different to one another,
who chose the population, etc.
Procedure. At the beginning of each session, the three-person group
met in a UCSB experimental room. Roles were assigned to each person and
watches were synchronized. The target person then left the others and
obtained from R. L. Morris a "randomly selected target location concealed
inside an envelope."
The target person then went to the assigned location, timing his arrival
to be half an hour after leaving the subject and experimenter. He remained
there for 15 minutes, observing the physical surroundings and taking notes.
He then left the target to return to the experimental room.
Experimenter and subject relaxed in experimental room until target
person was to arrive at target. Then, experimenter turned on tape recorder
and recorded verbal, viewing responses of subject. Experimenter "occasionally"
183
Approved "For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10
96-00787R000100260003-2
sought clarification of a subject's statement or asked for more detail.
Tape recording ended after 15 minutes, or when target person left target.
Target person gave subject verbal feedback upon his return.
Data Analyeis. The 12 tapes were transcribed and given to 3 judges
along with target locations. One judge "chose to use a matching method
in which he visited each location and matched it with a transcript which
most resembled it. The same transcript could be chosen for more than one
location."
The other two judges "used.a rating method which permitted quantification.
Each of the.12 transcripts was rated in terms of its similarity to each of
the 12 locations as the judges visited each location. A six-inch line was
marked to indicate between "no correspondence" and "complete correspondence."
Confidence ratings were also obtained but not analyzed at time of reporting.
Means and standard deviations of ratings, across the 12 transcripts,
were calculated for each target for each of the latter two judges. Rating
for correct transcript was determined, in Z-score, for each target.
Results. Judge who used matching method got one hit out of twelve,
which is chance performance.
Of the other two judges, one rated the correct transcript above the
mean on 4 of 12 targets; the other judge. rated the correct transcript above
the mean on 3 of the 12. Six would be chance performance. Interjudge
agreement, of sign (above or below mean) was very good, with such agreement
on 11 of 12 targets.
"There was an insignificant but suggestive decline in results from the
first six sessions to the last six."
Thus, results were decidedly negative. No evidence for remote viewing
was obtained.
Approved For Release 200
4A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Conclusions. Although results were negative, authors note three
differences from SRI procedures. (1) Subjects were students critically
evaluating research methods, and "may thus have found it hard to maintain
the'mind-set of complete confidence that appears to be a vital part of the
SRI procedure." (2) Data were gathered in.a preset finite time period,
and "later sessions had a more hurried aspect to them." (3) Feedback was
limited to verbal feedback by target person; subjects did not visit target
locations.
Critical Evaluation. Several procedural weaknesses, or loci of criticism,
can be noted in this research.
(:L) No rationale is given for departure from the SRI procedures,. although
these departures are used to "explain" the negative results. Thus, the
experiment should not be considered a critical test of the SRI procedure.
(2) Subjects (and therefore target persons and experimenters) could
clearly be negatively biased toward existence of RV capability.
(3) No indication is given of order of each subject serving as subject,
experimenter, target person. This could be critical. For example, if target
person or experimenter concluded that previous sessions led to negative
results, then their performance as subject could be adversely affected
through nonbelief or low motivation.
(4) No information given as to instructions to judges, why they were
permitted to use different judging procedures, who they were, etc.
(5) Targets were undefined, no indication of target similarity was
given, and nature of target population was unspecified. Further, it is
not known who selected (and therefore knew) target population. Finally,
it is not known if any subject had previously seen any target location.
Approved For Release 2000/0
A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
(6) Did target person visit target aflone? How was this verified?
Did target person concentrate on "transmitting" to subject? Because target
person can reasonably be-considered a skeptic, these questions can be highly
pertinent to the obtained negative results.
(7) Nature of feedback to the subjects is not described. If inadequate,
it could lead to deterioration in performance over trials. While this
deterioration is suggested by authors, the nature of the statistical test
is not given.
(8) No quantitative data (Z-scores, signs, rating scores, rankings)
are given, While results are considered negative, data could be of interest
to other researchers.
Summary. In summary, this experiment produced negative results. However,
it has sufficient methodological insufficiencies to question the completeness
of the procedure. Were the results positive, numerous methodological problems
would cause us to question the validity of the results. The same questions
should apply to acceptance of the negative results.
B. Bisaha, J. P. and Dunne, B. J. Multiple subject and long distance
remote viewing of geographical locations. In Proceedings of the
International Conference on Cybernetics and Society, IEEE, 19-21
September 1977, Washington, D.C., 512-5.16.
Purpose. The first experiment tested the abilities of pairs of subjects
simultaneously to predict where the experimenter would be in the future.
The second experiment required the subject to predict, 23 to 24 hours in
advance, where the experimenter would be over 5,000 miles away. These two
experiments are described separately below.
186
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 0 : P96-00787R000100260003-2
Experiment One
Experimental Design. Seven subjects were tested (in four different
subject: pairs) on seven targets in the Chicago area, for a total of 14
subject transcripts. No effort appears to have been made to randomly or
logically pair the subjects. For the seven target encounters, the indicated
subject pairs, by subject number, were: (4,5), (10,9), (4,5), (6,7), (5,4),
(6,8), and (7,6).
The transcripts were randomly divided. into two sets (A and B) for
scoring. The first member of each pair of subjects had his/her transcript
assigned to Set A, the second to Set B.
Subjects. Su
bjects were volunteers, two males and seven females,
ranging in age from 24 to 37. Subjects knew each other, but were unrelated
by blood or marriage.
Experimenter(s): No mention is made of the identity of the experimenters;
they are probably the authors.
Target ,Person. This is one of the experimenters (female), identity
not given otherwise.
Judges. These were "six persons, not otherwise affiliated with the
experiment." No further information is given.
Equipment. Subjects' responses were tape recorded; no other equipment
is mentioned. Target locations were photographed by the target person.
Targets. The seven targets were randomly selected from the "target
pool of over one hundred locations in the city and suburbs of Chicago,
previously compiled and sealed [in envelopes] by an individual who had no
other association with the experiment." The contents of the envelopes
(target pool) were unknown to either experimenter or subjects.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ti -RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Prrocedure. Experimenter explained nature of the experiment to subjects
before the trials began, and they were told they would have sufficient time
to relax, become comfortable, and allow their minds to become as blank as
possible. They were then instructed.to visualize where the experimenter
would be between 35 and 50 minutes after the trial began. They were asked
to describe whatever images or thoughts they had, during that 15-minute
period, into a tape recorder, and to make any sketches if they wished. They
were asked not. to try to define specifically or identify their impressions,
but to describe them generally with as much detail as possible, "even if
the images appeared to make no sense or have no continuity." Subjects
within pairs were to have no communication with each other during the trial.
In four trials (undefined), subjects were in separate rooms on different
floors of the same building. In the other three trials, they were approximately
10 miles apart.
"When subjects began generating their descriptions, an experimenter left
the area with ten envelopes which had been randomly selected from the target
pool of over one hundred locations .... Driving continuously for 20 minutes
with no particular direction, or until five minutes after the subjects had
completed their descriptions, the experimenter then blindly selected a
number from one to ten from an enclosed container holding ten numbered and
folded sheets of paper, counted down to the chosen number of envelopes,
opened the envelope and proceeded to the location indicated on the enclosed
card, arriving at the target 15 minutes later, or 35 minutes after the
subjects started recording their descriptions. The experimenter remained
at the target for 15 minutes, photographed the location and made notes as
to her impressions of the site, then returned to the point of origin ....
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C$A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Typed unedited transcripts were made of the subjects' recorded responses
and attached to any associated drawings which a subject may have made."
No mention is made of feedback to the subjects.
Data Analysis. The 1.4 transcripts were divided into two sets, A and
B. Set A consisted of the subjects 4, 10, 4, 6, 5,.6, and 7, while Set B
consisted of.5, 9, 5, 7, 4, 8, and 6. Two judges blind ranked Set A
transcripts against the target photographs and notes; two other judges
blind .ranked Set B transcripts against the same photographs and notes; and
two final judges blind ranked Set A transcripts against Set B transcripts.
Each set of rankings was on a 1 to 7 basis, with all transcripts force
ranked against each target.
Morris' (1972) method of evaluation of preferentially matched free-
response material was used. (This method is described and evaluated in
Appendix A of this report.) Results are given in Table 12 As seen there,
all four judges successfully match the transcripts to the targets (p < .01).
Further, the two judges comparing Set A with Set B transcripts had sums of
ranks of 12 and 14, respectively, which are also statistically significant
(p < .005), as indicated in Table 13.
Of the total of 42 rankings made, 17 were direct hits. (Chance level
is 1/7 x 42 = 6.) "The degree of accuracy varied among subjects, as did
the specific details of the target." Transcript excerpts are given for
Plaza del Lago and the NWRR Station.
Conclusions. Results from Experiment One are clearly positive. All
judges were able to compare transcripts to the seven targets well beyond
chance probabilities.
Approved For Release 2000/08/101$CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
timnrT
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 0;. c.l - DP96-00787R000100260003-2
TABLE 12. RESULTS OF EXPERIMENT ONE OF BISAHA AND DUNNE (1977)
RANKS ASSIGNED
SUBJECTS
SET A
SET B
TARGET
A
B
JUDGE 1
JUDGE 2
JUDGE 3
JUDGE 4
Plaza del Lago
4
5
1
5
4
2
Wrigley Field
10
9
1
1
3
2
Techny Mission
4
5
3
1
1
3
Lindheimer Obs.
6
.7
2
3
1
3
Madonna del Strada
5
4
3
1
2
2
NWRR Station
6
8
2
1
3
1
Grant Park Bandshell
7
6
3
1
1
2
Associated p-value
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :QA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
TABLE 13. SET A VS. SET B RANKINGS FROM BISAHA AND DUNNE (1977)
RANKS ASSIGNED
TRANSCRIPT OF JUDGE 5 JUDGE 6
Plaza del Lago
1
1
Wrigley Field
2
.2
Teo.hny Mission
2
2
Lindheimer Obs.
1
1
Madonna del Strada
2
3
NWRR Station
3
4
Grant Park Bandshell
1
1
Sum of Ranks
Associated p-value <
.001 <
.005
191
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ; CIA_RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Experiment Two
Experimental Design. Five. trials were conducted, over a five-day
consecutive period, with the subject in the midwestern U.S. and the target
person in Eastern Europe. Subject responded between 8:30 and 8:45 AM (CDT),
while target person was at target location between 3:00 and 3:15 PM (local
time) 23-1/2 to 24-1/2 hours latet. Distances were 5087 to 5284 miles.
Subject. No description or mention is made of subject, selection
procedure, or ljis/her experience.
Experimenter. No mention is made of any experimenter located with the
subject. Presumably the timing and protocol were previously established,
requiring no experimenter to be. present during the trials.
Target Person. Probably this is the senior author, although such was
not stated explicitly.
Judges. The subject, the target person, and a third person, who had
no connection with the experiment, served as judges.
Equipment. A tape recorder was used to record subject's target
descriptions. No other equipment or subject location is mentioned.
Targets. Five locations where the target person happened to be at
the predetermined times served as targets. No mention is made of how such
locations were chosen.
Procedure. At the preset times the subject described the target person's
location, 23-1/2 to 24-1/2 hours in advance. Descriptions were tape recorded
and subsequently transcribed.
The target person concentrated on his surroundings, took a photograph,
and wrote brief target descriptions during the preset time periods. Upon
his return, target person presented the five photographs and brief descriptions
192
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C~R ~-00787R000100260003-2
in random order to the subject for matching. Subject gave the target person
copies of the transcribed descriptions, also in random order, for target
person to blind match against the targets. Also, a (third) judge blind
ranked the photographs against the subjects' descriptions.
Results. Following Morris' (1972) procedure (Appendix A), the results
are shown in Table 14. All three sets of ranks produced a result beyond
chance significance. Transcript excerpts from the Danube River and Exhibition
of Economic Achievement targets'show meaningful' commonality between descriptions
and targets.
Conclusions. Distance and time appear to pose no barrier to the effective-
ness of the RV channel.
Critical Evaluation. The methodology can be easily criticized on several
mechanical issues. First, the pairings of subjects were odd in Experiment
One: SL and S5 served three times each, always paired together; S6 served
three times, paired twice with S7; S7 served twice; and S8, S9, and S10
served once each. This does not appear logical by any system of a priori
planning. If experience in this type of task is pertinent to performance,
then confounding surely has occurred.
Secondly, some subjects were watched during the trials, while others
were not. No mention is made of how, by whom, why not all, etc. This
inconsistency also contaminates the results.
Third, judges were presumably naive. Instructions to judges are not
given.
Fourth, inconsistent locations of the subjects further confound the
results. Are some locations better than others? Did subjects communicate
telepathically between themselves? Did they do so only .at given distances?
Approved For Release 2000/08/13' CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : Cl
00787R000100260003-2
In certain locations? The fact that the. results are significant statistically
and the assertion by the authors that "the reports of the subjects in each
pair differed enough to make it obvious that ... the perceptions reflected
individual differences" do not rule out such intersubject communication. If
such communication existed, it was possibly inconsistent across subject
pairs due to the various circumstances of the trials.
Fifth, were the targets really located randomly in the Chicago area?
If the target'person drove continuously for 20 minutes with no particular
direction, opened the envelope, and then had only 15 minutes to get to the
target, the radial distance from the starting point had to be somewhat
constrained due to Chicago size and traffic density. These constraints are
not mentioned.
While the Experiment One excerpts appear very accurate, the judges'
responses are not that impressive for the targets for which excerpts are
printed (means of 3 and 1.75, respectively), not as good, on the average,
as that for all targets combined (mean rank = 2.07). Thus, one must conclude
that (1) all transcripts had elements pertinent to these targets, or (2)
the judges were not particularly accurate in the use of specific transcript
information. Since the judges behaved well above chance, (2) is illogical,
and one must admit that transcript element commonality (1) is a distinct
possibility. (This will be discussed in Section III as part of the response
criterion problem.)
An interesting question is whether the mean rankings, by target, for
Sets A and B are correlated with the mean agreement rankings, by target,
as given in Table 12,. If a positive correlation exists, one might logically
conclude that targets which lead to better remote viewing (Table 12) can
also be judged more reliably (Table 13. A product-moment correlation
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 MA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C
;00787R0001 00260003-2
between the mean ranks, by target, in Tables 12 and 13 was calculated; its
value is - .51 (p > .05). While not statistically significant, the negative
correlation casts some doubt as to the logical and consistent intertarget
differences.
The methodology in Experiment Two is very poorly described or controlled.
No mention is made of the following:
(1) Who was the subject, his/her experience, how chosen, etc.?
(2) Where was the subject during each trial, with whom, etc.?
Why was only one subject used?
(3) Who was the target person? Did the subject or anyone else
know anything of his itinerary? Did the subject know anything about his
particular tourism interests?
(4) Use of target person and subject as judges is unusual. Why
not employ more than one "naive" judge?
This second experiment is much too loosely reported.
Swnmary. While the results of both these experiments are impressive
and statistically significant, the methodology has several shortcomings, at
least as reported. More detailed reporting of procedural steps, subject
assignments and selections, and the like would produce greater confidence
on the part of the reader.
In their introduction, the authors state that "over twenty laboratory
experiments have been conducted in our lab with more than ten subjects
involving the precognitive protocol" and that positive results have been
obtained for each subject. This paper describes 12 trials, using eight
subjects. The next reported experiment (Bisaha and Dunne, 1977b) used
eight trials and two subjects. These sum to exactly twenty trials and ten
subjects, assuming the subjects are all different, which cannot be determined
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 L lA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
UOUSM
Approved For Release 2000/08/10.: CIA-.RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
from the reports. If the authors have performed "over twenty experiments"
using "more than ten subjects" the remaining data are unknown to us. It
should be noted that subjects in Experiment One are numbered S4 through S10,
and the subject in Experiment Two is not numbered. The two subjects in
Bisaha and Dunne (1977b) are similarly not numbered.
C. Bisaha, J. P. and Dunne, B. J. Precognitive remote viewing in
the Chicago area: A replication of the Stanford experiment.
In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R..,L. Morris (Ed.), Research in
Parapsychology, 2976. Metuchen, N.J.: The Scarecrow Press, 1977,
84-86.
Purpose. The purposes of this research were (1) to investigate the
ability of untrained persons to perceive and describe remote sites before
the target locations have been determined, thus replicating the SRI
experiments, and (2) to explore conditions of spatial and temporal
separation between subjects and target persons.
Experimental Design. Two subjects received a total of eight trials,
six for one subject and two for the other. Subjects were to describe the
target prior to target visit by target person.
