TRANSLATIONS ON USSR SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES (FOUO 8/78) EFFECTS OF NONIONIZING ELECTROMAGNETIC RADIATION

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4
Release Decision: 
RIFPUB
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
39
Document Creation Date: 
December 22, 2016
Document Release Date: 
May 10, 2012
Sequence Number: 
7
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
February 24, 1978
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4.pdf9.93 MB
Body: 
-?-`- Decla sified d Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 UIV L Y ' ? ,7P RS L/7629 2 4 February 19 7.8 TRANSLATIONS ON USSR SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES CFOUO 8/78) EFFECTS OF NONIONIZING ELECTROMAGNETIC RADIATION U. S. JOINT PU~LIC~-TI~NS RESEARCH SERVICE Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10 :CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 BIBLIOGRAPHIC DATA 1. Report No. 2. - 3. Recipient's Accession No. 'SHEET ~ .1PitS L./7629 4. u e an u ut a S. Report Date 'TRANSLATIONS ON USSR SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY 24 Februa 1978 BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES (FOLIO 8/78) 6. Effects of Nonionizing Electromagnetic Radiatio 7. Author(s) 8. Performing Organization Re pt. No. 9. Performing Organization Namt end Address 10. Pioject/Task/Work Unit No. Joint Publications Research Service ' 1000 North Glebe Road I1. Contract/Grant No. Arlington, Virginia 22201 1 L Sponsoring Organizntion Nnme and Address 13. Type of Report & Period Coveced As above __ la i 15. Supplementary Notes 16. Absti?acta Tl-'ie report contains information on aerospace medicine, agrotechnology, bionics arld bioacoustics, biochemistry, biophysics, environmental and ecological problems, food technology, microbiology, epidemiology and immunology, marine biology, military medicine, physiology, public health; toxicology, radiobiology, veterinary medicine, behavioral science, human engineering, psychology, psychiatry and related fields, and scientists anti scientific organizations in biomedical fields. 17. Key Words and Document Analysis. 170. Descriptors USSR Medicine Aerospace Medicine Microbiology ' Agrotechnology Physiology Biology Psychology/Psychiatry Botany Public Health Epidemiology/Immunology Radiobiology Human Engineering Toxicology Marine Biology Veterinary Medicine 17b. Identifiers/Upon-Ended Terms 17c. COSATI Fi~Id/croup 2, SG, 5J, 6, 8A 18. Availability Statement 19.. Security Cless (This I'or Official Use Only. Limited Report) 3 7 Number of Copies Available From JPRS 2 ' Pe Cr,ty aga to z2. P UNCLASSIFIED Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10 :CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 ~,??~; JP RS L/7629 24 February 1978 TRANSLATIONS ON USSR SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES (FOUO S/78) EFFECTS.OF.NONIONIZING.. ELECTROMAGNETIC RADIATION Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 I'OR OF IAL USL OIJLY This serial publication contains abstracts of articles and news items from USSR and Eastern Europe scientific and technical journals on the specific subjects reflected in the table of contents. Ph otoduplications of foreign-language sources may be obtained from the Photoduplication Service, Library of Congress, Washington, D. C. 20540. Requests should provide adequate identification both as to'the source and the individual article(s) desired. CONTENTS PAGE Reactions To Unperceived Stimuli in the Presence of Functional Disturbances of Sense Organs (G. V. Gershuni; SOVREMINNYYE TENDENTSII V NEYROFIZIOIAGII, 1 1977) ........................................................ The Role of Isolation, Environmental Deprivation and Enrichment in Formation of Behavior (A. D. Slonim; SOVREi~NNYYE TEDENTSII V NEYROFIZIOIAGII, 1977) 19 _ a _ [III= USSR - 22 S&T FOUO] Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 REACTIONS TO UNPERCEIVED STIMULI IN THE PRESENCE OF FUNCTIONAL DISTURBANCES OF SENSE ORGANS Leningrad SOVREMINNYYE TENDENTSII V NEYROFIZIOLOGII in Russian 1977 pp 68-81 [Article by G. V. Gershuni, Institute of Evolutionary Physiology and Biochemistry imeni I. M. Sechenov, USSR Academy of Sciences, Leningrad] [Text] In this article, I should like to discuss the functions of human sense organs described by such nonstandard criteria as occurrence of reac- tions to unperceived stimuli. The appearance of such reactions is very clearly demonstrable in the presence of some pathological states of the central nervous system as well as, as we have established, in healthy individuals who have developed conditioned reactions to stimuli that are below the threshold of sensations of which they are aware. Studies of this type of phenomenon were conducted by a team of workers at the Physiological Institute imeni I. P. Pavlov, USSR Academy of Sciences, for many years (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1.945; Gershuni, 1947, 1949, 1955). A brief description of this research was published by L. A. Orbeli in 1949. The results obtained had not been summarized to this time. In this article, we shall discuss phenomena observed in the presence of pathology of of the central nervous system occurring as a result of aerial concussion, closed skull trauma and mental trauma. The onset of marked vegetative and other reactions to stimuli delivered to sense organs, the functional state of which is characterized by the usual clinical methods as partial or total loss of the relevant type of sensibilit_y___.__ (auditory, visual, tactile, nociceptive, olfactory, gustatory) is not unex- pected. The descriptions of disorders referable to perception of exogenous stimuli, observed in the presence of 'closed skull trauma and mental trauma (i.e., in the presence of excessive stress for man) of wartime and peacetime, provided by clinicians, have long since indicated this (Veraguth, 1909; Myasishchev, 1929; Panov, 1933; Astvatsaturov, 1935). What our studies contributed that is new is determined by the introduction of a quantitative evaluation of phenomena, based on the choice and record- ing of a specific set of reactions, in the first place, and development of measurement procedures for threshold stimuli inducing these reactions, in the _ i i Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 second place, along with classical methods of determining the thresholds of perceived sensations in response to the same stimuli. By means of these procedures, we were able to establish two quantitative criteria characteriz- ing reactions to unperceived stimuli: 1) The difference between the threshold levels of stimuli inducing this type of reaction and thresholds of per- ceived sensations.* This difference, which expresses the range of inten- sity of stimuli that are not perceived, was referred to as the subsensory zone (Figures 1 and 2). We have used the term, subsensory (Gershuni, Alekseyenko, et al., 1945) to refer to the actual reactions that arise to stimuli below the threshold of perceived sensation. 2) Differences in characteristics of reactions arising in response to unperceived and perceived stimuli. Hearing Cutaneous V sensibility Figure 1. Thresholds of galvanic skin response (GSR) and thresholds of sensation in 'patient M. upon sonic and electric stimulation of the skin. Vertically: intensity of sonic stimulus (dB) in relation to normal hearing threshold (0); intensity of skin stimula- tion (V) for cutaneous sensibility. White circles--GSR to stimuli below sensation threshold; black circles-- sensation thresholds; black circles with crosses--GSR to perceived stimuli. Striped area--range of stimuli that are not perceived (subsensory zone); L--left;. R--right. ? These criteria enabled us to observe the dynamics of the pathological pro- cess in the presence of impaired perception of exogenous stimuli. Thus, at the stage of profound impairment of perception, the difference between threshold levels of stimuli inducing vegetative reactions and thresholds of sensation could reach enormous values, then undergo typical changes in the course of recovery of function. At the same time, we observed typical changes in the characteristics of the reactions. In order to determine the thresholds of stimuli., in our first work, which dealt with research on functional impairment of sense organs in the presence of wartime trauma (aerial concussion)(Gershuni, Alekseyenko et ~1., 1945), we used the reaction of dilatation of the pupil, as well as electroencephalo- graphic indices, the change in spontaneous rhythm and initial responses (Gershuni, Klaas et al., 1945). In our subsequent studies, we made extensive use of the galvanic skin response (Gershuni, 1947). *The term, "perceived (or overt) sensations," is used to refer to phenomena demonstrable in standard psychophysical measurements, as opposed to another group of phenomena referred to, even by I. M. Sechenov, as "sensations in discrete form" (1863). 2 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 o ~-` 17.01 2.02 7.02 i4 /~~ i ~~} 4 ? ~? i ?/ ~'~ 1 o ~ ?? 2 4 6. 8 f0 !2 14 16 1d 20 22 V Figure 2. Reduction of subsensory zone and recovery of cutaneous sensibility of patient~at the recovery stage (A) and magnitude of GSR as function of intensity of stimuli in and beyond the range of the subsensory zone (B) In A: X-axis, day of examination; y-axis, intensity of electric stimulus (V). White circles, GSR to sublim~.nal stimuli; black circles, perception thresholds. Striped area, subsensory zone. In B: X-axis, intensity of electric stimulus (V); y-axis, magnitude of GSR (relative units). White circles, stimuli that did not elicit responses; white circles with crosses, GSR to subsensory stimuli; black circles, GSR to perceived stimuli. GSR threshold, S V; perception threshold, 14 V Such vegetative and electroencephalographic reactions were found to be t~.he most sensitive indicators of activity of the central nervous system occurring in response to stimuli that were not perceived. Figure 1 illustrates a typical case of altered threshold values of stimuli evoking galvanic skin reactions and perception thresholds in a patient with severe, unilateral hypesthesia of the skin and impaired hearing (on the left) as a result of brain concussion. It shows that galvanic skin reac- tions, to both electrocutaneous and sonic stimuli on the left, arise at threshold levels that are much lower than the perception thresholds for cutaneous and auditory sensations. Upon stimulation of the right side of the body, with normal cutaneous sensi.bilir_y, the GSR occurs only in response to stimuli that reach the threshold of perceived sensations. Along with an increase i.n sensibility as demonstrated by the thresholds of perceived sensations, decrease in subsensory zone until~it disappears com- pletely are inherent in recovery of perceptive function in the presence of the above-mentioned disturbances. This phenomenon was demonstrable both in the pupillary reaction test in the course of restoration of hearing (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945) and in the GSR in the course of recovery of cutaneous sensibility (see Figure 2A). Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 The characteristics of vegetative reactions undergo substantial changes when the stimuli that induce them reach the sensation threshold, i.e., when they begin to be perceived. The most typical of these changes are a reduction in magnitude of reaaction and faster extinction under the in- fluence of successive stimuli. The former phenomenon emerges distinctly when measuring the magnitude of reactions as function of intensity of stimuli that are within and above the subsensory range. Figure 2B illustrates such data, obtained upon measurement of the galvanic skin response (GSR) as related to intenstiy of electrical stimulation of the. skin in a patient with markedly diminished dermal sensibility; the figure shows that when the stimulus reaches the sensation threshold there is a sharp decline of GSR. The entire curve for perceived stimuli is shifted in the direction of higher intensities. Figure 3 illustrates differences in magnitude of GSR and rate of extinction with delivery of stimuli to areas of skin with normal and markedly diminished sensibility. As can be seen in Figure 3, upon stimulation of the hypesthetic skin area, delivered at intervals of 1 to 1.5 min, GSR of greater amplitude occur throughout the period of stimulation; on the side with normal sensibility, GSR. occur only in response to the first stimulus reaching the sensation threshold; there is no reaction to subsequent stimuli. We should mention one more distinction in the dynamics of effects of un- perceived and perceived stimuli; it i~s referable to increased sensibility (sensitization) under the influence of successive stimuli. This phenomenon, which was studied in healthy individuals by A. I. Bronshteyn (1946), is very marked in patients with impaired dermal sensibility (Figure 4). Figure 4 shows that with successive delivery of stimuli to the skin surface, which induce GSR, the threshold of perceived sensation is reached with the ninth stimulus of very great intensity (3 times greater than the threshold for occurrence of GSR). With continued stimulation, there is a drop by almost SO% of sensation thresholds (sensitization phenomena); accordingly, there is a sharp reduction in the subsensory zone. The thresholds of occurrence of GSR to subsensory stimuli do not undergo apprecialbe changes. Thus, heighted sensibility is inherent expressly in conditions, under which conscious perception of exogenous stimuli occurs. In different cases of impaired conscious perception, different variants of the above-described phenomena may be observed. The described features of reactions arising to subsensory stimuli, namely, greater amplitude and stability in response to a series of stimuli and lack of dynamics typical of conscious perception (extinction of reactions and sensibilization), are the typical signs that are demonstrable in studies of diverse forms of sen.si bi].ity (auditory, cutaneous) and different vegetative reactions (papillary, galvanocutaneous). 4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 v 120 24 1 I! 111 NO 22 f00 ZO 4 90 18 ~ 80 16 4 70 f4 , ~ ~?4d4o 0 4 60 ?Z ~ p 50 f0 ~ ~ e 4 40 8 ~ ~ ~ e o 0 0 30 6 ~ 0 20 4 f0 2 0 ~ 0 S f0 15 20 ZS 30 3 S k0 min Figure 3. GSR to successive stimulation of anesthetized and normal skin areas X-axis, time in min; y-axis, intensity of single electric stimuli delivered to the skin (by condenser discharges)(V) and magnitude of GSR (relative units). White circles, unperceived stimuli that do not elicit GSR; white circles with crosses, unperceived stimuli; black circles with crosses, perceived stimuli associated with GSR; black circles, perceived stimuli not associated with GSR. Columns, magnitude of GSR as related to stimuli. Patient So-va (deep anesthesia of both legs, from the toes to the knee). Ef- ferent electrodes on the left hand, silent electrode on the left foot. Stimulating electrode placed as follows: I and III on the lower third of the right leg, anesthetized region; II on the lower third of the thigh on the same side, area of normal sensibility (according to experiments of A. M. Alekseyev and A. A. Arapova). A comprehensive clinicophysiological description of a group of patients who had sustained air concussion (106 people) was published previously (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945). The data illustrated in Figures 1-4 a're referable to a group of patients who had sustained closed brain trauma in peacetime (concussion)(studies of Alekseyev and Arapova; Arapova and Orlova; Arapova, Gershuni and.Orlova). G. V. Gershuni (1947), A. A. Arapova and G. M. Orlova (1948) published a brief report. All such patients presented impaired perception with severely marked subsensory reactions (22 cases). In a small group of patients (six people), in the history of which the effects of mechanical factors inducing trauma could not be established, similar perception disorders were observed, characterized by marked subsensory 5 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY --- _.. _ - Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 reactions; the factors inducing these disturbances should have been referred to psychogenic ones (excessive stress for the individual). As we know, in the clinical literature such disturbances were often designated by the term "histerical" or "hysterotraumatic" (Astvatsaturov, 1935). A similar case in wartime was described in 1945 (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945). In a work published in 1957 (Avakyan et al., 1957) there is a comprehensive physiological analysis of one peacetime case. Patients with impaired hearing (Kristostur'yan, 1952; Gershuni et al., 1954) and cutaneous sensitivity (Arapova and Orlova, 1948) of peripheral origin were also studied. In addition, studies were made of patients with impaired cutaneous sensibility as a result of lesions to different levels of the nervous system (syringo- myelia, lateral amyotrophic sclerosis, hemorrhages in the region of the pons varolii, comminuted trauma to the right parietal region), but no sensory reactions were demonstrable; the GSR thresholds were found to either coincide or to be above the sensibility threshold. In only one case of a disorder of vascular origin (cerebrovascular thrombosis) with the main .focus localized in the right parietal region, accentuated ;GSR were observed to stimulation of the skin, which were considerably lower than the threshold of perceived sensation. In this case, it was not possible to rule out lesions to other structures, including subcortical ones. With reference to the above-submitted data as a whole, it should be indicated that the same symptoms, characterized by clinical neurologists (Kryshova, 1945) as subcortical-stem symptoms, are found in patients who have suffered wartime air concussion and peacetime concussion, with which there is typical occurrence of subsensory reactions. The obtained data are inadequate for more precise description of the structures, with injury to which there is typical occurrence of subsensory reactions. The set of phenomena observed with the described disorders of perception is quite typical. It can be des- cribed as the syndrome of unconscious [unperceived] perception or, more briefly, the subsensory syndrome. 5 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY In research dealing with the effects o.f stimuli below the threshold of per- ceived sensation, in addition. to vegetative reactions, electroencephalographic indices were studied. The results of analysis of the electroencephalogram (F.EG) and changes therein under the influence of stim~ili in patients who had suffered air concussion revealed that spontaneous (background) activity (occipital and temporal leads were used) deviated appreciably from normal and was characterized by the following: 1) instability of the main 8-12-s alpha rhythm, ready disappearance thereof and change to either a faster o~r slower rhythm; 2) presence of slow waves, of the order of 1-3 per second aid spike discharges considerably exceeding the normal EEG variations; 3) impaired electrical activity of tYie cerebral cortex during sleep (Gershuni, A.l.ekseyenko et al., 1945; Gershuni, Kl.aas et al., 1945). The responses to exogenous stimuli (sonic, photic, mechanical, olfactory) are usually manifested by a change in amplitude of dominant EEG rhythm, appear- ance of relatively fast electric waves and new rhythms at the time the stimuli are used. Iii the patient group examined, the reactions to exogenous stimuli were demonstrable with stimuli below the perception threshold.: Thus, distinct reactions were demonstrated under the influence of sonic stimuli in indi- viduals who were totally deaf, as well as in response to stimuli delivered to the skin in the presence of severe decrease in tactile and nociceptive sensibility, under the influence of odoriferous substances in cases of total lack of olfaction. A comprehensive study of reactions to sonic stimulation revealed that the electrical responses are the most distinct at specific times after trauma was sustained. Paradoxical changes were observed in response to relatively mild stimuli and a significant decrease in such changes was found with increase in force of the stimuli. The intensity of the electrical responses diminished in the course of the overall recovery process (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945; Gershuni, IClaas et al., 1945). The subsensory zone could be established from the difference between thresholds of electrical response of the cortex and thresholds of auditory perception, as had been done with respect to thresholds of other reactions (pupillary, galvanocutaneous). In the patient group studied, the nature of responses to exogenous stimuli presented several distinctions, as compared to the normal findings. In addi- tion to the paradoxical response to mild stimuli, which we have already dis- cussed, the reaction is quite often manifested by intensification, rather than depression, of alpha rhythm, which is very similar to the electric response to exogenous stimuli in the intermediate state between sleeping ar-d waking. It is significant that, in the course of recovery of different types of sensibility and speech, the stable slow rhythms do not demonstrate a Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 ~:,.,~'.~ _-_ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 correlation to these changes, whereas the reactions to exogenous stimuli change in accordance with recovery of sensibility of a given sense organ. One of the parameters used for quantitative description of the electrical reaction of the cortex to sonic stimuli was the latency period of such reactions. The latency period was defined as the time that elapsed from the moment sound was delivered to the moment of appearance of EEG changes (accentuation or attenuation of alpha rhythm, accentuation of beta rhythm, appearance of initial response). Shorter latency periods were found in a number of subjects who had sustained air concussion. Increase in the latency period to close to normal levels occurred concurrently with recovery of hearing and speech. Shorter latency periods were not observed in patients with penetrating skull wounds. T.abl~ 1. Latency periods of EEG responses to sonic stimuli Nature of Disturbance Subject T.atenc~period Penetrating wound M, ~ 0.23 p, 0.45 0.32 I, 0.29 F, 0.29 0.26 G. 0.23 Hearing and speech disorders P. 0.07 following air concussion A. 0.08 0.09 Z, 0.14 T; 0.15 I, 0.20 0.12 ~, 0.13 Table 1 lists mean latency periods for normal individuals, patients with penetrating wounds to the temporoparietal region and patients who suffered air concussion with hearing and speech disorders at the early posttraumatic period. The data referable to latency periods at different stages of recovery of hearing and speech (Table 2) are demonstrative. As can be seen in Table 2, the latency period increased by 3 times during the period, within which hearing and speech are restored. The EEG data obtained on patients who suffered peacetime concussion presented the same feature in common, appearance of reactions to stimuli below the threshold of conscious perception. To illustrate this, we have submitted in Figure 5 the results of EEG tests on the same patient, whose GSR was studied comprehensively (see Figure 1). This patient has a distinct alpha rhythm. Sonic and cutaneous stimuli (Frey's bristles and hairs) delivered to the hypesthetic half of the body induced subsensory reactions of de- pression of alpha rhythm. A comparison of these reactions to those occur- ring~in response to stimulation of the other half of the body, with normal cutaneous and auditory sensibility, showed significant differences in 8 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 j Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 latency periods and duration of alpha rhythm depression. Thus, the latency period for subsensory sonic stimuli (on the left) is considerably shorter (0.18 s) than upon subliminal (perceptible) stimulation of the right half of the body (0.45 s), The reaction to subsensory stimuli (0.1 s) was found to last much less time than in response to stimuli above the sensibility threshold (2.4 s) with the same physical intensity of these stimuli. The differences in duration of alpha rhythm depression, as can be seen well on the oscillograms, are due to the after-effect, which is several times greater with stimuli that elicit conscious sensations. The phenomenon is equally marked under the influence of both sonic and cutaneous stimuli. Table 2. Changes in latency periods of EEG responses (according to Gershuni, Klaas et al., 1945) and at different stages of recovery of hearing and speech Latency Patient Date examined Condition of hearing and speech period(s) A-v, trauma 19 Sep 43 Hearing and speech absent 0.08 on 15 Aug 43 25 Sep 43 Appearance of hearing at all fre- quencies on the left, no speech 0.16 12 Nov 43 Appearance of hearing on the right, speaks in a distinct whisper 0.30 F'-f, trauma 14 Sep 43 No speech or hearing 0.07 on 21 Aug 43 15 Oct 43 Appearance of hearing at all fre- quencies in both ears; stutters 0.12 22 Oct 43 Speaks more distinctly 0.13 21 Nov 43 Hearing improved, speaks freely 0.22 The foregoing data characterized perception disturbances that could be demon- strated by specific procedures of psychophysical measurement. These pro- cedures involved the use of simple physical stimuli (for example; pure tones for hearing) and examination of reactions that did not require preli- minary experimental development. These conditions facilitated significantly measurement of thresholds and determination of range of stimuli that were nut consciously perceived, However, such a study was not sufficient to characterize perception disturbances; in the first place, it was necessary to obtain data on how more complex stimuli, including those used in real life (the sounds of speech, for hearing), are perceived; in the second place, we had to determine the extent to which it is possible to learn to perform specific activity in response to stimuli that are not perceived. Experi- mentally, this was a question of. developing conditioned reactions to sub- sensory stimuli. Data pertaining io perception of speech sounds and development of conditioned reactions to subsensory stimuli had already been obtained in the first in- vestigation (Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945). We submit data below, which pertain to perception of speech sounds described in this work. FOK OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Figure 5. Reactions of depression of alpha rhythm in the EEG of patient M. Left-sided decrease in tactile, nociceptive and auditory sensibility following concussion. Occipital lead. 'Top, stimulation mark; bottom, time (1 and 0.2 s), From top to bottom: tactile stimulation of. dorsal surfaces of left arm (subliminal stimulus); tactile stimulation of the same area on the right (supraliminal stimulus); sonic stimulation on the left (1000 Hz; stimulus is 20 dB below sensibility threshold); sonic stimulation on the right, same intensity as on the left (stimulus is 5 dB above threshold) Perception of speech sounds: Studies of thresholds of auditory sensibility using sinusoidal oscillations of a specific frequency (pure tones) revealed a consistent course of restoration of hearing at different frequencies. In some patients, there was some instability to the degree of decline; however:, these fluctuations of threshold did not exceed 10-12 dB. Under specific condi- tions, speech sound stimuli elicited changes of a very different nature, which could be roughly described as constituting 30-50 dB. The conditions, under which these phenomena could be observed, ensued from consideration of a very interesting procedure for testing hearing, described by L. B. Perelman (1943) in a study of similar patients. This procedure, which the author called "combined test," is based on concurrent delivery of verbal and 10 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 ~----- ~- ---- ----- --- T T Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 written signals, fixing the attention on the latter: questions are posed to the patient in writing, and he answers them in writing,or veibally (if he can speak). Concurrently with writing the phrase, the experimenter pronounces it at a certain volume; several questions, which follow one another, are written more and more indiscernably. But the subject continues to give the correct answers, and this could only be the result of a reaction to the sounds of speech. We were able to completely corroborate the pheno- menon described by L. B. Perelman, having observed it distinctly in a number of patients (10 out of the 35 studied). By varying the volume of speech sounds, it is possible, in some, cases, to determine how the reaction threshold changes on the basis only of fixing the eyes on the pencil moving along the paper. Of course, this is a rough determination, but this does not diminish its basic significance. Let us consider the two most vivid cases. 1) Patient S-ov lost his hearing and speech after an aerial bomb explosion. At the time of the study, he had regained'speech; he had no hearing on the left and could hear loud speech close to the concha on the right. He could respond to relatively low speech, the intensity of which was at least 30-40 dB lower than Che level to which he usually reacted, by fixing his eyes on a moving pencil (nothing is written on the paper). The test was repeated many times; each time, fixing the eyes on the pencil elicited a distinct response to low speech. 2) Patient Ya-uk lost his speech and hearing after an aerial bomb explosion. At the time of the study he stuttered. When addressed directly, he could not hear loud speech uttered at close range, near the concha, or the sounds of tuning forks. He could respond to moderately, loud speech at a distance of 1 meter when fixing his eyes on a moving pencil, but did not respond to low speech. Thus, fixing the eyes on a moving pencil elicits a reaction to speech sounds, the intensity of which is at least 30 dB lower than the intensity of sounds that are inaudible to the patient when delivered at the concha directly. After 4 days, the patient suddenly began to hear a loud shout near the concha.; audiometry revealed a decline of audibility threshold on the order of 100 dB at all frequencies. ,After 2 more days, he perceived moderately loud speech addressed to him. Audiograms showed typical decline at moderate and high frequencies (of the order of 60 dB). We sY-all submit several facts referable to the study of development of con- ditioned reactions to subsensory stimuli and stimuli beyond this range. We tried to develop conditioned reflexes in response to sonic signals, in the presence of external signs of deafness. In our experiments, we used primarily the method of verbal (written) reinforcement, according to A. G. Ivanov-Smolenskiy. Upon appearance on a screen of the written signal to "depress," the patient squeezed a rubber bulb. A metronome began to tick a few seconds (2-3) prior to appearance of the instruction. 'Depression of the bulb at the sound of the metronome, before the writing appeared, served as an indication of formation of a conditioned reaction. In several tests we used electric stimulation of the skin rather than verbal reinforce- ment. The patient had to perk his hand away from the electrode during passage of current. The clicking metronome preceded delivery of current. Experiments on 29 patients who were totally deaf or had a severe hearing impairment (over 80 dB) revealed that it may not be possible to develop a conditioned reflex in response to the metronome (intensity level of the 11 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY order of 40-50 dB). In this respect, the experimental results were quite similar. As a control, we conducted tests on the same group of patients, using contact with the skin as a stimulus rather than the metronome. Test- ing of 21 patients revealed that 16 of them could develop a conditioned reflex to tactile stimulation as a result of several combinations. Finally, on :mother group of patients (15 people) whose hearing had been restored to the e:ctent of perception of the sound of the metronome, the conditioned refle:c could be readily formed in 13 cases. The above facts indicated, on the one hand, that the impossibility of forming conditioned reflexes to inaudible metronome sounds could not be the result of sorue methodological flaws that generally prevented formation of conditioned associations under our experimental set-up; on the other hand, they showed that conditioned reflexes to sonic stimuli can be readily formed in res- ponse to sounds above the audibility threshold. We altered the experimental set-up to determine the significance of audibility threshold to development of conditioned reactions. Thus, we used a generator of electric oscillations and a telephone as sonic stimuli. We determined the audibility threshold and range of the subsensory zone according to the pupillary reaction at a frequency of 512 Hz. Table 3 illustrates the results of these studies; it contains the typical data obtained on one of the patients who had suffered air concussion. These data show rather clearly that formation of a conditioned motor reflex (movement of the fingers) can he distinctly demonstrated only with sounds that are slightly above the audibility threshold. No conditioned reactions were developed to sounds of considerable intensity, 43 and 50 dB above the threshold for the pupillary dilatation reaction, i.e., stimuli that definitely elicited a flow of afferent impulsation in the acoustic tract. Table 3. Development of conditioned reflex with the use of different intensities of sound, in patient Sh. Intensity of sound Intensity of sound Number (512 Hz) above the (512 Hz) in relation Formation of of Level of normal audibility to patient's audibility conditioned combi= conditioned threshold (dB) threshold (dB) reflex nations reflex 92 _7 No 30 -- 99 0 (threshold) No 37 -- 106 +7 Yes 4 mild 114 +15 Yes 4 strong '.~~;~~~ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 The patient had suffered air concussion; he was totally deaf on the left, 8U dB threshold of cochleopupillary reflex; diminished hearing on the right, 100 dB, 50 dB threshold of cochleopupillary reflex. subsensory zone, 50 dB. The telephone was put to the right ear. Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 . FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY nditioned reactions to subsensory stimuli ment of CO ated in uestion of develop exception was investig an et al., The q impaired auditory P Avaky in the presence of uent study in our laboratory ( deaf in both greater depth in the subseq female]), who was totally 18 times) in act (patient M? ~ times 1957). The subj trauma, was tested many during an experimental ears as a result of mental She regained her hearing 5 months: we used the galvanic skin reaction the course of 1? reliminary development. This To test the absolute threshol s, the extinction session. without any P demonstrated in numerous case , response, which occurred ically intensities at three fre- was stereotyP marked. The threshold did not differ by over 10-12 dB from the phenomenon was minima11y4000 Hz) quencies (200, 1000 and i.e.