Subjects. Two volunteer female college students served as subjects.
No other information is given.
.Experimenter. Experimenter was not specifically identified, but text
suggests it was J. P. Bisaha.
Target Person. One of the "experimenters," apparently B. J. Dunne.
Judges. The three judges were persons "not otherwise connected with
the experiment." No additional information is given.
Approved For Release 2000/08/16g7CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C
6-00787R0001 00260003-2
Equipment. The only apparent equipments are a tape recorder and
camera. Sketches. were made by subjects on paper.
Targets. The target pool contained over 100 sites in the Chicago .area
or suburban area lying within a radius of a 30-minute.drive from Mundelein
College. Specific targets (eight) used in this experiment are not defined.
Procedure. The subject was told about the nature of the experiment,
the procedure to be followed, and. asked to relax, become comfortable, and
"let her mind.become blank." At that point she was to try to visualize
the location where the experimenter would be 35 minutes later. She had 15
minutes to describe the location aloud into the tape recorder and to make
sketches of her image of the target location. She was advised not to
attempt to define or identify specifically what she saw, but to offer only
general descriptions and impressions. She remained in a closed room with
an observer who had no knowledge of the target location.
The target person left the college at the same time the subject began
her description. The target person carried 10 envelopes, each of which held
a card designating a particular target location. The 10 envelopes were
randomly selected by "another experimenter" from the target pool which
"had been compiled by a person who had no other affiliation with the
experiment and who was the only person familiar with the contents of the
envelopes."
The target person drove continuously for 20 minutes, or until 5
minutes after the subject had completed her response. Then, the target
person selected "at random" a number from 1 to 10 from an enclosed container
holding 10 identically folded sheets of paper. While still driving, the
experimenter counted down to that number in the pile of target envelopes
and proceeded to the target location on the enclosed card in the designated
Approved For Release 2000/08/10.98CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
envelope, timing her arrival to be 15 minutes later, or 35 minutes after
the subject began her response. The target person remained at the site 15
minutes, photographed the location, and made notes as to her impression of
the site. She then returned to the college.
Typed, unedited transcripts of the subjects' responses were attached
to any associated drawings made by the subjects.
Data llnaZyri,?, The eight transcripts, labeled only A through H, were
given to the judges in random order. Judges were also given, in random
order, the photographs and notes taken by the target person at each site.
Each judge was asked to blind rank order the transcripts with each location.
The judges were also taken to visit the target locations, but were
asked to rank order only in terms of the photographs. The authors do not
state whether these target site visits occurred before or after the rank
order procedure was completed.
Morris' method of analysis was applied to the sum of the ranks.for each
target.
Results. The sums of the ranks were 12, 12, and 15 for the three judges.
Two judges each had five direct hits out of eight targets. The third judge
had four direct hits. "The highest rank given any transcript was three."
The results for the two judges having a sum of 12 are significant at
p < 10-4 ; the result for the third judge is significant at p < .0005.
Conclusion. The authors conclude that the results support the existence
of "perceptual and communication channels which lie beyond the senses as
they are currently defined."
Critical Evaluation. This paper is presented as a "research brief"
and is necessarily limited in detail; nevertheless, several critical items
Approved For Release 2000/O~RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :. CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
of methodology are missing, items which might cause skepticism on the part
of nonbelievers. Specifically, these are:
(1) Characteristics, backgrounds., experience of subjects are not
given. Why did one subject receive two trials, and the other six? How were
targets assigned to each subject?
(2) No information is given regarding the contents of specific
targets in the target pool, nor of the 10 used on any given trial. We
do not know how similar or different the pool targets are, nor do we
know how similar or different are the targets in any trial subset of 10.
(3) We are not told if targets are sampled with replacement, nor
if any target is used more than once.
(4) The observer remaining with the subject is not described other
than to say the observer had no knowledge of the target location." We
do not know if this observer coached the subject, knew anything about the
experiment, was one of the authors, or merely served as a guard.
(5) The 1.0 target envelopes were selected by "another experimenter,"
presumably Bisaha. How were they "randomly" selected? Why by the experimenter?
If the observer with the subject was also Bisaha, there exist other possible
communication channels, both normal and paranormal.
(6) Who was the person who selected the target pool? What were the
criteria employed? Did he have any communication with subjects and judges?
All these are unanswered.
(7)
How did the target person select "at random" the target designating
slip of paper.?
(8) No apparent feedback was given to either subject. It seems that
all trials were completed before transcripts were presented to the judges.
200
Approved For Release 2000/94
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : QUEM96-00787ROO0100260003-2
No mention is made of subjects visiting targets or being debriefed by the
target person. Further, no mention is made as to the scheduling of trials:
over how many days, how many per day, per week, etc.
(9) Judges were asked to rank order the transcripts on the basis of
the photographs and target person's notes only, although they did visit the
targets. This is totally unclear. If they performed the ranking before
visiting the targets, then why should they bother to visit the eight targets?
If they visited the targets before performing the ranking, how could they
completely disregard the visual (non-photographic) information obtained at
the target site? Inadequate detail is presented on both procedure and
rationale..
(10) The authors state that "the highest rank given any transcript was
three." Presumably, they mean the highest rank given any correct transcript,
not merely any transcript, for the Morris procedure requires all eight
transcripts to be force ranked from 1 to 8.
While this may be a poor choice of phrasing by the authors, it is
disconcerting if it indicates a misapplication of the Morris procedure and
therefore biased results. That is, if the judges scored each transcript
from 1 to 8, but did not use all 8 ranks per target (over the 8 transcripts),
rather scoring each transcript independently on a 1 to 8 scale, then the
sum of ranks could easily be much less than the forced sum of 36 per target
(1 + 2 + 3 ... + 8). Because no data are presented, and because this one
results sentence is unclear, the conclusions should be viewed cautiously.
(11) If the outbound person drives randomly for 20 minutes, then
selects a target, and times her arrival at the target for 15 minutes later,
there are some impossible-to-reach targets unless she deliberately drives
toward the (unknown) target. That is, targets are within a 30-minute radius.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Assume a target is 30 minutes due east of the campus, but the target person
drives due west for 20 minutes. She cannot then turn 180 degrees and arrive
at the 50-minute distant target in 15 minutes. In fact, if the target is
30 minutes from campus, the only way she can arrive there in 15 minutes is
to drive nearly toward the target for the 20 minutes before she learns the
location of the target. The random likelihood of her being within 15
minutes of such targets is clearly small. Assuming that no time is required
for the target,person to park her car and walk to the target, then the
approximate probability of her being within a 15-minute radius of the target
located anywhere in the 30-minute radius target area is:
7r (30)
Since parking/walking time is nonzero, it seems improbable that the target
person kept the assigned schedule for each target. The impact of this
procedural question on the results is unclear, but quite unsettling.
Summary. This experiment is described very briefly and incompletely,
causing one to be skeptical of its having a careful methodological base.
Nevertheless, the results are impressive, in view of both the statistical
significance and the precognitive nature of the procedure.
D. Dunne, B. J. and Bisaha, J. P. Multiple channels in precognitive
remote viewing. In W. G. Roll (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 2977.
Metuchen, N.J.: The Scarecrow Press, 1978, 146-151.
202
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
'x(15)
(3)
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: 00787R000100260003-2
Purpose. The stated purpose of this research is to replicate their
previous experimental procedures with the additional "variable" of testing
two subjects simultaneously.
In actuality, this paper presents the same Chicago-area experiment
reported at the 1977 IEEE International Conference on Cybernetics and
Society. [See B above.] In fact, the paper is essentially a verbatim
version of _the earlier paper in spite of the later copyright.
Conclusions. The conclusions drawn by the authors are the same as in
the previous report of this research. In addition, they acknowledge that
"the procedure followed for target selection does not exclude the possibility
of experimenter clairvoyance in choosing the number which yielded a target
which best matched the subjects' descriptions, presenting a difficulty in
specifically demonstrating the locus of the psi process." (p. 150)
The authors suggest that "the rankings for each transcript against the
pool of targets be made by a separate judge to control further for
independence in the ranking process." (p. 150)
"The positive results obtained from the various experiments
attempted to date indicate that the overall protocol of this experimental
design is generally a useful and effective tool for additional research
into the nature of non-ordinary information transfer in 'ordinary' people."
(p. 151)
Critical Evaluation. This experiment is a second reporting of.an
earlier publication; thus, the same criticisms apply. In addition, comments
appear appropriate on a couple discussion points presented by the authors,
and described above.
203
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 DP96-00787R000100260003-2
ULUME
First, the problem of target selection is a real problem. Selection
by the experimenter can be a source of.contaminating clairvoyant effects,
as indicated above. However, selection-of a neutral third person, who in
turn selects the target numbers (or targets), is subject to the same
criticism, only one step removed; i.e., the experimenter clairvoyantly
selects the neutral individual because this neutral individual is more
likely to (will?) select the right target, etc. At first blush, this
appears to be an insoluble problem.
Second, the discussion regarding independent judges rankings is
another example of careless prose. The authors recommend that a separate
judge should be used for each transcript against the pool of targets. That
is, under the recommended procedure, a judge would take one transcript and
rank order all targets against it, rather than the present convention of
ranking all transcripts against a single target. While the statistical
algorithm will be equally valid, it would appear that memory and order
effects might serve to reduce the judges' abilities because they would
necessarily have to visit the targets sequentially, rather than compare
all transcripts "simultaneously" as in the more conventional protocol.
Last, the conclusion regarding the effectiveness of the protocol is
highly suspect because of the previously defined travel time problem. If
targets are really randomly selected five minutes after the subject ends
his/her response, and if the target person really drives randomly for 20
minutes to arrive at a point 15 minutes from the target, then the target
person can reach less than one-fourth of the potential target locations
in the allotted time. This hardly appears to be an acceptable protocol,
and it is doubtful that it can be (was) followed for each trial.
Approved For Release 2000/08/16)~CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :
96-00787R000100260003-2
Summary. This experiment is suspect for the reasons cited previously
following its earlier publication. The additional discussion of the results
presented by the authors in this iteration of the paper does nothing to
dispel those suspicions.
E. Hastings, A. C. and Hurt, D. B. A confirmatory remote viewing
experiment in a group setting. Proceedings of the IEEE, 1976,
October, 1544-1545.
Purpose. The purpose of this experiment was to obtain more experimental
data on remote viewing performance. The research was stimulated by the
earlier SRI studies.
Experimental Design. Thirty-six subjects generated information about
a single (unknown) target. They then had the six possible targets described
to them and "voted" for the target they thought to be the site based. upon
their individual responses.
Subjects. Thirty-six persons, male and female, served as subjects.
They were "mostly professional", and met one evening (8:30 PM) with the
understanding that an experiment in ESP would be conducted. No further
information about the subjects is given.
Experimenter. Arthur Hastings.
Target Persons. David Hurt and one person selected from the group of
subjects. The selection procedure was not described.
Judges. The subjects themselves voted as the judging procedure. No
other judges were involved.
Equipment. Notes and drawings were made by all subjects. No equipment
was involved.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1605CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For. Release 2000/08/10 :
96-00787R000100260003-2
Targets. Six targets were chosen by the-experimenters prior to conduct
of the experiment. The subsequently selected target was a playground area
with a log structure in a nearby park. The other five target locations were
(1) a children's play tunnel, (2) an orchard of flowering trees, (3) a bar
and restaurant, (4) an ice cream parlor, and (5) a post office building.
The target locations were chosen to provide wide variation in indoor/outdoor
locations and dominant mood.
Procedures. The targets' names were written on cards which were sealed
in envelopes and randomly numbered. No one knew the number of any target
location. In the presence of the 36 person group, one envelope was randomly
selected, by throwing a die. Hurt and one person from the group then left
with the sealed envelope. They opened the envelope in Hurt's car and drove
to the target site, timing their arrival to be exactly 10 minutes after
their departure. They "observed and interacted with the location" for 10
minutes, then returned to the group.
For the 10 minutes the target persons were at the target, the group
members "attempted to generate information about the target site, making
notes and/or drawings of their impressions." Following this 10-minute period,
but before the return of the target persons,'the subjects were allowed to
compare notes. Then, Hastings named the six locations in the target pool
and described them briefly. (He knew the targets in the pool, but not which
one was selected.) The subjects then "voted" on which target was the one
selected.
The target persons returned a short while after the vote was taken,
handed over their envelope, and reported the target to be the playground
area.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 & 6CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? 96-00787R000100260003-2
Data Analysis. Analysis As by a t-test, comparing the obtained number
correct with that expected (6) from the null hypothesis. With 6 targets and
36 subjects, 6 correct "votes" would be expected by chance.
Results. Twenty of the 36 subjects voted for the correct site. The
authors state that a "t-test (one tailed) for the 20 votes actually given to
the correct target gives a Z score of 5.22, with a probability of less than
6 x 10-7."
Conclusions. The experimenters report "astonishment" by the success of
the subjects. The subjects reported correctly such elements as swings, trees,
park lights, sand, and the log structure. Two subjects correctly reported
images of the target team taking off their shoe's. One subject drew a circle
and wrote "playground" in it; the nonauthor target person "reported that at
the location she had drawn a circle in the sand and had written 'playground'
in it." One subject reported the correct name of the park. Other subjects
correctly reported objects seen from the target site, but not part of it,
such as "the.jungl.e gym, swings, lighted windows of houses surrounding the
park, a soft drink can or the ground."
The authors attribute the effectiveness of the experiment to the way it
was conducted. Three conditions seem to help: "(1) Accepting that remote
viewing is possible and agreeing that you can do it; (2) Turning your
attention away from external perceptions and to inner pictures, experiences,
and thoughts; and (3) Receiving feedback as soon as possible." Instructions
to the subjects and procedures during the experiment emphasized these
principles. (Descriptions of the pertinent techniques are given.)
The authors also recommend the following improvements on their procedure.
(1) In addition to having the subjects estimate the correct target from
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 61A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
onnM.
SOPEURE1
Approved For Release 2000/08/10ri DP96-00787R000100260003-2
their responses, also have outside judges estimate the target from the
subjects' responses. (2) The target pool should be made up by someone not
at the experiment. (3) Clarify whether the coaches (half of each pair of
subjects) should vote on the basis of their "viewer's" information or with
consideration for information they themselves have generated. (Coaches
voted in, this experiment with undefined criteria; coaches' votes were not
separately noted.)
Critical Evaluation. While the results are highly positive, several
methodological questions arise which indicate uncertainty or poor reporting
detail.
(1) The original subject pool had 36 people, male and female. How
many of each? When one person (how chosen?) left with Hurt, presumably
there were only 35 left. Yet, the remaining subjects worked in pairs and
36 votes were cast before the target persons returned. How is this possible?
Was Hastings also one of the coaches (subjects, voters?) in addition to
directing the experiment?
(2) How could the experimenters select the targets, put the target
cards in envelopes, and randomly number the envelopes without knowing the
number of each target, assuming one of them really wanted to know a given
target's number? They should have had a disassociated person do this.
(3) Who rolled the die to select the target? How?
(4) At 8:30 PM in March it was dark outside. Was the playground lighted?
If not, how could the target persons see to move about, write "playground"
in the sand, etc.? A daytime experiment might appear more logical.
(5) Statistically, the null hypothesis predicts chance success (assuming
subject independence!) of 36/6 = 6. The authors state that they used a
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
one-tailed t-test to obtain a Z score. This is inappropriate: a small
sample t does not yield a normal distribution Z or standard score. The
t-test was developed to account for the nonnormal distribution caused by
small sample sizes, typically less than 30. While similar, t and Z are not
equivalent.
Using the binomial distribution,. one can calculate the Z score for the
obtained result:
Z - (x ? ,5) - NP = 13.50 = 6.037.
N 2.236
(4)
Evaluating this Z-score by the known unit normal distribution, we obtain a
probability of occurrence under the null hypothesis of P ti 10-8.
However, it should be noted that the large sample Z-score approximation
to the binomial distribution is valid for n > 25 and P approximately equal
to Q = 1 - P. As the disparity between P and Q increases, n must become
larger for the approximation to be usefully close. A good rule of thumb
(Siegel, 1956) for this approximation is that nPQ must at least equal 9.
In this experiment nPQ = 36(1/6)(5/6) = 5.
Thus, it is most appropriate to evaluate the results by the binomial
distribution.