~ they were within the normala~cogrding be normal, clinical audiometry. Thus, levels considered to led in e of intensity of according to the criteria adop the subsensory zone involved the entire rang L-o the GSR, . sonic frequencies used. conditioned reactions thesmethod es of reactions to develoblinki.ng reaction using We used two tyP range 1) stimuli within the subsensory which permits measurement of absaevelop~ Avakyan (1955), during in individuals with normal hear n , developed by R. V. ential thresholds voluntary motor reactions based differ onses to sounds; b) G, Ivanov-Smolesnkiy meet of conditioned resp the method of A? s of. movements on verbal or written instructions by o ra~hic recording ~~ sin "lift your finger. (1933)? We made mechanical earancecof theg g when appearance or this onse to aPP ed rapidly performed in resp Normally, conditionedaneaudlonsignailop sign was preceded by the intensity of reactions to sounds, range) Development of conditioned ollSkagg(i?e?, within the subsensoryh it was ed from 90 as compared to normal, althoug which was Chang 1 altered, fixing of conditioned blinking re- was found to.be sharp y articularly slow: about 200 combinations possible. Formation and p was very Even flexes in response to low intensities for this, i.e., about 20 timesmm~~e f differentiations, an were required develop greater differences were observed in 'ndicative of significant intensification of success ve and this was i extinction inhibition. development of. conditioned reflex finger the blinking reaction, ust like the conditioned motor reac- Un].ike could not be lifting with written reinforcement, ~ ment of conditioned motor the finger away with electric reinforcement, lion of jerking id develop hotic stimuli. There was rap in response to p demonstrated at all? e in this patient, reactions of this tYP exception disorders that can The above facts are conditioned reflex method.P be detected by the our studies, occurred in this patient during of hearing, which earance of a marked condition rwhich henomenon, e of aPP ery i hi s p m Recov with the t sound. T coincided exactly onse to a erimenter during a test, the finger in resp the exP lion of articular interest, was detected by was of p 13 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 --:t._ ~:.;5..++T'sz-_,??= u,---~-. _ .. .. _~?.z~~aw?;:.~.~ tr:G~._._.:r3~~~.=-W ~r,~i?^-~ ~ -'~r~ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 on the basis of developing reduction of latency .periods of motor reactions. Thus, with delivery of the 18th stimulus, the motor reaction recorded on the electromyogram, occurred 0.1 s before appearance of the sign "lift your finger." Thus, we see that there is development of a conditioned motor response to sound. The appearance of distinct, conditioned finger movement, which could not be demonstrated in any of the preceding tests, compelled the experimenter to open the door and enter the chamber, in which the patient was situated. The patient was excited and her face was very flushed; when the experimenter appeared she said: "Last time, I heard something before the sign appeared; before, only the sign appeared. Before this, I felt something like a blow to the head." In answer to the question, "Do you hear ine?" the patient exclaimed "I do hear, I hear." She was very excited, tearful and exclaimed "I can hear," and hugged everyone there. The test was interrupted. Continuation of the study after a break revealed that, with each delivery of the sonic signal there was a conditioned motor reaction, in the form of lifting a finger, and after being instructed by the experimenter to respond verbally to the stimulus, the verbal report of "sound" appeared in response to the stimulus. A comparison of the signs of restoration of hearing in this patient to the reactions recorded on oscillograms shows that appearance of the first marked conditioned reaction of lifting the finger in response to a sonic stimulus coincides exactly with the same stimulus, with which the patient first reported auditory perception preceding appearance of the sign. Let us try to evaluate the obtained facts. According to the measurements of absolute thresholds according to the GSR, the sensibility of this patient's auditory system was close to normal. Hence, at sound intensities that were used to develop conditioned motor re~.ctions of finger lifting (50 dB), the flux of afferent impulsation in the auditory tract should have been quite significant. However, this flux could not be used to develop the motor act, which is referable to a complex system of movements referred to as voluntary, in just the same way as for construction of .conscious per- ception of the signal. However, development of conditioned reactions could occur on the basis of a motor reaction of very specialized significance (blinking reflex); in this case development of the reflex was slow and distorted. The foregoing indicates that the subsensory syndrome is characterized by rather profound and, at the same time, differentiated disturbances of central. nervous system function with complete preservation of afferent flux. Very generally, these disturbances can be described as impaired use of information. contained in the afferent flux in specific sensory pathways. lip Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 What facts can we offer to describe the changes that occur in the central nervous system i.n the presence of the subsensory syndrome: First of all, let us discuss the characteristics of generalized vegetative and electro- encephalographic reactions. The following were typical of vegetative reactions: a) greater amplitude of reactions; b) considerably greater stereotypy of responses in the case of multiple repetition of stimuli. The following are typical electroencephalographic reactions: 1) shorter latency period and shorter inhibition of. alpha rhythm (when this rhythm is pronounced); b) shorter latency period of other reactions (appearance of alpha rhythm in the early responses); c) distorted (accentuated) reactions to mild stimuli. It should be stressed that the above changes in characteristics of reactions are typical of stimuli in the subsensory range. Such findings are parti= cularly distinct in studies of the process of recovery of perception func- tion. This warrants consideration of the above-described changes as indications of changes in some common elements of the chain of events occur- ring in the central nervous system, needed for conscious perception of an external signal, as well as for development of conditioned motor reactions related to the system of voluntary movements, rather than concomitant pheno- mena. What is the significance of these changes? On this score, several hypotheses can be expounded, which are based on the attempts to interpret the aggregate of observed changes as an expression of impairment of some general biological. reactions, in particular, the orienting, waking up reactions, the set of emotional reactions determined by subcortical structures. Very plausible assumptions of this kind were voiced in the work of B. D. Asafov, per- forined in the laboratory of A. M. Zimkina (Asafov, 1965) and T. N. Reshchikova, performed in the laboratory of E. A. Kostandov (Reshchikova, 1969), as well as Zakharova (1973), in describing subsensory reactions ob- served in patients with lesions to deep structures of the brain. It is quite significant that, both in our studies and the above-mentioned, there is a link between the subsensory syndrome and disturbances referable to iieep structures of the brain. However, it is not deemed, possible at this time, on the basis of the available data, to describe .these structures more precisely and, accordingly, to expound hypotheses with sufficient sub- stantiation concerning the role of various regulatory systems in the observed functional disturbance. However, we could use another approach to this question, which has found ex- perimental expression in the form of studies of formation of conditioned reaction to subsensory stimuli in healthy individuals. It is only after reporting the results of this type of study, with which our team has been concerned for about 10 years, that it is possible to try to discuss the mairi question, which we can again formulate as follows: How are we to 15 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 interpret the internal correlation between such phenomena as awareness of perception of an exogenous signal (onset of conscious perception) and conditioned motor reactions related to the system of voluntary movements? 1. Avakyan, R. V. "Measurement of Threshold Intensities of Sounds and Differential Frequency Thresholds Using Conditioned Blink Reflexes of Man," in: "Fiziologicheskaya akustika" [Physiological Acoustics],. Leningrad, 1955, pp 52-59. 2. Avakyan, R. V.; Gershuni, G. V.; and Ratenberg, M. A: "Investigation of Function of Auditory Analyzers in Patients With Signs of Hysterical Deafness," ZHURN. VYSSH. NERVN. DEYAT. [Journal of Higher Nervous Activity], Vol 7, Vyp 3, 1957, pp 325-334. 3. Arapova, A. A., and Orlova, G. M. "Correlation Between Thresholds of the Galvanic Skin Reflex and Perception Thresholds in .the Presence of Disorders Referable to Cutaneous and Auditory Sensibility," in "Tez. Dokl. na XIII soveshch. po fiziol. probl." [Summaries of Papers Delivered at the 13th Conference on Physiological Problems], Leningrad, 1948, pp 8-10. 4. Asafov, B. D. "Functional Organization of the Orienting Reflex in the Presence of Lesion to the Deep Structures of the Human Brain," in: "Rol' glubokikh struktur golovnogo mozga cheloveka v mekhanizmakh patologicheskikh reaktsiy" [The Role of Deep Structures of the Human Brain in Mechanisms of Pathological Reactions], Leningrad, 1965, pp 18-20. 5. Astvatsaturov, M. I. "Nervous Diseases," Leningrad, 1935,.432 pp. 6. Bronshteyn, A. I. "Sensitization of Sense Organs,'.' Leningrad, 1946, 133 pp. ' 7. Gershuni, G. V. "Investigation of Subsensory Reactions in Sense Organ Function," FIZIOL. ZHURN. SSSR [Physiological Journal of the USSR], Vol 33, Vyp 3, 1947, pp 393-412. 8. Idem, "Reflex Reactions to Delivery of Exogenous Stimuli to Human Sense Organs as Related to Sensations," Ibid, Vol 35, Vyp 5, 1949, pp 511-560. 9. Idem, "Distinctions of Conditioned Galvanic Skin Reactions and Reactions pf Inhibition of Alpha Rhythm Occurring Under the Influence of Subliminal and Supraliminal SOI1iC Stimuli in Man," ZHURN. WSSH. NERVN. DEYAT., Vol 5, Vyp 5, 1955, pp 665-676. 16 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 ; "~~"?.~p~'f; ~~ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 10. Gersl~~uni, G. V.; Alekseyenko, N. Yu.; Arapova, A. A.; Klaas, Yu. A.; Maruseva, A. t1.; Obraztsova, G. A.; and Solovtsova, A. P, "Impairment of Activity of Sense Organs and Certain Other Nervous Functions in the Presence of 'Air Concussion'," in: "Voyei:no-meditsinskiy sbornik" [Military Medical Collection], Moscow--Leningrad, Vol 2, 1945, pp 98-192. 11. Gershuni, G. V.; Klaas, Yu. A.; Liva.nov, rS. N.; and Maruseva, A. M. "Electrical Activity of the Brain in the Presence of Hearing and Speech Disorders Occurring as a Result of 'Air Concussion'," in: "Tr. Fiziolog. in-ta AN SSSR" [Works of the Physiology Institute of the USSR Academy of Sciences], Moscow--Leningrad, Vol 1, 1945, pp 115-128. 12. Gershuni, G. V.; Kozhevnikov, V. A.; and Matyatova, Ye. S. "Investiga- tion of Some Manifestations of Activity of the Human Auditory Analyzer Using Conditioned Galvanic Skin Reflexes," VESTN. OTORINOLAR. [Vestnik of Otorhinolaryngology], Vol 16, Vyp 4, 1954, pp 14-20. 13. Zakharova, N, N. "Distinctions of Perception of Sensory Stimuli in the Presence of Emotional Stress and Posttraumatic Pyschopathoid Syndrome," ZHURN. NEVROPATOL. PSIKHIATR. [Journal of Neuropathology and Psychiatry], Vol 73, Vyp 3, 1973, pp 401-406. 14. Ivanov-Smolenskiy, A. G. "A Method of Studying Conditioned Reflexes of Man," Moscow, 1933, 104 pp. 15. Kristostur'yan, S. G. "Use of Conditioned Galvanic Skin Reflexes in the Presence of Pathology of the Auditory System," VESTN. OTORINOLAR, Vol 14, Vyp 2, 1952, pp 11-15. 