If P is the probability of occurrence of an event, and that event occurs
x times out of n opportunities, then the probability density function is:
f (x) = ('t) i.ir, (1 - 1')n-x
.x,
The cumulative probability of the obtained result plus all more extreme
results is the value of interest to us. For this experiment, we have
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : LI3\-RDP96-007.87R000100260003-2
(5)'
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : P96-00787R000100260003-2
n = 36, x = 20, and P = 1/6. We wish to find the probability of obtaining
20 or more correct responses. That is, we wish to know:
36
Prob. (20 < x < 36) = E f(k).
k= 20
The probability of obtaining exactly 20 correct is 1.08 x 10-7, while the
probability of'obtaining 20 or more correct is 1.24 x 10-7.
Thus, the exact binomial probability of 1.24 x 10-7 is slightly less
than the authors' quoted probability of 6 x 10-7. While the difference is
unimportant numerically, the statistical selection and incorrectness of
definitions lead to concern for the authors' appreciation of experimental
methodology.
Summary. The obtained results are fairly impressive, in spite of the
methodological questions and inappropriate statistical techniques.
F. Jahn, R. G. Psychic process, energy transfer, and things that go
bump in the night. Princeton Alumni Monthly, December 4, 1978,
pp. S-1 to S-12.
(6)
Jahn, a noted researcher on advanced space propulsion systems and Dean
of the Princeton University Engineering School, has had a recent interest
in psychic phenomena. This publication is essentially the talk delivered
at Princeton on that subject in April 17, 1978. An edited transcript of
the tape of that lecture is also available, but not for quotation,
reproduction, or publication.
The studies of Jahn, in collaboration with Carol Curry, a Princeton
student, consisted of a. variety of demonstrations and experiments in remote
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : t'lA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
nnr-w
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? _ P 96-00787R000100260003-2
viewing, psychokinesis, and modeling of psychic phenomena. Most of the
"controlled" experimentation is in the psychokinesis area; since that is
beyond the immediate interests of this report, it shall be disregarded.
The remote viewing demonstrations took place early in their studies.
Since the reported details are sparse, we shall not present them in the
experimental format used for other publications, but rather summarize them
in the narrative style used by the author.
Stimulated by the Puthoff and Targ (1976) IEEE paper, Jahn and Curry
alternated as target person and subject in demonstration experiments with
the target person/subject locations being Brookhaven, Long Island/Princeton;
Princeton/Pompano Beach, Florida; northern New Jersey/Princeton; Stanford
University Chapel/SRI; Holiday Inn, Palo Alto/SRI; and perhaps others.
In each case, there are significant similarities between the target
person's. sketch of the target area and the subject's sketch. often things
in the sketches are reversed left-and-right; often there is similarity in
numerosity of elements, such as time in minutes and number of dismountings
of a horse; usually there is commonality of commonplace things such as trees,
sky, arches, horses, etc.
While the similarities are striking, some of the results suffer from
the "grass is green, sky is blue" criticism. Since no independent, uninformed
judges were used for comparisons and no a priori response criteria were
given, these results. can only be considered as demonstrations, not as true
experiments.
Further, the paper can be criticized on other grounds. The author
states (p. S-3) that: "We have tried this type of experiment many other
times with many other people. Almost always there is some correlation between
Approved For Release 2000/08/10,F,01
RDP96-007878000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
laboratory at the University of California, Santa Barbara." The reference
given to Schmidt's interaction with Curry is dated 1970, while the Morris
reference is dated 1978. Clearly, Curry was involved in psi activity well
before she walked into Jahn's office in the spring of 1977, and her interest
went beyond her "saturation" with electrical engineering and computer science.
In summary, the results of these incompletely presented demonstrations
are interesting and perhaps provocative, but the lack of details on the
methodologies, procedures, and results prec-lude this paper from serious
discussion in.this report. Hence, it will not be discussed further nor
included in subsequent analyses.
G. Karnes, E. W. and Susman, E. P. Remote viewing: A response bias
interpretation. Unpublished manuscript, Metropolitan .State College,
Denver, Colorado, 1978.
Purpose. The authors attempted to extend the SRI protocol by applying
three. major modifications within the framework of a signal detection
experiment. These modifications are:
(1) The subjective nature of the subject's responses (and the
necessity of using uninformed judges to rate the accuracy of the responses)
was eliminated. Subjects' accuracies of perception were measured by their
ability to visually identify color photographs of target locations. This
procedure permitted the direct measurement of inaccuracies as well as
accuracies of perception.
(2) A control group was used to provide a baseline measure of chance
remote viewing. The logic here is that, if the subject knows the target
person will be at one of X locations, the probability of chance selection
Approved For Release 2000/0
DP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
ET
of the correct target may not be X , due to response biases. The chance
level should be determined empirically.
(3) For efficiency, multiple (unselected volunteer) subjects were used.
Experimental Design. In the experimental group, the target person was
randomly selected from the subject pool and sent to one of nine target
locations. The subjects in the laboratory had photographic booklets of 18
sites, including the actual target.
In the control group, the target person was also randomly chosen, but
was sent to a location not contained in the photographic booklet. Both
the subjects and the target person were unaware of this fact, however,
believing that the location to which the target person was sent was actually
in the booklet.
Thus, the control group provided a baseline for chance guessing and
response bias against which the experimental group's responses could be
compared.
Subjects. The subjects were 115 volunteer college students drawn from
introductory psychology classes. They were told the experiment dealt with
ESP, and that the experimenters were particularly interested in subjects
who possessed ESP capabilities or who were interested in having their ESP
capabilities measured.
Experimenters. The authors served as experimenters.
Target Persons. For each group, one of the volunteer subjects was
selected to be a target person by his/her blindly drawing a blue poker chip
from a bag of otherwise red poker chips, with the number of chips equal to
the number of subjects plus target person.
Judges No judges were necessary as responses were objectively
evaluated.
214
Approved For Release 2000 ? CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Equipment. Four color prints were made of each site, each from one of
four distinct vantage points. The four prints of each site were mounted
on an 8-1/2.x 11-inch cardboard. The nine target and nine "noise" site
photograph sets were designated A through R, and placed in a test booklet.
They were not identified by name, location, or whether each was "noise" or
"target."
"Envelopes containing the four photographs of a target site and a map
showing the route to and location of the target sites were prepared for use
by the senders." No mention is made of how many such targets or "noise"
sites were prepared in this fashion.
Subjects used a response sheet that had blank spaces next to the 18
site letter designations. They were instructed to select one or more sites
as possible locations for the target person and to rate their confidence of
each selection according to five categories:
"1.) I am very certain that this is the correct location
2) I have a large amount of confidence that this is the correct
location
3) I have a moderate amount of confidence that this is the correct
location
4) I have a small amount of confidence that this is the correct
location
5) A possible site, but I have practically no confidence in its
being the correct location."
Targets. Twenty architectural sites in and around downtown Denver
were selected. The experimenters selected these sites on the basis of
architectural uniqueness, visual distinctiveness., and being within an
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 iOIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :_ CI - 96-00787R000100260003-2
OLUM=
easy 20-minute walk from the subjects' location.
The nine target sites were: (1) a Catholic church interior, (2) a
downtown college campus library interior, (3) the interior of a large self-
service liquor store, (4) a low income multiunit federal housing project,
(5) a street of remodeled historical houses, (6) a walkway in a redeveloped
commercial downtown area (Larimer Square), (7) an outdoor amphitheater
(Greek theater rotunda), (8) a small outdoor park area containing water
fountains and waterfalls (Skyline Park), and (9) the front entrance to the
State Capitol Building.
The nine "noise" sites included three distinctly different college
classrooms (a lecture hall, an experimental psychology laboratory, and a
typing/business machine laboratory), a restaurant interior, a college
bookstore interior, three distinctly different pedestrain malls, and an
indoor swimming pool.
The two control condition sites (to which target persons were sent, but
photographs of which did not appear in the subjects' booklets) were (1) the
front entrance to a large auditorium (Currigan Hall) and a heliopad at the
rear of Currigan Hal`1.
The basis of assignment of each of these 20 sites to one of the stimulus
groups is given by the authors as "architecturally different and visually
distinctive." The targets were chosen on this basis, the "noise" and
control group sites were the remaining sites after the targets were selected.
Procedure. The sequence of the 11 target sites (9 experimental group
targets and 2 control group sites) was determined by a table of random
numbers. Subjects were assigned to these conditions on the basis of their
appearance for the various sessions of the experiment. Ninety subjects
Approved For Release 2000/08/102~IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Ornn
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
served in the experimental group (10 subjects per target) and 25 subjects
served. for the two control group targets.
Subjects, upon reporting for the experiment, were read a typed set of
introductory explanations and instructions, which were identical for all
115 subjects. The nonuniqueness of the remote viewing ability was stressed
to permit subjects to feel "safe to explore the possibility of paranormal
perception.
Subjects were told that the-target person would be sent to a site within
a 20-minute walk and remain at the site for 10 minutes. During the time the
target person was at the site, they "should concentrate on communicating ...
by trying to receive impressions of the physical surroundings of the [target
person]." Subjects were asked to observe confidentiality about the experiment
and not to discuss it with others.
The target person was dispatched alone and was instructed to view the
site from the four camera positions during the 10-minute period, trying "to
communicate his/her,impressions of the site in ESP fashion ...." The target
person returned the envelope to the author's office upon returning..
Subjects were "told that 'remote viewing' skill probably varies among
individuals as does any other human skill and, if they received no impressions
of the [target person's] location, they should guess at least one target site
and rate the confidence of their guess(es)."
Subjects were individually isolated in offices, were told to familiarize
themselves with the 18 target sites, were informed when the 10-minute
"sending" period began and ended, and were instructed to put all materials
aside and to concentrate on receiving impressions of the target person's
location during the "sending" period.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1021f IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
era=
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 96-00787R000100260003-2
Data Analysis. Analysis of data was based on t-tests of proportions
of responses falling in various categories.
Results. Table 15 indicates the responses of the 90 experimental group
subjects. Note that these 90 subjects made a total of 409 selection responses,
of which 24 were "hits" and 385 were "false alarms." The 1211 no selection
responses are broken down into 66 "misses" (no response to the correct target)
and 1145 "correct rejections" (no response to a nontarget). The associated
probabilities are also tabled.
The proportion of correct selections is 24/409 = .0587. Chance is 1/18
.0555. The control group data provide a basis for estimating if this a priori
chance value of .0555 is valid. The 25 control group subjects made a total
of 160 selections, 85 (53%) to the 9 target sites and 75 (47%) to the 9
"noise" sites. The difference was not significant, t = 1.22, p > .10.
Thus, the a priori chance probability of .0555 is retained as the null
hypothesis value. A binomial test comparing the obtained .0587 with .0555
yields a p-value of .395, as reported by the authors. [We calculate .34
by the exact binomial, and .43 by the Z approximation for large N; in either
case, the difference is not significant.] Thus, there is no evidence for
a group averaged remote viewing capability.
Using signal detection theory, the authors calculate d' to be 0.033,
which indicates a very weak remote viewing signal strength. However, to
attach meaning to this measure, the proportions of hits (.2667) and false
alarms (.2516) should be significantly different, and they were not
(-l; = .35, p > .10).
The mean confidence rating for correct selections was 3.208, while the
mean confidence rattug for incorrect selections (misses) was 3.297. The
Approved For Release 2000/008/10 ?CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787ROO0100260003-2
TABLE 15 KARNES AND SUSMAN (1978) RESULTS FROM EXPERIMENTAL GROUP
RESPONSE
SELECTION
NO SELECTION
HITS = 24
MISSES = 66
24 PH
= ?2667
P
= 96 = .7-333
90
90
M
24
_
PCS = 409 = .0587
FALSE ALARMS = 385
CORRECT
REJECTIONS = 1145
1530
385
1145
__
PFA 1530 ?2516,
_
PCR 1530 ?7484
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?4IA-RDP96-OO787ROOO1OO26OOO3-2
ArAg
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ~96-00787R000100260003-2
difference, while in the logical direction, was not significant (t = -0.35,
p > .30).
Receivers (24) who obtained hits rated these hits with a mean confidence
of 3.208. The same receivers rated their false alarms 3.607. The difference,
while again in the logical direction, was, not significant (t = -1.38, p > .10).
Confidence ratings attached to selections (guesses) of the nine target
sites were compared to confidence ratings for the nine "noise" sites. The
mean for the target sites was 3.44, and the mean for the "noise" sites was
3.60. The difference was not significant (t = 0.61, p > .10), but the
direction of the difference suggests greater confidence for target sites.
Conclusions. While none of the statistical analyses provided clear
support for the existence of a remote viewing ability, all the differences
in means were in the direction predicted by the existence of remote viewing.
Critical Evaluation. This experiment is of particular interest because
the authors have attempted to apply rigorous scientific method to the
problem area. Controls were attempted for (1) response bias and (2)
response criterion lack of definition, by use of a forced multiple choice
procedure. The results clearly do not support a remote viewing hypothesis;
nontheless, several of the statistical measures are in the "right" direction
for RV support, although not nearly statistically significant.
This paper has been submitted to at least one refereed journal for
publication consideration and was rejected as being inappropriate in content.
Apparently, it has also been circulated informally among other investigators
because Puthoff and Targ at SRI received a copy and wrote to Karnes and
Susman with a critical evaluation of the research. Puthoff was kind enough
to provide us. with a copy of his letter, some of the elements of which are
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 2DDIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
no
erg=
n
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :-Znn 6-00787R000100260003-2
contained in the following evaluation (and referenced accordingly).
First, the,targets were apparently selected on the basis of their
distinctiveness, thus following accepted guidelines of other investigators.
The sequence of target selection ("random") is not well described, nor is
the person doing the selection. Since clairvoyance on the part of the
experimenters must always be admitted as a possibility, the use of a "blind"
third person for target selection would be desirable.
Second, 25. subjects served in the two control group sessions. While a
minor point,.why an odd number? How were they split between the two groups?
Details are lacking here.
Third, subjects viewed the 18 target photo sets before their efforts at
remote viewing and selected their responses subsequently to each potential
target. Puthoff, in his letter to the authors, suggest that this very
procedure virtually precludes success. He states that the most fundamental
finding of the SRI work is that "analytical functioning is inimical to
paranormal perception; therefore, paranormal perception will tend toward
null in direct proportion to analytical knowledge about target possibilities."
That is, knowledge of the target population essentially blocked success
because analytical functioning was included in the protocol. Puthoff feels
that the Karnes and Susman protocol "in principle could not speak to the
remote viewing phenomenon as we [Puthoff and Targ] understand it, since it
did not duplicate the most important requirement: free response to unknown
targets as opposed to forced-choice response to known targets."
While Puthoff indicates analytical function to be detrimental to
paranormal performance, he has also landed subjects who, for example, define
by name their targets (e.g., Hoover Tower). This allegation is somewhat
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 4kLlA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : go~-00787R000100260003-2
inconsistent. Clearly, if a subject offers a specific response (e.
Hoover Tower), he is either right or wrong. Inappropriate response
interpretation by a practiced judge is thus precluded!
This is an extremely important point. If RV takes place best (only?)
under free-response conditions, then a conservative scientific method
approach with no opportunity for response criterion errors seems incompatible.
Stated another way, response interpretation and evaluation by some judges
will likely be necessary and objective evaluation of responses seems
impossible. While Puthoff and Targ have certainly attempted to devise
methodologies which circumvent this constraint, their methodologies thus
remain foreign and unacceptable to a segment of the scientific community
for this reason.
Fourth, Puthoff suggests that a "blind" judge is best, and that the
subject himself is the worst judge. The experimenter is also considered to
be a poor judge. Yet, Bisaha and Dunne (1977a) found excellent, long
distance results using both target person and subject as judges. This
contention is clearly not supported by other investigators, and may not be
a valid criticism of the Karnes and Susman paper.
Fifth, the subjects in the Karnes and Susman experiment received no
feedback regarding their response accuracy. Puthoff feels feedback is
critical to paranormal functioning. However, if a subject makes only one
response (participates in only one experiment) then the feedback appears
quite irrelevant as it cannot change the already completed response and no
future responses will be made. In addition, Puthoff and Targ have often
stated that paranormal ability is likely to be at its greatest strength
on the first trial with a given subject and decline thereafter.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? lA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C - 6-00787R000100260003-2
Sixth, Puthoff claims multiple (simultaneous) subject experiments
do not work well. Yet other experimenters have obtained successful results
with simultaneous subjects (e.g., Hastings and Hurt, 1976; Vallee, Hastings,
and Askevold, 1976; Whitson, Bogart, Palmer, and Tart, 1976). In fact,
taken together, there appear to be more "positive" cases of paranormal
.functioning with multiple subjects than there are "negative" cases.
The experiment is the closest (of which we are aware) to classical behavioral
science methodology and control. Yet, it has been validly criticized as
violating some of the currently accepted. principles of RV functioning held
dear by currently successful investigators. It appears to be an interesting
step in the direction of rigorous scientific methodology. Such approaches
are warranted and needed; yet they should not be taken as a refutation of
results obtained by less conventional investigators active in the area.
Seventh, the control condition for response bias consisted of sending
the target person to a place not, on the 18-target list. To the extent that
the target person's-actual location has some elements in common with a real
target, that real target may be selected by the forced choice procedure.
As Puthoff very cogently points out, this is not a true control condition
but "rather a measure of the correlation between the target sites actually
visited in the control condition and the sites to be visited in the
experimental condition." Unfortunately, the outbound experimenter cannot
be sent "nowhere".
Summary. This experiment clearly produced statistically nonsignificant
results, although several of the comparisons were in the "right" direction.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?blA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 96-00787R000100260003-2
H. Rauscher, E. A., Weissmann, G., Sarfatti, J., and Sirag, S.-P. Remote
perception of natural scenes, shielded against ordinary perception.
In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in
Parapsychology, 2975. Metuchen, N.J.: The Scarecrow Press, 197.6,
pp. 41-45..
Purpose. The authors' stated purposes were (1) to confirm the existence
of paranormal perception in the description of remote natural scenes shielded
against ordinary perception, and (2) to replicate recent experiments of
Puthoff and Targ.
Experimental. Design. One subject viewed eight different targets, one
per day, over a period of two weeks.
Subject. The subject was a female music student at the University of
California, Berkeley, "who had no particular interest in ESP, although she
was open-minded and curious." She was not paid for her participation.
Experimenter. G. Weissman monitored the subject.
Target Person. J. Sarfatti selected and visited the targets.
Judges. Five judges were selected by either Rauscher or Sirag. No
mention is made of characteristics of the judges.
Equipment. A tape recorder recorded all the subject's verbal descriptions.
She also made sketches.
Targets. Ten natural target locations in the San Francisco Bay area
were selected by Sarfatti. Each was chosen-to be distinct in appearance
and at least 300 meters from all other targets. Each target was described by
a single. phrase, assigned a number between 0 and 9, and this target list
was kept by Sarfatti. Discussion of the list with anyone was strictly
forbidden. Neither the subject nor the experimenter had any knowledge of
Approved For Release 2000/08/1022tlA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
~ ~ Fa rao wee
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :
96-00787R000100260003-2
the target pool, or even in which city they. were located.
Procedure. The target person, experimenter, and subject met at a
"test location" one hour before the experiment. The target person and.
subject were in eye contact for 10 minutes, to establish a feeling of
rapport. The experimenter monitored the conversation to be sure that no
site information was divulged to the subject.
After 10 minutes, the target person left the test location alone, got
into his car, and randomly selected a site number by putting a "random"
ten-digit number in an I-IP-35 calculator, pressing the square root key
twice, and reading the resulting third digit from the left. Targets were
used without replacement; hence, this process was repeated if it resulted
in a target which had been previously selected.
The target person then drove to the target location, and spent 40
minutes within 5 meters of the target vantage point, concentrating on the
surroundings. "This involved seeing and attempting to get a feel for the
gestalt of the target location." Driving time to the target. is not stated,
nor is the timing for the subject to respond to the target.
The experimenter gave the subject encouragement by asking questions
about impressions the subject might be receiving about the location of the
target person. After 40 minutes, the viewing session was ended by the
target person telephoning the experimenter to divulge the target location,
and to permit feedback to the subject by having the subject go to the site.
This feedback was delayed for the first two sessions. (Length of delay and
reason for delay were not stated.)
Each of the five judges received transcripts of the tapes, the. subject's
drawings, the target persons list of target sites, a map, and a set of
Approved For Release 2000/08/102?6 IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 _f 96-00787R000100260003-2
Polaroid photographs of the target sites. The photographs were made by
either Rauscher or Sirag, who were unaware of the correct target-session
matchings.
Judges matched the transcripts with, the target locations (presumably
using the photographs, not by visiting the sites, although neither is
specifically stated). For each target site, they recorded a first, second,
and third choice of transcript, and also gave a percentage weight to each
choice to indicate the likelihood of its being a first choice. Judges were
instructed "to rate each transcript-site correlation.independently of the
other sites. Thus the same transcript selected as first choice for one
site could also be first choice for any other site." Judges were instructed
not to interact with one another, with the subject, with the target person,
or with the experimenter.
Data Analysis. Values were assigned to target choices on a 3, 2, 1
(first, second, third choice) basis. The five judges' choices were pooled
for each session. "If the correct target was among the top four targets
chosen by the pooled judges, this was counted as a hit with a chance
probability of one-half."
Results. There were four hits: sessions 3, 4, 6, and 7. "This was
clearly not a significant result. We ignored the weightings assigned by
the judges as they did not seem to change our analysis significantly."
In spite of the lack of significance, the authors believed that "only
a high degree of coincidence could account for some of the correlations
between the actual target and the subject's descriptions." Specifically,
the first target closely resembled the second session response, and the
second target "seemed to be related. to" the third session response. Note
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 j.% fl 9~6-00787R000100260003-2
that no feedback occurred after the first session. Her street address
.knowledge of .the first target was in fact erroneous in nature. She was
also not taken to the second target site until after the third session.
Conclusions. Authors were impressed by subjective similarities between
targets and responses, although results were not statistically significant.
They believe the design could be improved by the following:
(1) Immediate feedback, and sessions spaced not more than a day apart,
to facilitate learning and avoid. target '-'flashbacks."
(2) Selection of targets which are as peculiar and different as possible.
(3) Encouraging subject to give just "primary impressions" and avoid
trying to recognize exact target.
(4) Note "frame of mind or mood" of the subject and correlate that
with performance.
(5) More elaborate techniques for putting the subject at ease, including
sensory deprivation.
(6) Variation in the experiment, such as a precognition paradigm or
using simple objects or pictures.
Critical Evaluation. This experiment was poorly conducted in many
respects, and thereby presented several "opportunities" to obtain negative
results. While this was the first attempt at remote viewing research by
the authors, it nevertheless exhibits several sources of behavioral science
naive methodology.
The use of a single, inexperienced subject was an initial risk.
Although some authors claim that all people have some paranormal ability,
this particular subject had not previously demonstrated such ability, nor
.did she necessarily believe she had the ability.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 96-00787R000100260003-2
While careful target population control was observed, the statistical
techniques could have been better. First, the judges should have visited
the targets rather than view the photographs. Polaroid pictures are hardly
wide-angle photographs, and more information might have led to better
selection. Indeed, the sample responses so suggest. Second, ranking only
three choices is not consonant with the Morris (1972) procedure used by
Puthoff andTarg. Forced ranking of all target responses could lead to
a different conclusion if the results were "close."
Last, the inconsistent use of feedback, comments on "frame of mind",
desire to use sensory deprivation, and the like sound like amateur armchair
psychology of the "what if?" variety. The authors might benefit substantially
from the advice of someone more deeply trained in behavioral research
methodology.
Sunvnary. This experiment is not of particular importance due to its
several methodological flaws. Its results are neither conclusive nor a
critical test of the paranormal phenomenon.
I. Solfvin, G., Roll, W. G., and Krieger, J. Meditation and ESP:
Remote viewing. In W. G. Roll (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1977.
Me.tuchen,.N.J.: The Scarecrow Press, 1978, pp. 151-157.
Purpose. This experiment was done in the context of a series of
experiments exploring meditation and ESP in group situations. The purposes
of this particular experiment were to (1) explore the "liking-disliking"
aspects of the psi production and (2) make the psi process exciting for
this secondary school junior-senior age group to maximize the possibility
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : GIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
e
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: ClAQP
of positive results. Accordingly, as remote viewing situation was chosen
in which one of the students would. leave the classroom to visit a randomly
selected location in Durham, N.C., while his classmates meditated.
Experimental Design. The class was instructed in Eno meditation by
W. G. Roll for two class sessions. Then several personality tests were
administered,. among them a scale measuring their like-dislike for other
students in the class. Then, for six apparently successive class days, one
student went with G. Solfvin to a randomly selected target. The remaining
students (subjects) meditated and attempted to select the correct target
from a set of four.
Subjects. The subjects were 11 males and 5 females in a junior-senior
class at Durham Academy, a private school in Durham, N.C. The experimenters
indicate the students to be highly interested and motivated throughout the
experiment.
Experimenter. Apparently the experimenter remaining with the subjects
was W. G. Roll.
Target Persons. Each day a student was randomly selected from the class
to accompany G. Solfvin to the target. The student target person was known
to the subjects and was instructed what he/she might do at the target location.
Judges. No judges were needed as the subjects voted on the target.
Equipment. Nothing of note.
Targets. Twenty-eight targets were selected and photographed prior to
the.beginning of the class. Seven pools were formed, each containing four
dissimilar locations. None of the targets is listed, named, or described
in the paper.
Procedure. Each experimental session lasted about one hour. The
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CPA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1096-00787R000100260003-2
student target._person was randomly (procedure not specified) selected and
left the room with Solfvin. The subjects "would meditate for about 20
minutes and then attempt to experience what the target person was experiencing
for another 5 minutes." Each subject then completed a questionnaire dealing
with the quality of his/her meditation and indicated how emotionally close
or friendly he/she felt (on a I to 9 scale) about that day's target person.
They were then shown photos and instruction sheets of the four possible
target locations, in random order, and asked to rank them "according to
their own impressions of where" the target person visited.
The instruction sheets gave the target person the name and address of
the target location, directions to drive there from the school, and what to
do at the target. The instructions and target photos were in identical
envelopes. The target person selected one of the four randomly after
arriving at the car. Solfvin and the target person did not return to the
classroom until the subjects had turned in their responses.
No mention is made of daily or other. feedback to the subjects, although
it is reasonable to assume that daily feedback may have occurred. It would
be unreasonable to assume that the target person would not tell his class-
mates of the target location.
Data Analysis. A majority vote was used to determine the group response.
The mode was used to break target ties based upon the mean vote.
Subgroups of the subjects were formed based upon how they rated the
target person (liked, disliked) and how the target person ranked them (liked
by, moderate, disliked by). "For the former we calculated the median rating
from all. the [subjects] for all six days. This was used as a cut-off point
to assign the [subjects] to the 'liking' or 'disliking' subgroups. We then
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?LQIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :6-00787R000100260003-2
applied the majority vote procedure ... to the target responses of these
subgroups."
"From the,[target person] ranking of his 15 classmates we categorized
ranks 1 to 5 as 'liked by [target person],' ranks 6 to 10 as 'moderate'
and ranks 11 to 15 as 'disliked ....' The majority vote procedure was
also applied to target responses of each of these subgroups for each day."
The sum-of-ranks statistic for preferentially ranked data, developed
by Solfvin and Kelly, was applied to examine for individuals achieving
significant responses., No reference is given for this test.
:Results. "No significant psi scoring was evident in the group majority
vote rankings. This was also true.of the majority vote rankings of the
various subgroupings--liked [target person], disliked [target person], liked
by [target person], moderate, disliked by [target person]. The target
rankings of these subgroupings did not differ significantly for one another
nor from the rankings of the entire group."
"None of the [subjects] showed significant psi scoring, although several
of them showed tendencies towards psi-hitting ...."
Conclusions. The authors plan.to use the like-dislike concept in future
psi research, feeling that the results are suggestive and warrant future
evaluation. They recognize no group remote viewing positive results were
demonstrated in this experiment.
Critical Evaluation. Clearly, no positive remote viewing was demonstrated.
Possible reasons are the following.
(1) Inadequate assurances were given to the subjects to encourage a
remote viewing experience.
(2) The use of a forced-choice response may block the remote viewing
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 Z CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ,p6-00787R000100260003-2
phenomenon. See the Puthoff criticism of the Karnes and Susman paper;
the same applies here.
(3) Inadequate or no feedback may have taken place after each trial,
thereby permitting trace responses/impressions from one target to contaminate.
the next.
Summary. The methodology is again somewhat different from the Puthoff
and Targ approach. These differences, should a true remote viewing capability
exist, may have suppressed it in. this experiment. Clearly, however, no
strong remote viewing results were obtained in this study.
J. Tart, C. T. Psi. New York: E..P. Dutton, 1977, Chapter 8.
In this book, which deals with the author's twenty years of psi research,
Chapter 8 is devoted to a discussion of three remote viewing experiments.
The first two were previously published and are described below (Whitson,
et al., 1976). The third, apparently unpublished elsewhere, is described
here.
Purpose. The author was impressed by the procedures and results of
the Hastings and Hurt (1976) experiment, and attempted to replicate the
experiment during the conduct of his two-day workshop on consciousness and
extrasensory perception for the professional staff of the Nebraska Psychiatric
Experimental Design. Workshop participants were formed into 25 teams to
remote view one randomly. selected target site.
Subjects. The subjects were the staff of the Institute who attended
the workshop. Originally there were 25 teams, each consisting of a coach
and a viewer. Some participants had to leave early, however, and 25 viewers,
Approved For Release 2000/0 23tIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? P96-00787R000100260003-2
MUMT1
21 coaches, and 20 intact teams provided responses. No other details are
known about the subjects.
Experimenter. Charles Tart.
Target Persons. Marjorie Hook-Gegoud (workshop organizer) and her
husband.
Judges. Subjects judged their own responses in a forced-choice fashion.
Equipment. "About sixty" slides of possible target sites were provided
Tart by Hook-Gegoud prior to his arrival in Omaha. Notes and drawings
were made by subjects.
Targets. Tart (alone) selected six slides from the 60 that he "felt
were visually quite distinct." He then sealed each slide, "along with a.
set of instructions that I made up then and there on appropriate things that
could be done to interact with the site, in an opaque envelope." No mention
is made of where these selected slides were stored or what was done with
the nonselected target slides.
Procedure. The participants were instructed on psychological procedures
for eliciting psi for remote viewing, and then divided into two-person teams
(assignment procedure is undefined). The coach's main functions were to take
notes of the viewer's imagery, ask questions to help elaborate imagery, and
[Tart then conducted a general ESP test in the workshop room to get
their psi talents to operate. Using color slides of four classical
paintings, he was quite successful. Hastings (later) served as a blind
judge of the results and correctly matched 24 of the 28 responses to the
correct target slide. The target slide, chosen deliberately by Tart (The
Sacrament of the Last Supper, by Dali) was used successfully in the
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?elA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 -096-00787R000100260003-2
Maimonides studies, with which both Tart and Hastings were familiar! This
experiment is considered too confounded in methodology to be critically
discussed here; it is mentioned only because it was part of the "warm up"
procedure.]
The target persons left with all six envelopes and used an HP-25 calculator
with a random-number generating program to select a target by a number between
1 and 6 as the last digit. (They had "to push the [Generate] button many
times before they got a number between 1 and 6." flow many times is not
stated.) They then got into their car and started to drive away, opened
the target envelope, and were to arrive at the target in 20 minutes, at which
time the subjects would try to remote view. .
After all drawings and notes were collected, the envelopes were opened
and all six target slides were projected for the subjects to see. Each
team voted separately, as did each viewer and each coach. Voting instructions
were not given, although the word "ranking" is used in the text. The
"Combined first and second choice responses for each site" are presented.
After all votes were tabulated, the target persons returned to brief
the subjects.
Data Analysis. No statistical analysis of the data is presented (or
warranted).
Results. The. results are given in Table 16. Clearly, the results do
not confirm the existence of remote viewing. However, the author offers a
strong explanation for the results, as follows:
When Hook-Gegoud returned, she immediately apologized for "messing up"
the experiment. The HP-25 give a room in the art museum as the target. Her
husband stated the art museum was closed that day, which they verified by
Approved. For Release 2000/08/10. CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 P96-00787R000100260003-2
TABLE 16. TART'S (1977) RESULTS
TARGET SITES
VIEWERS
COACHES
TEAMS
MORTUARY
17
12
13
MUSEUM
11
11
9
CHURCH (TARGET)
10
7
7
WOOLWORTH'S
8
9
10
BOOKSTORE
4
2
1
PRINTSHOP
0
1
0
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ZCIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1pP96-00787R000100260003-2
telephoning the museum. They then selected the next target envelope, the
church, which they got to on time.
Hook-Gegoud, throughout the experiment, had hoped the mortuary would be
selected as the target due to the fact that a close friend had died a few
days earlier and that his funeral was taking place that afternoon at a
different mortuary. She had wanted to attend the funeral but chose to
participate in the experiment instead.