16. Kryshova, N. A. quoted by Gershuni, Alekseyenko et al., 1945. 17. Myasishchev, V. N. "The So-Called Psychogalvanic Reflex and Its Signi- ficance in Personality Studies," in: "Novoye v refleksologii i fiziologii nervnoy sistemy" [News in Reflexology and Physiology of the Nervous System], Leningrad, Vol 3, 1929, pp 233-255. 18. Orbeli, L. A. "Problems of Higher Nervous Activity," Moscow--Leningrad, 1949, 499 pp. 19. Panov, A. G. "Experimental Analysis of Hearing Disorders in the Presence of Nervous System Pathology," in: "Psikhofiziologicheskiy eksperiment v klinike nervnykh i dushevnykh bolezney" [Psychophysiological Experi- mentation in Symptomatology of Neurological and Mental Disease], Leningrad, 1933, pp 6-23. ?_0. Perelman, L. B. "Reactive Postcontusion Surdomutism, Detection and Treatment Thereof," Moscow, 1943, 56 pp. 21. Reshchikova, T. N. "Investigation of Subsensory Reactions of Patients With Long-Term Sequelae of Closed Cerebrocranial Trauma," author abstract of candidatorial dissertation, Moscow, 1969. 17 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 ~~; Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY 22. Sechenov, I. M. "Reflexes of the Brain," in: "Izbr. proizv." [Selected Works], Moscow, Vol 1 (1863), 1952, pp 7-127. 23. Veraguth, 0. "Das Psychogalvanische Reflexphenomenon," Berlin, 1909. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", 1.977 10,657 CSO: 8144 18 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10. CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 THE ROLE OF ISOLATION, ENVIRONMENTAL DEPRIVATION AND ENRICHMENT IN FORMATION OF BEHAVIOR Leningrad SOVREMENNYYE TENDENTSII V NEYROFIZ~IOLOGII in Russian 1977 pp 304-316 [Article by A. D. Slonim, Institute of Physiology and Experimental Pathology of High Altitudes, Kirgiz Academy of Sciences, Frunze] [Text] The problem of separating complex forms of behavior into phenotypic and genotypic elements compelled researchers to develop a number of experiments with animals at different stages of postnatal and prenatal ontogenesis. Addi- tion to the environment of new factors, unusual for the organism, or elimina- tion of some of the customary ones were used extensively in recent years to determine the genesis of certain elements (patterns) of reactions of the organism and its systems. Environmental changes not only have effects based on the plus-minus inter- action principle, they also disrupt sharply the entire process of formation of behavior as an integral system. This prompted researchers to investigate r.he effects of a so-called enriched or deprived environment, as well as to evaluate the significance of this interaction, which occurs in accordance with the feedback principle. Reverse afferentation [feedback], postulated by P. K. Anokhin (1968) as an acceptor of action acquries exceptional import- ance in many animal species at a specific stage of development, and it is a most important mechanism (for example, in some passerines.) that adjusts vocal reactions. At the same time, elimination of some types of afferentation and reduction of overall information from the environment lead to prevalence of intero- ceptive influences on the central nervous system and could create a new level of physiological state, sleep. Isolation, with regard to certain types of food, sharply alters some types of appetite (Ugolev, Kassil', 1965) and even leads to energy balance disturbances (Chernigovskiy, 1962). At the present time, researchers have accumulated considerable material dealing with the effect of isolation and environmental deprivationaon the developing organism. These studies can be arbitrarily divided into the following three groups: 1) experiments involving isolation, with regard to some environmental factors; 2) experiments involving isolation of an animal of a given species lg Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 from its usual habitat (and development); 3) experiments involving exclusion of sensory systems (sensory deprivation). Historically, it happened that the method of isolation of the developing organism from the external environment turned out to be the simplest and, it would appear, most promising method. The history of this method goes back to the legendary experiment of Lycurgus, which has retained its signifi- cance to this day. The famous legislator of ancient Sparta, Lycurgus, took two puppies from the same litter, keeping one of them in a ditch in isolation from the environment and the other under ordinary conditions, among people and animals. When the puppies grew up, they were released to chase a hare. The dog that was raised in isolating hid from fright; the one raised in liberty dashed after the hare and choked it. This experiment served as the basis for education theory, which stipulated that it was imperative to overcome vital difficulties at an early stage of development (childhood) in order to develop positive personality traits in the adult. The experiment of Lycurgus was repeated in the laboratory of I. P. Pavlov, and it was found that the dogs raised in isolation were notable for cowardice and poor adjustment to changing living conditions (Vyrzhikovskiy, Mayorov, 1933). Isolation may refer to elimination of specific environmental factors, specific types of food, other animals of the same kind, specific forms of activity, physical environmental factors (for example, light), chemical environmental factors (change in air composition), habitat, etc. The isolation method is used at all stages of ontogenesis, but it is particularly important at the early stages of development of an organism, when different forma of behavior and activity are formed. In the experiment of Cuvier (1842), a young beaver was nursed by a woman. When it switched to plant feed, it would first stack part of the willow twigs in a corner of its cage after having torn the bark off,. When some earth was brought into the cage, the little beaver, which had never seen beaver dams, tamped the earth with its tail and stuck the willow twigs into it. Evidently, the experiment of Cuvier is. the first reliable experiment on isolated rearing of an animal in order to detect his natural instincts, and it defined the significance of isolation to formation of genetically programmed behavior. patterns. This was confirmed by Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1961). Spalding (1965) kept young swallows in small cages, that precluded any possibility of flying or exercising wing movements. In spite of this, when the swallows were rel.eased.at the stage of development when they should normally fly, they flew just as well as other birds of the same age. In the experiments of Grohman (1938), pigeons were kept in cardboard tubes that restricted wing movement. Control pigeons of the same .age were able to move their wings without restriction in their nest. The experimental birds were then released from the tubes and their capacity for flying was Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 then compared to that of control birds in their first flight. Grohman failed to demonstrate a difference between the two groups. This experiment showed that the main motor elements of flying in pigeons could develop without prior experience and formation of proprioceptive feed- back. However, there was no quantitative evaluation of flying performance. Analysis of fine adaptations developing in the course of exercise is very difficult to make. Petersen et al. (1957) studied development of flying in cabbage butterflies (Pieris). Butterflies that had just emerged from their cocoons were let out from a brightly lit area to determine the height to which they could fly. With age, they ascended higher and higher. Three groups of butterflies were allowed different periods of flying time in order to investigate the role of practicing flying in this refinement. One group was tested from the 1st to 5th day of life, 4 times a day, at 20-min intervals. The second group was kept inactive for the first 4 days, cooling them in darkness to 18?~, then tested just like the first group, on the 5th and 10th days of life. The third group was tested similarly, but was allowed 20 additional "practice" flights before each mandatory test in the first 5 and on the 10th day of life. Flying characteristics improved with age in all groups, regardless of practice. This improvement was the result of increased rigidity of the wings. The height reached by representatives of all groups of the same age was the same, which rules out the significance of practice. Similar results were obtained in a study of formation of motor activity in an aquatic environment, while swimming. Carmicheal (1927) restricted tadpole movement that usually takes place in the eggs by submerging them in urethane solution. The tadpoles developing in a motionless state swam quite normally after the anesthetic solution was washed off. Carmichael's data were not completely corroborated by D. A. Sakharov (1957) in the laboratory of Kh. S. Koshtoyants, who observed impaired swimming movements in clean water, after removal of the anesthetic, in frog larvae (Rana pipiens) raised under anesthesia. Considerable data have also been gathered on formation of specialized reactions in animals using the isolation method. Even L. Morgan (1899) had described a case, when baby squirrels, removed from the nest before they could see, were raised in a room and took nuts, put them on the rug and made "bsrrowing" movements. After a certain number of such movements, the squirrel would take another nut and repeat the whole procedure. In studies of the effects of isolation on development of behavior, much attention was devoted to sexual and parental behavior, which are related to preservation of the species. Thus, animals on different phylogenetic levels, which were raised in.isolation and encountered the relevant stimuli for t11e first time, manifested sexual activity. Male leaping spiders. go through a very complex courting ritual, which is usually also manifested in specimens raised in isolation (Drees, 1952). True, there are data to the effect tha t the sexual behavior of guinea pigs is impaired as a result of being raised . Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 in isolation (Young , 1957): significantly after isolating animals (males) at the age of 2 days and less so when isolated at the age of 10 days (Gerall, 1965). However, according to the~data of Harper (1968), when isolated immediately after birth and tested between the ages of 80 and 90 days, the sexual. behavior of male and female guinea pigs did not differ from that of control animals. There is reason to believe that these differences are related to the incomplete physical development of animals as a result of ruling out play activity when they .are kept alone in opaque cages. This fact confirms our conclusions (Slonim, 1961, 1971) that play is a programmed element of development and has distinct quantitative characteristics refera ble to specific stages of development (Ponugayeva, 1964, 1968). Precise quantitative characteristics are very difficult Co obtain with regard to the behavior of inexperienced mammalian mothers. For example, rab- bits build each successive burrow better than the preceding one (Ross et al., 1956). Such behavioral changes, elicited by activity, are not very noticeable. A more variegated set of findings is observed in experiments with lower monkeys. Singh (1969) demonstrated that wild specimens of Macaca rhesus and monkeys from Indian temples did not differ from animals raised in partial isolation, with respect to their ability to solve problems in a problem cage. Moreover, the monkeys raised under artificial conditions solved the problems faster than their wild relatives. At the same time, raising Macaca rhesus in isolation led to impairment of "social" and sexual .behavior of these animals. In particular, maternal rejection of its offspring and aggressive behavior toward offspring of the same age were observed (Harlow et al., 1971). This problem, that of development of aggressiveness in isolation, is being worked on very intensively in several laboratories. It was investigated the most systematically on laboratory mice (Valzelli, 1969x, 1969b, 1973), in which a distinctive "isolation syndrome" was described, consisting of several peripheral, behavioral and neurochemical changes in the organism. They include increased muscle tones, corneal and skin reflexes, piloerection and tremor. There is an increase in spontaneous motor activity, hyper- reactivity and development of hypertension. Isolated aggressive mice pre- sented an increase in weight of the adrenals and very accentuated orienting reaction. Progression of. these symptoms can be observed when the animals are kept in isolation for 1 to 4 weeks. This effect is observed only in males, since females are not aggressive. Aggressiveness does not develop if over S mice are kept in the cage, but it develops to a mild degree when there are 2 and 3 animals per cage. Castration prevents development of ag- gressive behavior; however, if it had developed prior to this operation, the latter did not eliminate it (Burge, Edwards, 1971). Adrenalectomy attenuates aggressiveness but does not prevent development of the phenomenon (Brain et al., 1971). The above-described phenomena should be classified as the consequences of "social" isolation, when environmental deprivation involves primarily iso- lation from specimens of the same species. Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Vocal reactions of animals are another exampJ_e of the role of experience in development of motor patterns of behavior. In birds, there is a distinct difference between a song, which is a more or less complex sonic expression and calls, which are shorter and simpler sounds. Most calls develop nor- mally in birds raised in isolation from adult specimens of the same species, regardless of whether or not they had heard the sounds of other species. Thus, normal vocal reactions were observed in roosters and chickens raised in an incubator (Schjelderup-Ebbe, 1923). Experiments with small Passeri- formes, warblers and black thrushes, raised in isolation, revealed that their calls develop normally (Sauer, 1954; Messmer, Messmer,r-1956; Thielcke- Poltz, Thielcke, 1960). The calls were exactly the same as in wild specimens of the same species in all 25 thrushes raised in isolation. The young of many other species of nesting Passeriformes birds were removed from their nest at different times between hatching and acquiring their plumage, and they were hand-fed in varying degrees of isolation from their own and other species. The authors believe that experience in perception of sounds during the period prior to isolation could not have a direct effect on subsequent development of calls, aJ.though this possibility cannot be entirely ruled out. The calls of small Passeriformes, skylarks and pied flycatchers (Canyon, 1957; Curio, 1959), developed Normally in almost all cases with isolation. A wild hybrid from a cross between the pied flycatcher and collared flycatcher developed an intermediate call, differing appreciably from the calls of both species (Haartman, Lohrl, 1950; Lohrl, 1950). Consequently, the calls of passerine Passeriformes raised in isolation did not differ from calls of birds in their natural habitat (Miller, 1921; Nice, 1943; Marler, 1956). Ttiis confirms the conclusions in old studies involving hand-feeding of nesting birds (Heinroth, 1924) to the effect that, in general, development of inter- specific differences in bird calls is relative and unrelated to the acoustic influences of other birds (Stadler, 1929). Normal development of singing was demonstrated in handed baby swallows and sand martins, wrens, pikas, star- lings, orioles, reed and corn buntings, as well as bullfinches (in the surveys o.f Heinroth, 1924; Stadler, 1929; Thorpe, 1961; Marler, 19.63). A. N. Promptov (7.944, 1956), A. N. Promptov and Ye. V. Lukina (1945) demonstrated the great importance of imitation in development of singing in birds, but the authors also devoted much attention to coordinations in the vocal effector apparatus and differences that arise here. According to A. N. Promptov, vocal reactions as a form of congenital behavior are largely determined by morphological distinctions and function of vocal muscles .of the lower larynx. Attention is devoted to both imitation proper, i.e., the capacity for imita- tion which is congenital, and conditioned reflex changes in focal reactions acid motor behavior of birds. At the present time, it may be considered established that imitation and learning play different roles in formation of vocal reactions (calls and singing) in birds. In this respect, all birds can be divided into three groups (Galambos, Worden, 1972): first group, birds (for example, domestic ones) that retain species-specific vocalization, even if~they were deprived of hearing immediately after hatching, i.e., before exposure to calls. This implies the existence of genetically programmed vocal reproduction. In these species, there is no process of comparison of the genetic program Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 to acoustic information. The second group refers to birds (passerine Passeriformes) that can develop normal singing, even if raised in isolation; if thl~y are deaf at birth, normal. singing does not develop, and this suggests that acoustical feedback (their own voice and singing) plays an important role, apparently in relation to the genetic sound code; the third group of birds refers to those for which not only auditory feedback but an external "mode:l." of species-specific singing must exist for development of voice and singing. For example, in chaffinches raised in isolation singing is impaired, and there are many structures, which are related to imitation in the course of deVelopment~ that are wanting. There are very few data concerning voice development in mammals, with the exception of man. Most important is the study of Boutan (1913) of the be- havior ofa hand-fed gibbon, in whom all 30 studied cries developed normally, although it was not known to cahat extent this animal was familiar with the sounds of its wild relatives. This question often arises in primate studies. For example, up to 32 different vocal signals can develop in hand-fed chim- panzees (Yerkes, Learned, 1925), but there are no data pertaining to com- parison thereof to the calls of wild ani.ma.ls. In spite of persistent attempts to tench lower monkeys new sounds, including human speech, they either failed, or else the results were very insignificant (Furness, 1916; Yerkes, Yerkes, 1929): Riggs et al. (1972) tried to investigate the effect of lack of hearing on vocalization of adult Saimiri sciureus monkeys. They demonstrated a lack of visible differences in vocalization of normal and hearing-deprived animals. After their birth, the mothers raised their young in an environment deprived of species-specific sounds. One of the offspring was deprived of hearing surgically 5 days after birth. Two otF-ers were raised under normal conditions, i.e., they were exposed to species-specific vocalization. Additional data were obtained on 6 other offspring: 4 were raised normally, 2 isolated from their mothers. The sonic spectrograms were studied for 6 months on those raised in isolation and for up to 17 months on the normal ones. Samples of this spectrographic material were analyzed (form of calls and quantitative criteria, i.e., duration, frequency characteristics of squeals and cries). No differences could be demonstrated between experimental and control animals. The sounds emitted by babies and adult animals also failed to differ with regard to these parameters (Winter et al., 1973). Perhaps, there is normal development of vocalization in lower and even higher primates, gibbons and orangutans in isolation. Seitz (1955) raised a group of raccoon dogs (Nyctereutes procyonoides) in isolation and observed that six calls and variants thereof developed normally; the same was observed in two domestic cats raised in isolation (Weiss, 1952) and in Duplicidentata isolated after the first month of life (Severaid, 1958). In our laboratory, a large series of studies was conducted at'different times on ma~ranals isolated from the environment, related to formation of specialized (species-specific) alimentary and defense reactions. Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 In the studies of E. R. Uzhdavi.ni (1958a, 1958b), salivary duct fistulae were created in puppies immediately after birth; he found that, at the age of 20-21 days, the puppies raised on a milk diet presented a positive reaction to meat, consisting of movement toward meat and salivation. Ttris fact indicates that the inborn reflex to the odor of meat appears in dogs, as in predators, at a specific period of life, at the time when ani- mals switch from milk to a mixed diet. If this reflex is not reinforced with meat, it is extremely unstable and no longer demonstrable at the age of 8-9 months. It is totally replaced by natural conditioned reflexes, which developed in the course of being on a milk diet. Similar findings ware made on puppies isolated from their mothers and raised on bottles. The reaction of newborn Ungulata to shading .[dark] above the head, which is particularly vivid on the 1st-3d day of life, disappears when the animal is fed from a bottle, i.e., when this darkness is not reinforced by food (Slonim, 1961). In puppy dogs, a positive reaction to stimulation of the snout with fur is particularly marked on the 2d day, rather than immediately after birth, even if the animals are fed from special droppers. Then, on the 3d-5th day, this reaction disappears, failing to be reinforced by contact with the nursing female. The studies of A. M. Ugolev (1953) performed with kittens revealed that there is no salivation in response to the sight and odor of meat while they .ar.e hunting for live prey, just as .is the case in response to the sight and odor of living prey, mice and birds. It was found that this is specific for a predator with the type of nutrition involving stalking of the prey and prolonged tracking thereof; in the kittens that 11ad never hunted, there were the usual, natural conditioned alimentary salivary reflexes, even though they were on a mixed diet. The transformation of kittens into "hunters of prey," i.e., the transition to independent searching for food (feeding on live prey) is associated with an abrupt change in the entire sys- tem of unconditioned and conditioned alimentary reflexes. The natural salivary reflexes disappear entirely during the period of ,stalking the prey or prior to eating, as is the case in adult cats. This unique "trans- formation" of the kitten into a hunting animal, a predator, occurs at a certain age which, however, fluctuates over a rather wide range. It was also established that if a kitten, which had not yet hunted for food, is given cut pieces of dead mouse, then a live mouse but with some cuts in it, this transformation can be accelerated due to development of con- ditioned reflexes to the mouse as a source of food. The abruptness of this transformation in predators is of enormous interest, since it permits determination of the direct effect of the environment on rate of formation of unconditioned reflexes, which are thus far from absolutely independent of. environmental factors that have immediate biological significance. However, the isolated maintenance of kittens in many homes does not prevent them from changing into predators and catching mice and rats; consequently, ~j FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 26 'r'Oh OFFICIAL U~E ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 neither imitation nor development of special reflexes determine formation of the predator reaction to live prey. According to the data of A. M. Ugoley, in cats that do not hunt for mice, there is no inhibition of saliva- tion at the sight and odor of food and live prey, which they had never caught. Formation of alimentary reflexes in predators was confirmed in a newborn lion cub raised on a milk diet to the age of 50 days. It was found that, starting at the age of 30-31 days, it developed a stable, positive motor reaction, without extinction, to the sight and odor of meat. It appeared concurrently with an eye movement reaction to a moving object. Consequently, inborn reactions to meat develop independently of feeding conditions (lJzhdavini, Shepeleva, 1966). In the studies of A. I. Shcheglova (1959, 1961), young rodents were nursed by their mothers or females of another species: an albino rat nursed a great gerbil, a great gerbil nursed an albino rat, a gray rat nursed a gerbil, etc. In a special series of experiments, a group of gerbils and rats was raised in a warm bag and fed milk from a piece of cotton and a dropper. 