Conclusions. The author believes the experiment was a "failure" in
terms of the subjects' votes. "Yet, the viewers' choices showed a clear
preponderance of first and second choices for the mortuary (the target site
that the principal agent had strongly wanted to go to), a second-place vote
for the room in the art museum (where the agents had been supposed to go),
and a third-place vote for the church .(the site they had actually visited).
The coaches' separate votes and the team votes also showed a preference for
the mortuary, with the room in the art museum and the church also getting a
very high number of votes."
Tart believes the results to demonstrate psi, but points out that we
do not know the Limits of psi. That is, "we cannot assume that the
experimenter is independent of the experiment." In this case, the target
person's desires would appear to influence the outcome as much as the target
person's actions.
Critical Evaluation. This experiment clearly shows negative statistical
results. Interpreted objectively and scientifically, it does not support
the remote viewing hypothesis.
The author's post hoc explanation, while plausible, is untestable.
It remains interesting speculation.
Approved For Release 2000/08/103,t,J4-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10
96-00787R000100260003-2
UMffW
If one assumes that the target person's desires and knowledge can
influence the results, and further that the experimenter's desires and
knowledge can also do so, then this experiment is strong evidence for a
true double blind protocol, one in which both the target person and the
experimenter are unaware of:
(1) the target pool and any element therein,
(2) the constraints on the target pool,
(3) any possible nonselected targets,
(4) any previous experiments involving the subjects, and
(5) any hypothesized results expected from the experiment.
These are difficult criteria to meet.- They will be discussed in a
subsequent section of this report.
Summary. The negative results, coupled with the post hoc theorizing,
make this experiment of little value in confirming the existence of remote
viewing. However, the lessons learned from the methodology and results can
be important.
K. Vallee, J., Hastings, A. C., and Askevold, G. Remote viewing
experiments through computer conferencing. Proceedings of the IEEE,
October 1976, pp. 1551-1552.
Purpose. The purpose of this research is to extend the work of Puthoff
and Targ 01to cases in which the participants were several thousand miles
from each other, with control. of sensory conditions automatically and
unobtrusively provided by the medium of communication ...", the computer
network.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1V? CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Lxperimental Design. Twelve persons had individual computer terminals
and attempted to describe ten mineral samples held in a geologist's hand,
one per day at a prespecified time. Five of the 10 samples were enclosed
in larger envelopes and labeled a "double-blind" pool. Because all persons
were not logged onto the computer each day, a total of 33 descriptions were
contributed by. six persons for the 10 samples. Thirteen of the descriptions
were "double blind" and the remaining 20 were "open."
Subjects, Twelve persons served as subjects. They were in New York,
Florida, Quebec, and California. Each was supplied a computer terminal for
his/her home or office. They all volunteered I to participate. Nothing is
known of the subjects; however, the four excerpted transcripts in the paper
indicate some geological knowledge by at least two subjects. Of the 33
transcripts, 11 were contributed by "author" A. H. In the published table,
the word "author" presumably means subject, although it is interesting to
note the similarity of initials to one of the authors of the paper.
Experimenter. Messrs. Hudson and Wilson are thanked for "carrying
out and analyzing the experiments.". They are not otherwise described. The
role of the authors in this research is undefined.
Target Person. "A geologist" sitting at his home terminal selected
the sample (target) each day. He is not otherwise identified or described.
Judges. These were five persons who had no prior knowledge of the
correct pairings; they are identified as a sociologist, an editor, a
physicist, a secretary, and a librarian. In no particular order, the
following persons are (later in the paper) thanked for serving as judges:
Ms. Amara, Ms. Chula, Dr. Johansen, Dr. Lipinski, and Ms. Spangler.
Approved For Release 20
: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 DP96-00787R000100260003-2
Equipment. Each subject had a portable computer terminal in his/her
home or office, linked to a computer teleconferencing system. Participants
-.y l..Vtl111LGLLLb aLe 11ILLlieUJaLely printed
by. the computer on the terminals of any participants who are currently logged
in (or are stored for later retrieval)." No mention is made of whether or
not each subject's response was printed immediately on all other terminals.
The security of the input information, the nature of the system software
logic, and other pertinent details are absent from the paper.
Targets. Ten mineral samples were selected from "geological collections."
The samples were "the rare mineral bastnosite, a vein filling of galena and
quartz, opal, gold ore, halite, cinnabar, magnetite, realgar, barite, and
cobaltite. Subjects were told only that the targets were mineral samples
from North America. The manner of. select and the person doing the selection
are not described at all, nor is the storage place of the samples.
Procedure. "Each day at 7:30 AM and 7:30 PM Pacific Daylight Time, a
geologist sitting at his home terminal took one of the envelopes from the
open pool, extracted the sample, and held it in his hand. Anyone logged
into the conference at that time could volunteer a remote viewing description.
Such descriptions were recorded and printed by the computer with a date and
time stamp. After all descriptions were in, the geologist entered a brief
description of the specimen to provide feedback for the participants. The
sample was then removed from the open pool.
"Similarly, each morning the envelope for the day was taken from the
double blind pool and placed at a designated office location where it was
a target for remote viewing for eight hours. Anyone logging into the
conference during that time could type in a description of the sample
Approved For Release 200,
IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1P96-00787R000100260003-2
ULUIILI
contained in that envelope. At the end of the day, the envelope was taken
to the geologist, who added the sample to the open pool. No feedback was
given for the double blind targets."
Thirteen of the 33 responses were double blind, and 20 were open. Four
of the 10 targets were run under both open and double blind conditions.
(Which targets led to responses under each condition is not indicated in
the paper.)
The five judges were given transcripts of the responses (without time,
date, or subject identification) and the geological specimens. They were
instructed to assign one or more specimens to each response. "Each specimen
assigned to a given [response] was allotted a percentage score which
reflected the judge's certainty of the 'match.' The total of 100 percent
could be divided among any or none of the [targets]. We then totaled all
five judges' assignments for each [response] to find which [targets] scored
the highest for each [response]."
Data Analysis. The data were analyzed two ways. First, the likelihood
that the correct target received the highest score by chance was calculated.
Second, the percentage scores were evaluated by a one-tailed T [sic] test
"to determine the probability that the assigned percentage scores for correct
and incorrect targets were due to chance."
suits. The results are shown in Table 17. The correct target was
assigned the highest score in 8 out of 33 responses. These were the
following responses, with subject's initials and target given in parentheses:
3 (I.S.,F); 6 (R.B.,D); 11 (R.B.,I); 16 (I.S.,H); 18 (I.S.,F); 21 (R.B.,J);
25 (R. .,D); and 27 (A.H.,D).
T e correct target was assigned the highest score in 8 of 33 cases.
chance is 3.3 cases, the outcome is stated by the authors to be
Approved For Release 2000/08HO: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
A~proved For Release 2000/08 DP96-00787R000100260003-2
TABLE 17. RESULTS OF VALLEE, et at. (1976)
OPEN (0)
SE BLIND (B)
SUBJECT
BEST
2ND
SCORES
3RD 4TH
5TH
TARGET
1
0
I.S.
J(134)
F(60)
C(50)
G(44)
D(30)
C
2
0
A.H.
A(100)
1(6.0)
F(52)
H(50)
C(45)
C
3
B
I.S.
F(212)
G(50)
C(10)
D(10)
H(10)
F
4
0
I.S.
J(100)
E(76)
F(55)
K(30)
A(10)
G
5
B
A.H.
K(70)
H(62)
G(60)
D
6 B
R.B.
D(284)
J(10)
D
7 B
R.T.
A(205)
1(90)
G(80)
D
8 B
I.S.
I(210)
J(40)
D
9 0
A.H.
11(150)
F(70)
J(50)
C(20)
I
10 0
I.S.
D(50)
J(50)
K(50)
E(48)
C(30)
I
11 0
R.B.
1(208)
D(30)
C(20)
11(20)
I
12 0
A.H.
F(45)
.E(20)
D(17)
E
13 0
R.B.
F(188)
E
14 B
R.B.
K(110)
D(56)
J(30)
H
15 B
A.H.
A(180)
D(10)
F(10)
H
16 B
I.S.
H(166)
H
17 0
R.B.
D(100)
F(90)
F
18 0
I.S.
F(246)
D(120)
1(20)
F
19 0
I.S.
G(104)
C(100)
J(84)
J
20 0
A.H.
F(30)
K(20)
D(6)
J
21 0 .
R.B.
J(56)
1(50)
D(30)
A(10)
C(10)
J
22 B
R.B.
11(62)
K
23 B
A.H.
D(52)
G(10)
J(10)
K
24 B
J.B.
C(4.0)
D(14)
K
25 0
R.B.
D(72)
J(10)
D
26 0
A.V.
J(80)
D(25)
D
27 0
A.H.
D(55)
E(5)
F(5).
K(2)
D
28 0
R.B.
1)(126)
II
29 0
A.H.
D(222)
F(10)
H
30 0.
I.S.
J(58)
C(30)
11(22)
A(10)
1(10)
H
31 B
A.11.
11(32)
E(30)
G(10)
I(6)
A
32 B
R.B.
C(130)
G(124)
A
33 0
R.B.
1(60)
D(16)
J(15)
K(6)
A
Approved For Release 2000/08/)4q: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 96-00787R000100260003-2
significant at p < .01. For verification, one can calculate the level o
significance, using the binomial:
33
p (n > 8) = 1 - [ E (3n) (0.1)n (0.9)33-n]
n = 8
(7 )
Thus p is just slightly better than .01.
For the analysis of percentage scores, the authors use a one-tailed T-test
to compare assigned percentage scores for correct vs. incorrect targets. They
report a p-value of 0.08; the value of T is not reported. They also state
that the individual p-values for subjects I.S. and R.B. were equal to 0.04.
Conclusions. The authors conclude that "about two-thirds of the [responses]
contained descriptive elements that corresponded with the correct target
specimen, but often these were mixed with noncorresponding elements ...."
Authors were encouraged by the "accurate and significant remote perception
[up to] ... 2500 miles away from the targets, ... [and] that the double
blind conditions provided equally correct descriptions ...."
Critical Evaluation. This experiment can be criticized on several
methodological grounds, including at least the following:
(1) Selection of the subjects, their backgrounds, their knowledge of
remote viewing phenomena, and the like are all unknown.
(2) Did they have communication with one another or with anyone else
regarding the experiment? Since the teleconferencing system was available
to them at all times, such communication is certainly possible.
(3) Who selected the targets? Are they a representative "random"
Approved For Release 2000/08/02: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
IMUnr
e
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 DP96-00787R000100260003-2
sample of North American minerals? Who else knew of the target pool? Where/
how were they stored during the experiment? If the observers could remotely
view the double blind targets in envelopes over the distances involved, it
seems reasonable that they could remotely view both open and double blind
targets at all times.
(4) The original five open targets were used once each; the original
five double blind targets were used twice each, once double blind and once
open,.with the double blind viewing first. This confounding of frequency
of usage with the double blind vs. open variable limits the sampling assumptions
of the data..
Secondly, the experimental analysis of the data can be criticized as
follows:
(1) Subject I.S. was correct three times, subject R.B. four times, and
subject A.H. once, out of 10 subjects. This allocation of the eight best
match correct responses seems somewhat extreme. For any given subject, the
likelihood of being correct can be evaluated.
For subject R.B., who contributed 11 responses (of the 33), the likelihood
of being correct, by chance, four or more times is:
11
p '(n > 4) = 1 - [ E (ln) (0.1)n (0.9)11-n]
n = 4
= .0028.
(8 )
For subject I.S., the similar probability is:
p (n >
9
1 - [ E (n) (0.1)n (0.9)9-nJ
n = 3
(9 )
Approved For Release 2000/08AV: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved. For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787R000100260003-2
Subject A.H. responded 10 times., and was correct once. With an a priori
probability of 1/10, little can be said for his performance, whether an author
or riot!
Note, however, that these three persons contributed 11 + 9 + 10 30 of.
the 33 transcripts. Subject R.T. contributed 1, subject J.B. contributed
1, and subject A.V. contributed 1, for a total of 33. Clearly, the overall
positive results are due entirely to subjects R.B. and I.S.
(2) The authors indicate that 8 correct out of 33 is significant at
p < .01, yet the binomial calculation yields a value of p = .004. The authors
do not state the test by which they calculated the p-value; it would have
been more scientific had they done so.
(3) The selection of the t-test for correct vs. incorrect scores is
awkward. The authors are not very explicit which correct and incorrect scores
they used, nor what value of t was obtained. One might reasonably assume
that the 15 correct matches in Table 17 were used, and compared with the
remaining 87 scores. If so, this should be evaluated by a non-correlated
score t-test, even though the scores are clearly not independent. Nevertheless,
following this approach:
t = 3.36, df = 100. (10)
The exact p-value for t = 3.36, df = 100 is .0011. The authors report
a p = .08, but do not indicate their basis of calculation.
For subject 1.S., using the same approach,
The exact.p-value for subject I.S. is .0055.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1'4 CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 . O@WWP96-00787ROO0100260003-2
Similarly, for subject R.B.,
t = 2.83, df = 25, p = .009.
All of the above p--values are two-tailed, but clearly very significant.
(12)
It is unclear to use what t calculation the authors could have used to
obtain p = .08 for the entire experiment data set. The only other plausible
possibility is to obtain a mean correct score and a mean incorrect score
for each subject (N = 6), and to analyze the difference between these means
by a paired-score t statistic. This analysis yields a mean, across subjects,
score of 56.17 for correct responses.and 64.17 for incorrect responses. While
the difference of 8.00 is not statistically significant (t = -0.25, df = 5,
p = .19), it is interesting to. note that the scores were generally higher for
incorrect responses!
Summary. This experiment is interesting in that it uses a different
task and different methodology. However, the lack of procedural detail,
coupled with the imprecise and inexact statistical analyses, cause us to
have less than complete faith in the results. Like many other experiments,
it would be quite impressive if the reporting were more detailed and the
methodology cleaner. As it is., it can only be considered suggestive of
the existence of long distance remote viewing.
L. Whitson, T; W., Bogart, D. N., Palmer, J., and Tart, C. T. Preliminary
experiments in group "remote viewing". Proceedings of the IEEE,
October 1976, pp. 1550-1551.
Purpose. The purposes were to check on the validity of remote viewing,
as reported by.Puthoff and Targ, and to test such in a group experiment.
Approved For Release 2000/0 V-R.;4A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/ DP96-00787R000100260003-2
Experiment One
Experimental Design. Students in an art class remotely viewed one
target site and attempted to select it from a set of 10 presented in color
slide form.
Subjects. Twenty-seven students in an art class at the University of
California, Davis.
Experimenter. T. Whitson.
Target Person. D. Bogart.
Judge. An employee of the university art department served as judge.
No other information is given.
Equipment. Sketching materials and color slides of the targets were
used. Slides were made by Whitson and Bogart.
Targets. "Thirty target locations were selected by the experimenters,
all within ten minutes driving time from the Davis campus. Of these thirty,
ten locations clearly differentiated from each other by visual criteria were
chosen as the target pool. A few examples of the target pool are: a palm
tree, a Hammond organ, a bike underpass tunnel, and a gravestone statue of
an angel.
Procedure. A color slide of each site was sealed in an envelope together
with traveling instructions from the university to the site. The experimenter
randomized the envelopes while the target person was not present.
The experimenter introduced the nature of the experiment at the beginning
of the class period and conducted the experiment after the class had been
drawing for two hours, reasoning that the subjects' visual imagery would be
more activated at this time.
The experimenters described the SRI studies and emphasized to the subjects
that remote viewing might be a widely distributed perceptual ability.
Approved For Release 2000,
CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/ ' - P96-00787R000100260003-2
KY. I* , I
When the experiment began, the target person selected one of the
envelopes, did not tell the experimenter what it was, traveled to the
target, and viewed it for 15 minutes. The subjects simultaneously "viewed"
the target and attempted to produce a "drawing of the images that corresponded
to the remote site." The experimenter collected the drawings and told the
subjects the results would be discussed at a later class meeting.
The target person returned, removed the remaining nine slides from the
envelopes, and rerandomized them with the target slide included. The
experimenter was not present at this time.
The judge was asked to match a first and second choice of the 10 possible
target slides to each drawing, as all 10 slides'were projected simultaneously.
After the judging was done, the target person "revealed the target site to
everyone concerned." [It is unclear whether this included the subjects.]
Data Analysis. First and second choices were counted as hits, a
"procedure decided upon before the analysis." The data are presented in
Table 18.
Results. Authors "were not able to apply a formal statistical test to
this single session ...." However, they were impressed that the correct
target received almost twice as many correct matches (11) as the next most
selected (6) target.
A binomial test can be applied, if one assumes that thea priori
probability of a hit, P, is 1/10. Then, there are 11 hits out of the 53
matches, and:
53
p (n > 11) = 1 - [ E (5n) (0.1)n (0.9)53-n]
n = 11
Approved For Release 2000/08/ Q : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 3 P96-007878000100260003-2
TABLE 18. EXPERIMENTS ONE AND TWO (IN PARENTHESES) RESULTS
TARGET
FIRST CHOICE
SECOND CHOICE*
TOTAL
ANGEL
1
(0)
3
(0)
4
(0)
BANJO
1
(3)
4
(3)
5
(6)
BEAN POLE
3
(1)
2
(2)
5
(3)
BIKES (TARGET, 2)
3
(3)
3
(1)
6
(4).
BIKE TUNNEL (TARGET, 1)
5
(1)
6
(0)
11
(1)
DIRT MOUNDS
3
(0)
2
(2)
5
(2)
LOGS
4
(2)
0
(1)
4
(3)
ORGAN
3
(4)
2
(4)
5
(8)
PALM TREE
1
(0)
3
(0)
4
(0)
TRACTOR
3
(0)
1
(1)
4
(1)
*One second choice not given in Experiment One.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 o48CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08110 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000.100260003-2
Thus, assuming no drawing bias on the part of the subjects, these
results are statistically significant at the p < .01 level of confidence.
Experiment Two
Purpose. If, by chance alone, the target selected in the first experiment
happened to meet a predominant drawing bias of the subjects, the results
would be artifactually inflated. Thus, the second experiment was a replication
of the first, and was also intended to see if images of tunnels (the first
experiment target) occurred frequent when the target was not a tunnel.
Experimental Design. Same as first experiment.
Subjects. Fourteen different students in a different art class.
Experimenter. Same as Experiment One.
Target Person. Same as Experiment One.
Judge. A graduate student in the art department.
Equipment. Same as Experiment One.
Targets. Same as in Experiment One, except that the tunnel was precluded
as the target.
Procedure. Same as in Experiment One, but Whitson noted before the
data were analyzed that "this class seemed less interested and involved in
the experiment than did the first class."
Data Analysis. No separate analysis was run on this experiment. However,
the combined data are analyzed, as follows.
Results. Table 18 also indicates the results of this experiment, which
appear to be not as good as in the first experiment. The authors conclude
that "of all possible target pairs in both sessions combined, the total
number of matches assigned to the actual target pair was the third highest
Approved For Release 2000/
RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
of the 90 possible pairs. This is associa e with a one-tailed probability
of 0.033."
Conclusions. The authors conclude that the results offer modest support
to Puthoff and Targ's results and that they intend to do further experiments.
Critical Evaluation. The statistical analysis given by the authors is
somewhat unusual. Apparently, they determined that the total number of
possible pairings (permutations) of the targets in the two experiments is 90,
and that the 11 (first experiment) + 4 (second experiment) = 15 correct is
the third highest total. However, the first highest total would be 11 + 8 = 19,
while the second highest total would be 11 + 6 = 17. In each of these cases,
plus the third (actual) case, the 11 comes from the first experiment and the
second component from the second experiment. Given that the first experiment
was already conducted, one can look at the results of the second experiment
by the binomial theorem as was done above for the first experiment. Then,
28
p (n > 4) = 1 - [ E (28) (0.1)n (0.9)28-n~
n = 4
(14)
This result is clearly not significant by any statistical criterion.
Similarly, were the decision made a priori to combine the two experiments,
then the binomial can be used to evaluate the combined number of hits (15)
out of the total "matches" (81), to obtain
81
p (n > 15) = 1 - [ E (8n) (0.1)n (0.9)81-n]
n = 15
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 pclA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
r L
(15)
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CW%&-00787R000100260003-2
This calculation, while laborious, would appear valid if the decision were
in fact made prior to the conduct of the first experiment to combine the
data of both experiments. Clearly, such a decision was not made as the
authors so indicate in the purpose of conducting the second experiment.
Thus, any combination of the data from both studies, after noting that the
first produced positive results, has dubious validity.
In his book Psi, Tart (1977) describes these experiments in essentially
the same form as in the previously published (and copyrighted) papers. In
evaluating the first experiment, Tart states "That [the result] seems
quite significant statistically. I say `seems' because there are some
technical problems involved in exactly what assumptions are valid for making
a statistical test; for that reason, we did not make a formal evaluation,
although it would have been extremely significant" (p. 166). His problem
is that of response bias, for example, if the Davis art students drew tunnels
frequently. Of course, this response bias is inherent in the procedure but
could be considered eliminated, over many experiments, by random target
selection.
Following the second experiment, Tart (1977) concludes "It is statistically
legitimate to combine the results of the two experiments for an overall
evaluation, and we did so" (p. 168). If the first experiment suffered from
response bias, and therefore null hypothesis chance probability estimate
inaccuracies, combining the data of the second experiment with those of the
first would not give the first set of experimental data instant respectability!
It would appear that either the authors (or editors) are statistically
naive, or that the authors have combined the data in a form to result in a
positive verification of remote viewing. As such, the results must be taken
.most cautiously.
Approved For Release 2000/08/
RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 CI -00787R000100260003-2
Summary. This paper is but another example of nonconventional analysis
of paranormal experimental results. 'I'lte statistical analysis causes the
reader to be skeptical of the other parts of the experiment. Otherwise,
the methodology, tersely presented as it is, seems consonant with that of
Puthoff and Targ.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1
96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08110 : 'h6-00787R000100260003-2
IV. EVALUATION OF POTENTIAL CRITICISMS
Sections II and III of this report have summarized all pertinent
known research. The SRI work summarized and critiqued in detail in
Section II was clearly given more attention than the assorted papers
in Section III. Two reasons exist for this differential attention.
First, SRI researchers Puthoff and Targ have done more research,
received more funding and attention, published more, and generally
advanced the research concepts more than have the other investigators.
Other experimenters have used their basic protocol, with occasional
additions and deletions. In a word, they`are the reigning experts.
Secondly, other publications, with the exception of the well
written manuscript by Karnes and Susman, lack sufficient detail to
perform a critically detailed evaluation. (The SRI work is not reported
in sufficient detail either, but the multiple reporting provides more
information than that provided by other investigators.)
Because a great amount of attention has been drawn by the SRI work,
Puthoff and Targ have also received their share of criticism. To
combat this criticism, Puthoff has published a one page summary entitled
"Potential Criticisms and Responses." It is well done, although our
preceding evaluations tend to disagree with some of his "responses."
Since it serves as a good summary of research philosophy, that paper is
used in this Section of our report as a "straw man" for overall evaluation
of the SRI methodology. Each potential criticism (Cl through C9) is
repeated below, along with Puthoff's responses (Rl through R9) and our
pertinent comments.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?!3IA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : %18 96-00787R000100260003-2
MCURLI
The use of this rebuttal'method by Puthoff, however, does tend to
draw attention away from other areas of potential criticism as it makes
it appear as if these are the only methodological areas of potential
criticism. Such is not the case, as has been hopefully demonstrated in
Section II. The potential criticisms and responses, on the other hand,
are sufficiently important to warrant separate discussion here.
Experiment Selection
Cl: The.experiments discussed could be selected out of a larger
pool of experiments of which many are of poor quality.
RI: Selection of experiments for reporting does not take place;
every experiment is entered as performed on a master log and
is included in the statistical evaluations.
Comment: Reported experiments, sketches, and the like are clearly and
understandably selected. There is no room in journal or open literature
reports for the 7000+ experiments run with Swann, nor for all experiments
conducted with other subjects. Unfortunately, as we have amply demonstrated
in Section II, many of the other experiments are not reported, even in
summary. form, anywhere. Further, statistical analyses are not given for
some experiments, and contain overlap for other series of experiments.
This criticism appears valid to us.
Data Selection
C2: Data for the reported experiments could be edited to show only
the matching elements, the nonmatching elements being discarded.
R2: Data associated with a given experiment remain unedited;. all
experiments are tape recorded and all data (tape transcripts,
Approved For Release 2000/08/102!
RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787R000100260003-2
drawings, clay models) are included unedited in the data
package to be judged and evaluated.
Comment: There is inconsistency, although sometimes minor, in the parallel
publication of the same quoted transcripts. Presumably, judging cues are
edited out (see Appendix B). This-has not been done consistently. This
criticism is at least partially valid. It. is also unclear as to how many
viewings are allowed both prior to and following an experiment. From
one publication, it appears that all transcripts are given to the judge.
Although all data may be given to the judges, other readers and audiences
are given selected data.
Cueing
C3: The study could involve naivete in protocol that permits various
forms of cueing, intentional or unintentional.
R3: The use of double-blind protocols ensures that none of the
persons in contact with the subject is aware of either the
particular target or target pool; similarly, no one in contact
with a judge is aware of the target-list/su.bject-output
correspondence. For example, judges are not taken to target
sites by a knowledgeable persons, but rather proceed to the
target sites, unaccompanied, on the basis of written instructions
generated without knowledge of subject output.
Comment: We have shown above that the questions and comments offered by
the experimenter could easily serve as perceived or subliminal (shaping)
cues. Similarly, we have shown that, in some experiments, the experimenter
does know something about the target pool or has helped to select it.
Approved For Release 2000/08LIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ` 6-00787R000100260003-2
ULUMV
Other concerns about judging procedures and available information have
already been raised.
.The.authors use the term "double-blind" frequently. Yet, they are
quite naive as to the behavioral science meaning of this term, a naivete
which is apparent throughout their publications. Traditionally, "double-
blind" refers to an experimenter who collects the data and who is "blind"
to the purpose, theory, and potential nature of the results of the
experiment. Similarly, the investigator is "blind" to the subjects, the
data per se, and the data recording, reduction, and analysis. Thus, the
experimenter has little influence on the results because he theoretically
does not know what should be obtained, while the investigator is suffi-
ciently blind to the direct subject contact and data so that he cannot
influence the results. They are both "blind" in a sense, thus "double-
blind." The word clearly does not apply in either the traditional, nor
in a meaningful, sense to the SRI protocol.
Educated Guess
C4: A subject may be able to guess as to which sites in a given
area are likely to be chosen as targets, and may have
familiarized himself with the locations.
R4: In the statistical judging procedure used, no advantage could
be gained even if a subject were to be given a list of possible
target sites beforehand and encouraged to familiarize himself
with the locations. Even in such an extreme hypothetical case
(no such procedure was ever used) where a subject could not
help but render a set of perfect descriptions of target sites,
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : t,l9k-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 96-00787R000100260003-2
he still has the basic statistical problem of generating
blind the correct target/description pair sequence upon which
the statistical evaluation is based.
Comment: The response is quite correct for the statistical evaluation
series. However, demonstration experiments, such as Grant's Tomb,
Supe'rdome, Washington Square, Ohio Caves, West Virginia Site, and all
foreign sites are not subject to statistical evaluation. Previous
comments have pointed out problems in the results for these targets.
Thus, the criticism is at least partially valid.
Target Limitations
C5: If a subject is given feedback after an experiment that
today's target was a fountain, he knows that the following
target is unlikely to be a fountain, since targets are chosen
for unique differentiable qualities.
R5: The target pool in use (> 100 target sites) contains several
fountains, several buildings, several parks, etc., and therefore
the content of a given target, determined by random. entry into
the target pool, is essentially independent of the contents of.
other targets.
Comment: Much has already been said about the local target pool, target
selection, etc. This criticism is at least partially valid.
As has been discussed earlier, the target pool did not seem to be
established prior to the beginning of all the experiments and subjects did
not have, say, two fountains, with the exception of two targets which
appeared once for two subjects. A sub-pool of targets was also selected
Approved For Release 2000/08/10e5CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA
00787R000100260003-2
from the larger pool; thus, this selection process may have eliminated
the possibility of more than one type of target appearing.
Target Generality
C6: Transcripts generated by subjects are so general as to match
anything. ("Sky is blue, grass is green.")
R6: Judging protocol involves differential matching. Therefore,
true but general statements do not help a judge to preferen-
tially assign a transcript to one site as opposed to another.
Comment: Again, this is a valid response for statistically judged targets,
but not for many others (e.g., Washington. Square versus Yankee Stadium).
It should be clear that many transcripts, in fact, match many targets;
i.e., the channel is noisy. Why then (and how) can so many excellent
responses occur?
"Read--in" Matches
C7: Given a transcript and a target, a judge can "read in" matches.
R7: Differential matching on a blind basis allows matches to be
"read in" equally for non-corresponding as well as corresponding
target/transcript pairs, and therefore provides no differential
advantage.
Comment: "Read-in" can occur for targets not judged statistically, as was
often the case. Experimenter cues in the transcript can be helpful here.
Such experimenter cueing, or the possibility thereof, must be eliminated
by protocol. revision.
Approved For Release 2000/08/102.cIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
rnnr.r
MEN ?EURTOPM
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : Clil 00787R000100260003-2
Inadequate Handling of Judging Materials
C8: Preparation of judging materials (transcript typing) may
provide opportunity for a "leak," or perhaps degradation of
typing ribbon may provide artifactual information as to
order of experiments.
R8: Transcript typing is carried out in a random order by
individuals kept blind to the key; one-time ribbons are used.
Comment: Typing cues are not nearly as important as transcript content and
judge's prior knowledge. How does the tape get from the experimental
room to the typist? The typed transcript from the typist to the judges?
How are both stored? Other "security" problems seem more important than
does the typewriter ribbon.
Post Hoc Photography
C9: Photographs used to illustrate remote viewing results are taken
after completion of the experiments, and therefore suffer from
the fallacy of post hoc matching.
R9: All blind judging, matching, and statistical evaluation of the
results (which is where the scientific issues are decided) are
completed before photographs are taken; judges do not have
access to photographs during their analysis, and therefore
judges cannot be cued into correspondences observed post hoc.
Comment: We have pointed out several temporal and content problems with
photographs. Was the San Andres airfield photograph taken after the
judging? Why do aspect angles of photographs always coincide with the
direction from which the subject "views" the target?
Approved For Release 2000/08/1025tlA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 596-00787R000100260003-2
The authors have "in five years of self- and other criticism, . .
not found a way to fault either the experimental protocols or the
conclusions derived therefrom." We do not agree, and we believe Section II
amply documents numerous such faults. '.rhe. next section offers guidelines,
within the general SRI approach, to improve this protocol and eliminate or
reduce many of the criticisms.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 zCIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10-: CI 6-00787R000100260003-2
Lengthy, careful study would be required to develop a safe, perhaps
foolproof protocol. Experience with this general type of research would
be required to refine the protocol further, to render it acceptable to
the behavioral science research community. Such is beyond our scope and
responsibility in this report, and perhaps beyond our capability.
Rather, we offer the following suggestions for improvement in the
experimental protocol used generally by Puthoff and Targ (and others).
We believe that these improvements, when used in the context of a "local
area" series of experiments, will yield valid results which can be used.
to address questions of channel capacity, phenomenon existence, learning
rates, and the like. Thus, the suggested improvements are classified
by experimental operation, much as is the SRI protocol (Appendix B).
A. Target Pool. Selection
As suggested in Appendix B, to carry out a series of n experiments,
the target pool should be >> n. The target pool should be selected prior
to the experiment and should contain distinctive targets. Once distinctive
targets are chosen, however, there should be other similar targets
selected, such as several fountains. These should have specific,
individual details so that a general fountain description will not apply
very well. Most important, the target pool should be selected by someone
not involved with the experiment and unknown to the experimenters,
investigators, subjects, or judges. Further, the experimenters et al.
should not know the size of the target pool.
Approved For Release 2000/08/102bCIA-RDP96-00787R00'0100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Ideally, the targets and their locations should be totally unfamiliar
to the experimenters, investigators, subjects, and judges. For example,
the targets could be selected in and the experiments conducted in a city
totally unfamiliar to the above individuals. In this manner, cueing and
reading-in are less likely. Each target should be listed on a separate
card and should include what aspects of the target are to be viewed,
the fountain in a plaza, and from what viewpoint. The particular
distinguishing aspects should also be noted as well as unique, meaningful
behavior of a target person for that specific target. The description
should then be enclosed in an opaque envelope and sealed. The envelopes
should then be thoroughly randomized. No numbering system is necessary.