1'he animals were tested, starting on their 2d day of life, for 10 min in a special chamber, which contained a digging object, i.e., dry roasted sand, and gnawing objects, wooden sticks. It was found that gnawing and digging began and developed in all gerbils at the same time, regardless of whether they had been nursed by a gerbil, white rat or with a dropper. Digging activity was much less marked in the gray rat than the great gerbil. It appeared on the 18th-19th day of life; gnawing was observed on the 22d day t and burrowing on the 14th day. A1]. these reactions developed at the above- mentipned times, regardless of conditions under which the animals were reared. In th~~ experiments of A. G. 1'onugayeva (1960) who studied the milk-hoarding reaction in young golden hamsters, the offspring were raised with their mother but fed a .liquid mixture that these animals could not store. In another series of experiments, young golden hamsters were taken from the mother on the 24th day and also fed a liquid diet. These experiments re- vealed that formation of the feed-storing reaction in the golden hamster is unrelated to rearing conditions. However, by far not all alimentary reactions develop in isolation, as was shown in the above examples. K. Rakhimov (1958, 1959) found that there was no attempt to graze by hungry lambs and kids raised up to the age of 5 months on a milk diet in strict isolation from others of their species, as well as from pasturage and rought plant feed, when out in pasture. They developed the ability to graze only after being with the herd for a few days. The act of grazing, which is in essence a conditioned reflex, is formed in the nature of an imitation reflex. It is important to mention this, since V. I. Klimova (1956, 1958) established that neonate rabbits, kept on a milk diet, presented an inborn reaction to green feed: The numerous rxirer.i.mente of: K. Rakhimov failed to demonstrate this unconditioned reflex to feed of plane origin at any of the phases of development of neo- nate Ungulata raised on a milk diet. These differences in formation of Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 unconditioned alimentary reflexes in rabbits and ungulates could be related to the difference in ecological conditions under which the young develop, a burrow for the rabbits and herd fior the ungulates, with the enormous oppor- tunities for forming behavior based on imitation. The above material warrants the conclusion and the isolation method is, so to speak, the antithesis of the method of development of artificial conditioned reflexes and experimental elimination of some forms of conditioned reflex activity. This route of research, largely substantiated by I. P. Pavlov, presents difficulties, with respect to interpretation of the results, since it requires mandatory comparison to development of inborn behavior: All of the studies lead to the same conclusion, that the alimentary act and associated salivation reaction are stimulated by the sight and odor of food, and that these reflexes should be referred to the natural conditioned reflexes. If an animal is denied the food inherent in adult specimens of its species from the day it is born, these reflexes are not manifested. Thus, we also find an explanation for the numerous findings of disappearance of predator's reflexes when raised with other species (for example, wolf cubs and lambs, etc.). However, when interpreting these facts, it is imperative to bear in mind that the transition from milk to a mixed diet, then to independent feeding occurs within specific ontogenetic time frames, and that it: is possible to demonstrate the appearance of specialized reflexes to specific foods only in these periods of development of an animal (Kossobutskiy, 1951; Ugolev, 1953; Slonim, 1955; Klimova, 1956, 1958; Rakhimov, 1958, 1959; Uzdavini, 1958a, b, and many others). With reference to the .results of experiments involving isolation from some types of food, we cannot fail to call attention to the fact that appearance and disappearance of a positive reflex to the odor of food is inherent in the ontogenetic process. In these experiments, it is difficult to imagine (in spite of all the precautions taken by the authors: isolated feeding, special uniforms for the service personnel, etc.) that there was total elimination of odors of meat, which had never been reinforced and therefore became an inhibitory stimulus. A special experiment conducted in our labo- ratory with puppies kept in the vivarium building but in separate cages where olfactory stimuli referable to the odor of meat were not ruled out, and where they were kept on a diet of milk alone, confirmed this thesis. Such puppies did not differ in any way from those kept on a milk diet in an isolated dog house. This is indicative of tYie special role of formation of inhibitory conditioned reflexes when using the isolation method. Isolation from different environ- mental factors not only precludes the possibility of development of natural conditioned reflexes in response to signal-value components of these factors, but could lead to formation of inhibitory reflexes, to development of signs of habituation. This applies, first of all, to olfactory and sonic stimuli, the sensibility thresholds for which ar.e exceptionally high in many animals. 27 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Disruption of the living stereotype is the second circumstance that is rather important to evaluation of the results of such research. Expressly exposure to a stimulus that was never uged before leads to inhibition of reflex activity. For this reason, for example, stow eating of meat by "milk" puppies and lack of physiological reactions adequate to this can be interpreted as the influence of anew alimentary stimulus, unusual for the animal. F:ina].ly, use of the isolation method compels us to pay attention to the fact that isolation from environmental factors and impoverishment of the latter affect not only formation of~certain natural conditioned reflexes, but the entire dynamics of cortical processes. This thesis gained experi- mental confirmation i.n the studies of staff members of the University of California (Rosenzweig et al., 1972). Studies were pursued of a number of changes in the brain (occipital cortex) of rats kept in an "enriched" and "deprived" environment for 25 to 100 days. The authors proceeded from the fact that, in the laboratory, 3 rats are usually kept per cage. In the "deprived environment," only one rat was kept in each cage. In the "enri.ched environment," 12 rats were kept together in a large cage that contained toys which were changed daily. Food and drink were available in abundance in all of the cages. Biochemical, histochemical and morpholo- gical. changes were demonstrated in the occipital cortex of rats from the "deprived" and "enriched" environment. Almost all of the changes were reli- able. We were impressed by the sharp increase in thickness of the occipital cortex of "enriched" rats, as well as ,the increase~in number of anatomical synaptic connections per unit area (by 50%) and decrease in their size. A team of Czech physiologists conducted a large series of studies of this type; they "impoverished" not only the external environment but the diet, and i.n particular its protein componen~ Animals raised on a low-protein diet revealed slower myelinization of nerve fibers, development of pyramidal neurons in the sensomotor cortex, an increase in latency period of optical and acoustical evoked potentials (Myslivecek, 1970; Safanda et al., 1971; Myslivecek et al., 1974). Thus, a wide range of phenomena related to nutrition, on the one hand, and delivery of stimuli from the environment, on the other, was demonstrated. It is important to note that both endogenous and exogenous influences determine not only general development of the organ- ism, but primarily formation of the central nervous system, i.e., the brain. If we scrutinize the method of isolation in detail, in connection with the study of ontogenetic development of functions, we cannot overlook the influence of afferentati.on on the genetically predetermined process of development. In this respect, it does not matter whether these influences are considered from the standpoint of action acceptor or construction of some other system with feedback. Experimental research involving elimination of different analyzers during the period of postnatal ontogenesis.enables us to investigate this question and detect disturbances in the natural course of development, which occur with any form of isolation or restriction of condi- tions inherent in a given species. In this regard, some interesting data FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 are given in the survey of Fox (1970). Fox et al. (1968), having deprived a dog of sight, found normal development and myelinization of the visual cortex, i.e., maturation thereof regardless of control and stimulation. In the in vitro studies of Crain et al. (1970), they demonstrated that blocking agents of the curare type did not affect development of nerve tissue; there was normal development of bioelectr.ic activity at the usual rate. It may be assumed that self-stimulation via proprioceptive feedback after a single movement or during continuous activity of embryos may be significant for' normal development of effectors, i.e., the skeletomuscular system, rather than for. organization of motor acts. Evidently, this feedback develops after spontaneous mobility is demonstrated, when reflex or evoked activity can be elicited by mild (cutaneous) or more profound (proprioceptive) external stimulation of the embryo. After this phase of effector-affector integration on the segmental level of the spinal cord, there is subsequent integration on higher levels of the developing nervous system until complex coordinated action appears. Hamburger (].963) discussed the possible role of these phenomena in embryogenesis of activity of chickens. Experiments conducted in his .laboratory (Hamburger et al., 1965) revealed that spontaneous activity develops before evoked activity, even in isolated segments of the locomotor system, and that extensive generalization, involving the entire nervous sys- r_em and observed in normally developing mammals after birth, appears on about the 17th day of incubation (Volokhov, 1951; Voyno-Yasenetskiy, 1974; Fox, 1966). Held and Hein (1963) showed convincingly that deprivation of kittens of all nonvisual sensibility impairs severely their subsequent exploratory behavior that is controlled by sight. Rats blinded at an early age were notable for, poor spatial auditory discrimination, as compared to those that had the experience of vision (Spigelman, Bryden, 1967); but