The targets should be stored in a safe or container inaccessible to the
experimenters, investigators, subjects, and judges. Further, the location
of the safe or container should be unknown to the experimenters at al.
B. Tnvestigator
This is the person or persons who designs the experiments and is
familiar with the literature. He does not collect data, select targets,
prepare transcripts, analyze data, or in any way interact with elements
of the experiment in a manner by which he might deliberately or
unintentionally affect the experiment or its outcome. In a word., he
remains "hands off."
C. Subjects
Subjects can be experienced or inexperienced, as the purposes of the
experiment dictate. As long as subjects remain totally uninvolved in
other aspects of the experiments, their characteristics are less important.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1026elA-RDP96-00787R000.100260003-2'
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CJ6-00787R000100260003-2
They should not serve also as experimenters, judges, coauthors, and
target persons.
Further, they should not be close friends of the experimenters,
investigators, or judges. With such a lack of personal familiarity,
idiosyncratic behavior by the subjects or investigators is less likely
to serve as a useful cue to the judge.
It is assumed that an intelligence application of remote viewing
would, necessarily and desirably, use the same subject(s) repeatedly.
Thus, successful subjects should logically serve consistently in that
capacity. However, while in a research mode, when the information channel.
is being quantified, care must be taken to avoid artifactual results due
to data contamination from subject/experimenter communication. The lack
of repeated use of Targ as a subject is thus supported, even though he
provided an excellent response to the San Andres airport. (One must
wonder why he wasn't used again in view of this highly accurate response!)
D. Experimenters
Although we fail to see the need for an experimenter to be present
during the actual transcription, if one is used, this person must be
totally unfamiliar with the target pool, selection procedure, target
person, and as many of the other details of the experiment as possible.
A defined procedure should be established to make the subject feel at
ease, and assure him/her that remote viewing is acceptable. Although it
would appear unnecessary to repeat this procedure with experienced
subjects, in order to keep this portion of the experiment standardized,
it would be best to repeat these instructions. No previous results should
Approved For Release 2000/08/102~CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: 0N6-00787R000100260003-2
be shown. in addition, a subject should not be told what kinds of
elements or aspects are to be used in their description of the target,
but rather to describe the perception of the target as accurately as
possible.
If an experimenter is present during the actual transcription, a
pre-set list of innocuous questions might be used. These should be used
only if the subject seems to be totally unable to continue describing
any aspect of the target.
E. Target Persons
If one or more target. persons are used, the number of these should
be specified in advance and then remain constant. It is understandable
that they must be known to the subject. However, this does not mean
that they must be present at the site from which the viewing takes place.
Since the subject does not "track" the target person prior to the start
of the experiment, every effort should be made to keep the subject and
target.person at a maximimal physical distance before, during, and after
an experiment. This is easily accomplished if the targets are located at
a physical distance, such as in another city.
The target person should receive the target from a person totally
unconnected with the experiment and unfamiliar to the experimenter. This
person would not know the contents of the target pool and would select,
on a predefined random basis, one envelope from the target pool. This
person would relay the target envelope to the target person at a predesig-
nated location distant from the location of the target pool and the target.
An experiment would begin at a predesignated hour on predesignated
days, the number of which would also be preset, for both individual subjects
as well as for the total experiment.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10264-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ? 96-00787R000100260003.-2
Targets should be used without replacement, essentially for the
reasons stated in Appendix B.
The target person should proceed to the designated target and view
the preselected elements of that target as specified on the. target card.
The target person should do only these prespecified activities, which
should be uniquely meaningful interactions with that particular target.
F. Subject Responses
The subject should begin his/her description of the target at the
predesignated viewing time. No prior viewing should be allowed. The
description should be tape-recorded and should include all experimenter
questions if an experimenter is present, although, again, we see no need
for such. (An uninterrupted videotape should also be used to verify the
absence of nonverbal experimenter cueing.) A subject should be allowed
to sketch or model if he/she so desires, but this should also be
predetermined by the subject and held constant for each experiment. A
subject may be encouraged to be as specific as possible, but not told
what kinds of elements to include. Only one viewing should be allowed.
G. Feedback
Feedback and no-feedback experiments should be conducted. In a
feedback situation, only the subject (not the experimenter) should receive
the feedback. The. contents of the target envelope can be transmitted to
the subject.. Neither the target person nor the experimenter with the
subject need have any knowledge of the subject's response to the target
or a description of the target.
Approved For Release 2000/08/102661A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
-VLULJL'_
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
The tape recordings and sketches or models should be dated, sealed,
and immediately forwarded to an independent person totally unrelated to
the experiment and unfamiliar with all persons thus far associated with
the experiment. This individual should hold all data until the judging
procedure is complete. This person will also type and edit the tapes,
eliminating all references to previous targets, including any experimenter's
question, should they not conform to the criteria for experimenter's
questions. A target description should be included with the packet.
H. Judges and Judging
Effort should be placed on the development of objective judging
criteria, perhaps measured by an item count or content analysis, semantic
content, or other techniques better known to psycholinguists. Type/token
ratio approaches might be modified to meet these needs. In any case,
objective criteria for the judging procedure would greatly reduce the
subjective element in this phase of the experiment.
Several judges should be chosen who are unfamiliar with the
experiment and unknown to those who have participated thus far. While
they may be selected on the basis of certain personal attributes (e.g.,
artistic ability, intelligence, sponsor representatives
they should
have no professional interest in the research. That is, they should not
be magicians, consultants to the project, coauthors, fellow researchers, etc.
Each judge should proceed to each target location, ordered randomly
with the edited tape, associated drawings or models, and the target
description card. No judge should be given a list of the targets. Each
judge should proceed to the targets in a different random order. At each
target, the judge should rank all transcripts against that target, as
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : 1A-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CAMAMM-00787ROO0100260003-2
of response criteria, in that there is-always some degree of interpreta-
tion of what a subject says,. the increased use of.judges seems beneficial
and logical.
J. Target Coordinates
If scanning by geographical coordinates is to be used, the coordinates
must be selected by an unimpeachable person not otherwise connected with
the experiment or familiar with other persons related to the experiment.
The coordinates should describe a variety.of targets so that a subject may
not try to guess a particular type, some of which should describe innocuous
sites. Preferably these should also vary in. geographical location such
that a subject could not memorize detailed maps of any given geographical
area. A possible approach is selection, by random number, of a large
(> 5000) list of worldwide targets of interest.
The coordinates should be transmitted just prior to the viewing time.
No maps and no feedback during the experiment should be allowed. Again,
there appears to be no need for an experimenter to be present. The
subject should complete his viewing in a pre-set time period and only one
viewing should be allowed. Details of the viewing should be relayed
immediately via a secure computer network or other similar form of
communication. Sketches should be dated, notarized by time, and mailed
immediately.. Again, if feedback is used, only the subject should be
given feedback.
K. P,eport t ng
A major problem with research in this field is the incomplete,
inexact, erroneous, and duplicate reporting. AZZ experimental details,
Approved For Release 2000/08/1026tIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 Y&Bff 96-00787R000100260003-2
00 1
responses, :instructions, transcripts, etc., must be reported, however
lengthy and laborious the task might be Only in this manner will the
"loyal opposition" be able to satisfy their desire for facts and
reanalysis. Only then will they have left only a malfeasance or
dishonesty criticism.
L. Application to Intelligence Systems
Research conducted and reported to date has a number of inaccuracies,
i?nconsistenciQs, and methodological weaknesses sufficient to cause concern
over its validity. If all results are accepted without question, on
balance the fidelity of the remote viewing channel appears to be of
limited intelligence value. However, that conclusion may be totally
premature due to the insufficient methodologies used. To assess validly
the value of the remote viewing channel for operational use, much more
careful research is required, perferably by several laboratories following
the same protocols with detailed documentation. It would be particularly
desirable to have different researchers (i.e., laboratories) conduct
experiments with the same experienced subjects (e.g., Swann or Hanmlid).
In this manner, the reliability of the remote viewing channel can be
assessed, with a "known capability" subject, yet satisfy the demands of
the "loyal opposition" by having replication of the research by an
independent research team using the same protocol.
269
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : USAM-00787ROO0100260003-2
Allen, S., Green, P., Rucker, K., Cohen, R., Goolsby, C., and Morris, R. L.
A remote viewing study using a,modified version of the SRI procedure.
'In, J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in
Parapsychology, 1975. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1976, 46-48.
Bisaha, J. P. and Dunne, B. J. Multiple subject and long distance remote
viewing of geographical locations. In Proceedings of the International
Conference on Cybernetics and Society, IEEE, 1977, 512-516. (a)
Bisaha, J. P. and Dunne, B. J. Precognitive remote viewing in the Chicago
area: A.replication of the Stanford experiment. In J. D. Morris,
W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1976.
Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1977, 84-86. (b)
Dunne, B. J. and Bisalia, J. P. Multiple channels in precognitive remote
viewing. In W. G. Roll (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1977.
Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1978, 146-151.
Gardner, M. Mathematical games. Scientific American, October 1975, 11.4-.118.
Hastings, A. C. Mental processing of ESP imagery: Theoretical considera-
tions. In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research
in Parapsychology, 1975. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1976,
187-190.
Jahn, R. G. Psychic process, energy transfer, and things that go bump
in the night. Princeton Alumni Monthly, 4 December 1978, S-1 to S-12.
Karnes, E. W. and Susman, E. P. Remote viewing: A response bias
interpretation. Unpublished manuscript, Metropolitan State College,
Denver, CO, 1978.
May, .E., Targ, R., and Puthoff, H. Possible EEG correlates to remote
stimuli under conditions of sensory shielding. In Proceedings, IEEE
V ectro/77, Special Session on State of the Art in Psychic Research.
IEEE, 1977, SS/3, 1-5.
Mitchell, E. D. Psychic exploration. New York: Putnam, 1974.
Morris, R. L. An exact method for evaluating preferentially matched :free-
response mater. ial.. Journal of the American Society for Psychical
Research, L972, 66, 401-407.
Puthof.t, H. E. Progress Report #1 on SRI Project 3183, 12 March 1974.
Puthoff, H. E. Progress Report #2 on SRI Project 3182, 24 April 1974.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 cCIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 6-00787R000100260003-2
Puthoff, H. E. Progress Report #3 on SRI? Project 3183, August 1974.
Puthoff, H. E. and Targ, R. PK experiments with Uri Geller and Ingo
Swann. In W. G. Roll, R. L. Morris, and J. D. Morris (Ed.),
Research in Parapsychology; ON. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow
Press, 1974, 125-128.
Puthoff, H. and Targ, R. Remote viewing of natural targets. In J. D.
Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology,
1974. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1975, 30-32. (a)
Puthoff, H. and Targ, R. Physics, entropy, and psychokinesis. In
Proceedings, International Conference on Quantum Physics and Para-
psychology. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1975, 127-144. (b)
Puthoff, H. and Targ, R. Final report:. Perceptual Augmentation Techniques
(U). SRI Project 3183. Menlo Park, CA: Stanford Research Institute,
1 December 1975. (SECRET) (c)
Putiic)ff, H. and Targ, R. Precognitive remote viewing. In J. D. Morris,
W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1975.
Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1976, 37-40. (a)
Puthoff, H. and Targ, R. A perceptual channel for information transfer
over kilometer distances: Historical perspective and recent research.
.Proceedings of. the IEEE, 1976, 64, 329-354.. (b)
Puthoff, H. and Targ, R. Direct perception of remote geographical
locations. In Proceedings, IEEE EZectro/77, Special Session on
State of the Art in Psychic Research, IEEE, 1977, SS/2, 1-16.
Puthoff, H.; Targ, R., and May, E. Advanced threat technique assessment(U)..
Report on Project 5309. Menlo Park, CA: Stanford Research Institute,
July 1977 (SECRET).
Puthoff, H., Targ, R., and May, E. P'sychoenergetic research: Suggested
approaches. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International, 1 May 1978.
Rausche:r, E. A., Weissmann, G., Sarfatti, J., and Sirag, S.-P. Remote
perception of natural scenes, shielded against ordinary perception.
In J. D. Morris, W. G.. Roll, and R. L. Morris (Ed.), Research in
Parapsychology, 1975. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1976, 41-45.
Siegel, S. Nonparametrie statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.
Solfvin, G.., Roll, W. G., and Krieger, J. Meditation and ESP: Remote
viewing. In W. G. Rol.1_(Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1977.
Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1978, 151-157.
Swann, I. To kiss earth good-bye. New York: Hawthorne Books, 1975.
Targ, R. Precognition in everyday life: A physical model. In W. G. Roll,
R. L. Morris, and J. D. Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, .7972.
Metuchen., NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1973, 49-51.
271
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 41086-00787R000100260003-2
Targ, R. Precognition in everyday life: A physical model. Proceedings,
.International Conference of Parapsychology and the Sciences. New
York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1974, 251-258.
Targ, R. and Cole, P. Use of an automated stimulus generator to teach
extrasensory perception. In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll., and R. L.
Morris (Ed.), Research in Parapsychology, 1974. Metuchen, NJ: The
Scarecrow Press, 1975, 27-29.
Targ, R., Cole, P., and Puthoff, H. E. Development of techniques to
enhance man/machine communication. SRI Technical Report, August
1974. Referenced in M. Gardner, Mathamatical Games, Scientific
American, October 1975, p. 114.
Targ, R. and Hurt, D. Learning, clairvoyance and precognition with an
extrasensory teaching machine. Parapsychology Review, 1972, 3,
July 9-11.
Targ, R., May, E., Puthoff, II., Galin, D., and Ornstein, R. Sensing of
remote EM sources (Physiological correlates). Technical Report on
Project 4540. Menlo Park, CA: SRI International, April 1978.
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. E. ESP experiments with Uri Geller. In W. G.
Roll, R. L. Morris, and J. D. Morris (Ed.), Research in. Parapsychology,
.7973. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1974, 57-60. (a)
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. Information transmission under conditions of
sensory shielding. Nature, 1974, 251, 602-607. (b)
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. Remote viewing of natural targets. In
Proceedings, International. Conference on Quantum Physics and Para-
psychoZogy. New York: Parapsychology Foundation, 1975, 151-170.
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. E. Replication sutdy of the remote viewing
natural t
of
argets. In J. D. Morris, W. G. Roll, and R. L. Morris
Research in P
(Ed.),
arapsychology, 1975. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow
Press, 1976, 33-37. (a)
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. Reply to Martin Gardner. Scientific American,
January 1976 (letters). (b)
Targ, R. and Puthoff, H. E., Mind-Reach: Scientists Zook at psychic
ability. New York: Delacorte Press, 1977.
Targ, R., Puthoff, H., and May, E. State of the art in remote viewing
studies at SRI, In Proceedings, International Conference on Cyber-
netics and Society, IEEE, 1977, 519-529.
Tart, C. T. Psi. New York: E. P. Dutton, 1977.
Approved For Release 2000/08/1Zb CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 6-00787R000100260003-2
Vallee, J., Hastings, A. C., and Askevold, G. Remote viewing experiments
through computer conferencing. Proceedings of the IEEE, 1976,
1551-1552.
White, R. A. Surveys in parapsychology: Review of the literature, with
updated bibliographies. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press, 1976.
Whitson, T. W., Bogart, D. N., Palmer, J., and Tart, C. T. Preliminary
experiments in group "remote viewing." Proceedings of the IEEE,
1976, 1550-1551.
Wortz, E. C., Bauer, A. J., Blackwelder, R. F., Eerkens, J. W., and
Saur, A. J. Novel biophysical information transfer mechanisms
(NBIT). AirResearch Technical Document 76-13197, 14 January 1976.
273
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : GHHW6-00787R000100260003-2
APPENDIX A. SUMMARY AND EVALUATION OF MORRIS' (1972)
FREE-RESPONSE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE
Several techniques have been devised to permit evaluation of the
correspondence between stimuli (i.e., targets) and responses (i.e.,
transcripts) to estimate the extent to which any given transcript is
descriptive of any given target. Previous researchers have derived
parametric statistical techniques for such, based upon the likelihood
that a given number of matches of transcripts to targets would occur by
chance. Others have developed rating or confidence scales to analyze
such data. Most of these tests assume independence of matching (i.e.
sampling with replacement) although Stuart (1942) devised a critical ratio
test to handle those cases in which the judge's ratings or responses were
not completely independent.
This independency problem is exemplified`by Stuart's example of a
tendency for a judge to avoid assigning any transcript a ranking of one
for more than one target. If a judge has ranked Transcript A number one
for Target A', Transcript Bnumber one for Target B', Transcript C number
one for Target C', and there are four targets and transcripts, then he
is unlikely-to rank anything other than Transcript D number one for Target
While parametric tests have been devised to handle such dependencies,
they are distribution based and have a small error in them, an error
which becomes larger as N becomes small.
Morris (1972) offers a general formula for calculating the exact
probability of a given sum (or less) of ranks for the preferential matching
approach. lie also provides a table [or representative situation calculations.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :
it
DP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : ClAM0787R000100260003-2
Assume the procedure whereby there are four targets (A' through D')
and four transcripts (A through D) which must be blindly matched, and that
the four transcripts must be ranked one through four for each target. Then
the correct ranks, summed across all four targets, can vary from 4 to 16.
The data matrix is shown in Table Al. Following the procedural requirements,
the sum in each target column is 1 + 2 + 3 + 4 = 10. The diagonal (underlined)
scores are the only ones used in the calculation of the summed ranks; thus,
the sum of ranks in this example is 6. In general, if there are n targets
(and n transcripts), the sum of ranks can vary from n to n2, with an expected
value under the null hypothesis of (I + 2 + ... + n).
TABLE Al. EXAMPLE OF PREFERENTIAL RANKS MATRIX
TARGETS
TRANSCRIPTS
A'
B'
C'
D'
1
3
C
3
4
2
4
D
4
2
1
In general, let
s = the obtained sum of the diagonal ranks,
N = the number of transcripts,
n the number of targets, and
k zero and all positive integers not exceeding (s - n)/N.
5
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIX-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release.2000/08/10.: CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
S N
P (< s) = E E. (-1)k (Q) (Z NQ1- 1)
= n k = 0 n
s - n
1 s N
E F
Nn i=n k=0
n! (i-NI-1)!
(k! (n - P,) (n - 1)!(i - Nk - n)!) (A3)
For the example in-Table Al, this equation is equal to:
k
4
1
4
Prob. (< 8) _ 4 E (-1)
! 2)!)(3'(j -
4)!
4k -
(x!(4
256 (1+4-10)
When values of k and s become large, the calculations become laborious,
although not complex. Morris (1972) has calculated the critical values of
s for one-tailed p values ranging from 0.20 to 10-7, assuming that N = n.
If n N, the above equation (3) must be calculated, as it must for exact
probability values or N > 12.
For the example given in Table Al, Morris' table gives a value of
.05 < p .10, which agrees with the obtained exact p value.
The method is statistically sound, although the Morris (1972) tabled
values do not permit exact p-value determination. The diligent researcher
would undoubtedly choose to perform the precise calculations by using
equation (A3).
277
Approved For Release 2000/084RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
(A4)
Approved For Release 2000/08/10: CIA-RD~P966--00787R000100260003-2
It must be noted that this statistical test is valid only if the rankings
are assigned independently for each target. As Morris points out (p. 406),
the obtained p-values should be used only as a rough approximation in the
case of one judge ranking a constant response transcript set to a constant
target pool. This caution is emphasized especially in the. case that (1) N
is six or less, or (2) the judge has previously not assigned any transcript
a rank of one or more than one occasion.
The first caution (N < 6) does not violate the sampling distribution of
the statistic; rather, it suggests that a judge is more likely to be
influenced by his memory of rank of transcripts applied to previous targets
when the number of transcripts is small. When the number of transcripts is
larger than 6, presumably the uncertainty increases to the extent that the
judge's rankings approximate independent responses. No data are offered to
support this notion.
The second caution is simply another means to assess the independence
of the judge's rankings. If he has not redundantly ranked the same
transcript one before, there is evidence he is'not behaving independently,
i.e., ranking with replacement. The caution seems reasonable.
Morris further indicates that either (1) or (2) is particularly pertinent
if more than one-third of the number one rankings are correct and therefore
contributing substantially to the small value of s. When a single judge
and constant target pool are used, other statistical procedures should be
devised and used, contrary to current practice among researchers.
Improvements on the Method
Two general techniques are validly offered by Morris (1972) to solve
the nonindependence problem. In the first method, separate judges might
278
Approved For Release 2000/08klW ? - DP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
be used.for each "ranking of the targets." This wording would suggest
that a single judge rank all targets against a given response transcript.
Such a procedure would involve sequential visits to all targets and
necessarily rely upon the judge's memory for at least some target details.
A better method would be to have a single judge rank all transcripts for
a given target, thereby having no knowledge of the other targets in the
pool or how the same set of transcripts might be ranked for any other target.
The second methodological improvement requires that a judge be given
one response transcript and its target (unknown) plus "other similar non-
target materials which are changed from one ranking to the next. That is,
the judge'might receive Transcript A along with materials describing
Target A' and nontargets E', F', G', etc. (Table Al). If the number of
targets (plus nontargets) is large, then,n > N, but equation (3) can still
be applied.
As N becomes large (that is, the. number of targets in an N = n experiment
becomes large), the judge's task becomes more difficult in the "standard"
protocol.; therefore, it may be more practical to increase n than N, and
let each judge rank on only one target. A good rule of thumb, suggested
by Morris (1972), might be to not use this exact test when nN < 35.
Conclusion
While Morris (1972) is an important paper, and his analysis technique
is followed by many researchers, there remains.cause for concern. Certainly,
it is more desirable to calculate the exact probability of a given $ than to
use the tabled value,.and the calculation is not very complex or demanding.
Of.greater importance is the problem of nonindcpendence of rankings by
the same judge. Most researchers disregard this problem, others argue it
279
Approved For Release 2000/08/1
P96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
away by indicating that some judges do in fact rank the same transcript
"one" on two or more targets. Neither is an acceptable approach; the
second argument merely points out that independence existed (or a "mistake"
was realized by the judge) on one specific set of responses. What is
needed is a more thorough measure of exact probability which takes into
account the degree of nonindependence, much as a covariant might be used
in parametric analysis to remove confounded sources of variation.
Perhaps of the greatest heuristic concern in this method is its partial
use of the data. For the case where n = N,'only n of the n2 data points
(ranks) are used. The (n 2 - n) unused data becomes large as n increases.
For example, in the n = 4 case, only 25% of the rankings enter into the
analysis. In the n = 9 case, only 11% of rankings are used! An exact
probability method based on the correlational relationship in the total
data matrix should be developed. It would potentially provide greater
sensitivity and more confidence among readers unfamiliar with this particular
280
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 :
0787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-R
References
7R000100260003-2
Morris, R. L. An exact method for evaluating preferentially matched
.free-response material. Journal of the American Society for
Psychical Research, 1972, 66, 401-407.
Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.
Stuart, C. E. An ESP test with drawings. Journal of Parapsychology,
1942, 6, 20-43.
Uspensky, J. V. Introduction to Mathematical Probability. New York:
McGraw-Hill, 1937.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 ?blA-R4 7R000100260003-2
, 4W
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA 87R000100260003-2
The following is a verbatim copy of the remote viewing protocol,
as described in.a written communication from H. Puthoff to a sponsor
in late 1977.
Approved For Release 2000/08/10Z$CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
REMOTE VIEWING PROTOCOL
With. regard to replication of our standard remote viewing protocols,
the basic outline is as given in our tutorial paper, "A Perceptual
Channel for Information Transfer over Kilometer Distances: Historical
Perspective and Recent Research., " H. Puthoff and. R. Targ, Proc. IEEE,
pp. 329-354, March 1976 (4 copies enclosed). The elements of the
protocol, each of which I address below consist of (a) target pool
selection.; (b) subject orientation; (c) outbound experimenter behavior;
(d) inbound experimenter behavior; (e) post-experiment feedback;
(f) judging procedure.
(a) Target Pool Selection: To carry out an experimental series of,
say, n experiments with a subject, a list of outdoor targets >> n
should be prepared in advance by an experimenter who will not interact
with the subject or experiment after that. The targets should be
chosen to be distinctive, but not necessarily distinct from each other;
that is, rather than a collection of nondescript street corners one
should select bridges, towers, fountains, gardens, plazas, etc., so
that a judge could in principle recognize targets on the basis of
correct, but sketchy descriptions. On the other hand, once having
chosen a fountain-type target, there should be several fountain targets;
for a bridge target, several bridge targets, etc., so that you avoid
the subject strategy of "I had a tower yesterday, so it can't be a
tower today." In fact, the subject should be told explicitly that the
targets are not chosen to be orthogonal to each other.
When the target list is made, each target location should be written
on a card and placed in an envelope, the envelopes randomized and then
numbered so as to lose all track of a key. These should then be stored
in a secure safe or similar container.
With regard to whether a target is replaced in the pool after use,
there are two ways to go. The preferable one is to not replace it,
but keep near-replacement statistics by simply having a very large pool
with several similar targets, or else replacing a used fountain target
with another fountain target. The problem with straight replacement
is that the subject would, upon becoming aware of a mental image of a
previous target, be biased to reject it as memory. Therefore, even
though straight replacement makes some psychologists happy, it is
actually an artifact-producing procedure.
(b) Subject Orientation: Before the experiment, the subject should be
shown some previous remote viewing results with one goal in mind--to
get across the idea that one should, as best as possible, report raw
perception rather than analysis, since the former tends to be correct
and the latter is almost always wrong. Figures 4 and 6 in the IEEE
paper are good examples. In Figure 4 the subject had absolutely no
concept of a pedestrian overpass, but simply saw a pattern of receding
squares; the target in Figure 6 with passable drawings was interpreted
as a. restaurant; even the correctly dimensioned pools of water in
283
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
~ I
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : - 787R000100260003-2
Figure 3 were misinterpreted as purification plant pools rather than
recreational swimming pools. Reading of our book mind Reach (Targ and
Puthoff, Delacorte Press, 1977) provides a good background, as we go
into this aspect in much more detail than we could afford in our
technical papers. They need to "get it" that a rounded piece of blue
metal is just that, and they shouldn't try to figure out whether it is
a car fender before they say anything.' Remind the subject that imagina-
tion constitutes noise in the channel, and therefore the closer he can
get to raw uninterpreted imagery, the better. To have success in the
above, the best guideline we have Found is to choose as subjects
individuals who are self-confident, not afraid to be wrong, uninhibited,
etc. No psychological test we have tried (and we have tried them all)
is as successful as the above subjective assessment when it comes to
choosing subjects. Artist types used to unevaluated observation are
among the most successful.
(c) Outb_ound Experimenter Behavior: At the start of an experimental
session, the inbound and outbound experimenters and subject should
rendezvous for a relaxed informal chat in the laboratory setting. (The
outbound experimenter or experimenters must not know the target at
this time.) Together they agree on a time for the subject description
to start. (E.g., 30 minutes hence--the length of time required for
getting to the furthest target in the pool. This time is then an
invariant for all experiments.) The outbound experimenter then leaves
the lab, uses a random number generating procedure to obtain a number
from 1 - ...(number of targets in pool), obtains the so-numbered
envelope from the target pool (preferably kept by another person) and
leaves the premises. (We use a Texas Instruments SR-51 hand calculator
which has a random number function.) . After driving away from the
laboratory, he opens the envelope to determine the target,.and proceeds
to that location. I. suggest he arrange to park and then come upon the
target .:location at exactly the starting time so his view of it is
fresh at experiment beginning. He then simply pays attention to the
environment and does not let his mind wander (especially to another
target). It appears not to matter how many people comprise the
outbound team, provided they don't (1) just pay attention to each
other or (2) scatter about. At the end of the agreed-upon target
viewing time they return to the lab (usually 15 minutes).
(d) Inbound Experimenter Behavior: During the period that the outbound
experimenters are enroute to the target, the inbound experimenter and
subject have a period to relax and discuss the protocols. (Inbound
it is best not to have additional observers.) The goal of the inbound
experimenter during this period is to make it "safe" for the subject
to experience remote view:i.ng. This typically includes a Iow-key pep
talk as to how remote viewing; appears to be a natural., not abnormal,
CufCLLou, that many people appear to have done it successfully, even
their first time, and always the reminder to eschew analysis and
simply render raw impressions.
Approved For Release 2000/08/14 CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : C - 0787R000100260003-2
Since we think that remote viewing is a difficult task, like perceiving
a subliminal stimulus, we think it takes the full attentive powers of
the subject. Therefore, the environment, procedures, etc., should be as
natural and comfortable as possible so that as little attention as
possible is on anything other than the job at hand. No mumbo-jumbo,
hypnosis, strobe lights, or sensory-deprivation procedures, since all
these (novel) environmental factors take away some of the subject's
much-needed attention. We are in this sense proponents of a "naturalist
school." If the subject feels more comfortable smoking or drinking a
cup of coffee, why not? These should be arranged ahead of time, however,
so that neither subject nor experimenter leave the experimental room
while waiting for the outbound experimenter to reach his target..
The experimenter should have arranged ahead of time to have pen and
paper available for drawing, and a tape recorder. When the agreed-
upon experimental time arrives; the inbound experimenter simply asks
the subject to "describe what impressions come to mind with regard to
where the outbound experimenter is." Most subjects prefer to close
their eyes, but they should simply do what comes naturally. The room
lighting is preferably subdued to prevent after-image highlights,
shadows on eyelids, etc. It is best that the inbound experimenter not
push the subject to say a lot, but act as if they have all. the time in
the world; otherwise, a subject may tend to embroider descriptions
just to be saying something to please the experimenter. If the subject
tends towards being analytical ("I see Macy's on El Camino Real"),
the experimenter must gently lead the subject into description, not
analysis. ("You don't have to tell me where it is, just describe
what you see.") This is the most important and difficult task of
the inbound experimenter.
It is also useful for the inbound experimenter to "surprise" the subject
with new viewpoints. ("Co above the scene and look down--what do you
see? If you look to. the left, what do you see?) For some reason, the
subject's viewpoint appears to.shift rapidly with a. question like this,
and the data sneaks through before the subject's defenses activate to
block it out. The shifting of viewpoint also obviates the problem of
the subject spending the entire time giving meticulous detail on a
single blade of grass or piece of concrete, which, even if true, will
be of no help to a judge. Once a subject feels he sees something, he
tends to hand on to this perception rather than commit himself to a
new viewpoint.
The subject must be encouraged to sketch what he sees, even over his
objections that he is not an artist, can't sketch, etc. He may do so
throughout, or wait until the last five minutes if intermittent drawing
would distract this concentration. Since drawings tend to be more
accurate than verbalization, this is an extremely important factor
for good results.
(e) Post-Experiment Feedback: When the outbound experimenter returns,
the inbound and outbound experimenters and subject should proceed
285
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CI . 9 6-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/1 P96-00787R000100260003-2
clirect,ly to the target for feedback. This helps to develop the subject's
subjective sense of what in his mental imaging is correct versus
incorrect, and completes that experiment for him so that when he does
a following experiment, his mind is not still on wondering how he did
on the previous one. Only a very experienced subject can function
well time after time without feedback, so this must be done to insure
success.
(f) Judging Procedure: In a sense, all the action in the remote
viewing procedure is in the judging. Any single experiment in remote
viewing, even if perfect, can in principle be dismissed as possibly
coincidence. Further, any result less than perfect can be dismissed
as a generalized "grass is green, sky is blue" transcript which fits
every target. Only blind differential discrimination across a series
of targets cats put both of these alternatives to rest.
The judging procedures are as follows. First, an experimenter not
involved in judging must read the transcripts and delete from them
any reference to dates or previous targets, so that a judge could not
order the ranscripts chronologically, or determine that a given
transcript can't be the boathouse because the subject mentions in the
transcript that what he is looking at reminds him of the boathouse
which was the previous clay's target. With these deletions, the
transcripts with their associated drawings are labeled in random.order
and given to the judge in one hand, so to speak, while a list of the
target cards, also in a (different) numbered random order is given to
the judge in the other hand. His job, than, is to go to a target
location (physically), read through all the transcripts, and order
them best to worst match (.1 through 6, say, if there are six targets
and six transcripts). He then proceeds to a second target site and
reorders the same set of transcripts again, best through worst match,
and so forth. The jduge is to do this exercise in a replacement
sense; that is, even though he may have assigned a given transcript
as best match to a given target, he may find at another target that it
is the best match to that one also. Even though he knows logically
that it couldn't go to both, we find that judges in fact have no
hesitation in using a transcript twice in first place, simply because
they aren't sure as to which one it does in fact belong, and they
want to insure the best possibility of not. missing a potential match.
Based on this we feel it is more appropriate to use statistics based
on replacement. Some argue with this, and if one thinks it is more
correct then one can use statistics or matching without replacement.
286
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2
Approved For Release 2000/08/10 : CIA-RDP96-00787R000100260003-2