rats deprived of sight at an even earlier age coped well with simple problems of auditory discrimination. On the basis of these comparisons, Fox concludes that it is difficult to separate the role of maturation processes from influences of prior experience in development of a specific branch of the nervous system or behavior pattern. In such cases, experiments involving isolation or. restriction often induce effects in the form of excessive excitation after isolation and lead to paradoxical phenomena. Thus, Lindsley et al. (1964) discovered that monkeys raised in darkness reacted to light just like monkeys raised under normal conditions react to the light being switched off. Increased excitation in dogs after isolation disrupts their ability to solve problems (Fox, Stelzner, 1.966) and to respond to nociceptive stimuli (Melzack, Scott, 1957). Other "nonspecific" influences that could distort efforts to examine a given sensory system or behavior pattern include masking or depression of certain responses due to lack of condi- tioning or reinforcement; these responses may simply disappear or become "overmature," or else they may be related to unusual stimuli. Kovach and Kling (1967), for example, found that kittens fed from a pipette subse- quently refused to suckle their mother. Isolation may disrupt not only maturation of a specific branch of the nervous system (or behavior pattern), but structural and functional integration of one system (or element of interaction) with another. For example, Held and Bauer (1967); Hein and 29 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 and Held (1967) showed that if a collar is put on the neck of a newborn , monkey or kitten, which prevented them from seeing their forelegs, it retarded development of association of the eye and front limbs and, after the color was removed, there was no sight-controlled movement of these limb's. Thus, some adjustments must be made to the isolation method. However, for the time being, it is the only means of eliminating learning and memory phenomena and thereby of separating genetically heterogeneous elements of complex behavior of an organism into different stages of formation of functional correlations. 1. Anokhin, P. K. "Biology and Neurophysiology of Conditioned Reflexes," Moscow, 1968, 546 pp. 2. Voyno-Yasenetskiy, A. V. "Pr.imary Rhythms of Excitation in Ontogenesis," Leningrad, 1974, 147 pp. 3. Vol.okhov, A. A. "Ontogenetic }?atterns of Nervous Activity," Moscow-- Leningrad, 1951, 301 pp. 4. Vyrzhikovskiy, S. N., and Mayorov, F. P. "Data on the Effects of. Rearing on Type of Higher Nervous Activity in Dogs," TR. FIZIOL. LABOR. AKAD. I. P. PAVLOVA [Works of the Physiology Laboratory of Academician I. P. P avlov], Vol 5, 1933, p 171. 5. Klimova, V. I. "Alimentary Reflexes and Natural Stimuli in Ontogenesis of Rabbits and Dogs," in "Soveshch. po vopr. evol. fiziol. Tez. i ref. dok7.." [Conference on Problems of Evolutionary Physiology. Summaries and Abstracts of Papers], Leningrad, 1956, p 82. 6. Tdem, "Alimentary Reflexes to Natural Stimuli in Ontogenesis of Rabbits and Dogs," in "Problemy sravnitel'noy fiziologii nervnoy,deyatel'nosti" [Problems of Comparative Physiology of Nervous Activity]; Leningrad, 1958, p 54. 7. Kossobutskiy, V. I. "Analysis of Development of Food-Hunting and Defense Instincts in Ontogenesis of Some Predators," author abstract of dissertation, Moscow, 1951. 8. P9organ, L. "Habbit and Instinct," St. Petersburg, 1899, 315 pp. 9. Ponugayeva, A. G. "Physiological Studies of Animal Instincts," Moscow--Leningrad, 1960, 180 pp. 10. Idem, "Prolonged Effect of Ambient Temperature on Formation and Per- formance of Voluntary and Muscular. Activity in Ontogenesis," in: "Teploobrazovaniye v organizme" [Thermogenesis.in the Organism], Kiev, 1964, pp 168-169. 30 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 k~~ r~~~~ II.iL~---_.._w.e_?4~ e~t.v r--~-~_vs-~_t~t.._.._ _.. ..._~i~li~.. _-' ~~.:~~~7:tY~+'~'i';~.+~u;.S,~ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 1.1. Ponugayeva, A. G. "Research on Play Activity of Rats and Golden Hamsters," in: "Sravnitel'naya i vozrastnaya fiziologiya" [Comparative and Developmental Physiology], Leningrad, 1968, pp 104-117. 12. Promptov, A. N. "Vocal Imitation in Passeriformes Birds as One of the Specific Properties of Their Higher Nervous Activity," DAN SSSR [Reports of the USSR Academy of Sciences], Vol 45, No 6, 1944, pp 278- 281. 13. Idem, "Essays on Biological Adaptation of Passeriformes Bird Behavior," Moscow--Leningrad, 1956, 310 pp. 14. Promptov, A. N., and Lukina, Ye. V. "Conditioned Reflex Differentiation of Instincts [Urges] in Passeriformis and Biological Significance Thereof," DAN SSSR, Vol 46, No 8, 1945,' pp 422-424. 15. Rakhimov, K. "Data Referable to the Study of Development of Natural Alimentary Reflexes in Ontogenesis of Ruminants," in: "Voprosy fiziologii. i patologii nervnoy sistemy. Tr. II konf. molodykh uchenykh In-ta fiziol. im. I. P. Pavlova AN SSSR" [Problems of Physiology and Pathology of the Nervous System. Proceedings of 2d Conference of Young Scientists of the Institute of Physiology imeni I. P. Pavlov, USSR Academy of Sciences], Moscow--Leningrad, 1958, pp x.03-104. 16. Idem, "Formation of Alimentary Reflexes in Postnatal Ontogenesis of Ruminants," dissertation, Leningrad, 207 pp (Institute of Physiology imeni T. P. Pavlov, USSR Academy of Sciences). 17. Sakharov, D. A. "Investigation of Factors That Regulate Swimming Activity of Developing Amphibians," summaries of papers, Moscow, 1957, p 164. 18. Slonim, A. D. "Fundamentals of General Ecological Physiology of Mammals," Moscow--Leningrad, 1961, 431 pp. 19. Idem, "Ecological Physiology of Animals," Moscow, 1971, 448 pp. 20. Slonim, A. D.; Uzhdavini, E. R.; and Fayziyev, S. M. "Developmental Distinctions of Unconditioned and Natural Conditioned Reflexes in Early Ontogenesis," in: "Mater. II nauchn. konf. po vopr, vozrastnoy morfologii i fiziol." [Proceedings of 2d Scientific Conference on Problems of Developmental Morphology and Physiology], 1955, p 113. 21. Ugolev, A. M. "Conditioned Salivation Reflexes in Cats," in: "Opyt izucheniya regulyatsii fiziologicheskikh funktsiy" [Knowhow in Research on Regulation of Physiological Functions], Moscow--Leningrad, 1953, pp 130-140. 31 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 2L. Ugolev, A. M., and Kassil', V. G. "Alimentary Behavior and Regulation of HOmeOStasis," in: "Slozhnyye formy povedeniya" [Complex Forms of Behavior], Moscow--Leningrad, 1965, pp 41-58. 23. Uzhdavini, E. R. "Congenital Alimentary Reflexes of Puppies," in: "Opyt izucheniya regulyatsii fiziologicheskikh funktsiy," Moscow-- I,eningrad, 1958a, pp 101-111. 24. Idem, "Formation of Natural Alimentary Reflexes in Ontogenesis of Dogs," Ibid, Vol 4, 1958b, pp 112-123. 25. Uzhdavini, E. R., and Shepeleva, V. K. "Essays on Development of Inborn Behavior," Leningrad, 1966, 119 pp. 26. Chernigovskiy, V. N. "Significance of'Interoceptive Signaling in Alimentary Behavior of Animals," Moscow--Leningrad, 1962, 78 pp. 27. Shcheglova, A. I. "Development of Digging and Gnawing Activity in Ontogenesis of Same Rodents," in: "Soveshchaniye po ekologicheskoy fiziologii. Tez. dokl." [Conference on Ecological Physiology. Sum- maries of Papers], Leningrad, Vyp 1, 1959, pp 83-84. 28. Idem, "Investigation of Physiological Distinctions of Life Forms in the Desert on the Example of the Great Gerbil," in: "II konf. fiziol. biolchim. i farmakol. Sredney Azii. Tez. dokl." [Second Conference of Physiologists, Biochemists and Pharmacologists of Central Asia. Summaries of Papers], Frunze, 1961, pp 395-396. 29. Brain, P. F.; Nowell, N. F.; and Wouters, S. "Some Relationships Between Adrenal Functions and the Effectiveness of a Period of Isola- tion in Inducing Intermale Aggression in Albino Mice," PHYSIOL. BEHAV., Vol 61, 1971, pp 27-29. 30. Burge, K. G., and Edwards, D. A. "The Adrenal Gland in the Pre- and Post-Castrational Aggressive Behaviour of Male Mice," 1'b id, Vol 7, 1971, PP 885-892. 31. Carmichael, L. "A Further Study of the Development of Behavior in Vertebrates Experimentally Removed From the Influence of External Stimulation," PHYSIOL. REV., Vol 34, 1927, pp 34-47. 32. Crain, S. W.; Bornstein, M. B.; and Peterson, E. P. "Development of Functional Organizar_i_on in Cultured Fetal CNS Tissues During Chronic Exposure to Bioelectric Blocking Agents," in: "Ontogenesis, Symp. Neurogen. 12th Sci. Conf., Prague, 1.970, pp 19-27. 33. Curio, E. "Verhaltensstudien am Trauerschnapper," Z. TIERPHYSIOL. (Supply, Vol 3, 1959, pp 1-110. 34. Cuvier, F. "Experimental Research on the Properties and Functions of the Nervous System," Paris, 1842. 32 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 `~~ Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 35. llrees, 0. "Utersuchungen Uber die Angeborenen Verhaltensweisen bei Sprinspinnen (Salticidae)," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 9, 1952, pp 169-207. 36. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, J. "The Interactions of Unlearned Behaviour Patterns and Learning in Mammals," in: "Brain Mechanisms and Learning," Oxford, 1961, pp 53-57. 37. Fox, M. W. "Neurobehavioral Development and the Genotype-Environment Interaction," QUART. REV. BIOL., Vol 45, 1970, p 131. 38. Fox, M. W.; Inman, 0.; and Glisson, S. "Age Differences in Central Nervous Effects of Visual Deprivation in the Dog," DEVELOP. PSYCHOBIOL., Vol 1, 1968, pp 48-54. 39. Furness, W. H. "Observations on the Mentality of Chimpanzees and Orangutans," PROC. AMER. PHIL. SOC., Vol 55, 1916, pp 281-290. 40. Galambos, R., and Worden, F. G. "Auditory Processing of Biologically Significant Sounds," NEUROSCI. RES. PROGR. BULL., Vol 10, 1972, pp 31-42. 41. Gerall, H. D. "Effect of Social ]:solation and Physical Confinement on Mother and Sexual Behavior of Guinea Pigs," J. PERS. SOC. PSYCHOL., Vol 2, 1965, pp 460-472. 42. Grohman, J. "Modifikation oder Functionsreifung? Ein Beitrag zur Klarung der Wechselseitigem Beziehungen Zwischen Instinkthandlung and Erfahrung," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 2, 1.939, pp 142-144 . 43. Von Haartman, L., and Lohrl, H. "Lautausserungen der Trauer- and Halsbandfliegen-Sc}~neppersMuscicapa p. Hypoleuca.(Pall.) and M. A. Albicollis Memmink," ORNIS FENN., Vol 27, 1950, pp 85-97. 44. Hamburger, V. N. "Some Aspects of the Embryology of Behavior," QUART. REV. BIOL., Vol 38, 1963, pp 342-365. 45. Hamburger, V. N.; Balaban, M.; Oppenheim, R.; and Wenger, E. "Periodic Motility of Normal and Spinal Chick Embryos Between 8 and 17 Days of Incubation," J. EXPTL. ZOOL., Vol 159, 1965, pp 1-14. 46. Harlow, H. F.; Harlow, M. K.; and Suomi, S. J. "From Thought to Therapy: Lessons From a Primate Laboratory," AMER. SCI., Vol 59, No 5, 1971, pp 538-542. 47. Harper, L. V. "The Effects of Isolation From Birth on the Social Behaviour of Guinea Pigs in Adulthood," ANIM. BEHAV., Vol 16, 1968, PP 58-64. 48. Hein, A., and Held, R. "Dissociation of the Visual. Placing Response Into Elicited and Guided Components," SCIENCE, Vol 58, 1967, pp 390-392. 33 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 49. Heinroth, 0. "Lautausserungen der Vogel," J. ORNIT., Vol 72, 1924, pp 223-244. 50. Ile1d, R., and Bauer, J. A. Jr. "Visually Guided Reaching in Infant Monkeys After Restricted Rearing," SCIENCE, Vol 155, 1967,, pp 718-720. 51. Kovach, J. K., and Kling, A. "Mechanisms of Neonate Sucking Behavior in the Kitten," ANIM. BEHAV., Vol 15, No 1, 1967, pp 91-101. 52. Lanyon, W. E. "The Comparative Biology of Meadowlarks (Sturnella) in Wisconsin," PUBL. NUTALL. ORN. CLUB, Vol 1, 1957, pp 1-67. 53. Lindsley, D. B.; Wendt, R. H.; Lindsley, D. F.; Fox, S. S.; Howell, J.; and Aday, W. R. "Diurnal Activity Behaviour and EEG Responses in Visually Deprived Monkeys," ANN. N. Y. ACAD. SCI., Vol 117, 1964, pp 546-574. 54. Lohrl, H. "Zum Verhalten Einiger Singvogel vor and Nach Flugewerde," VOGEL'WARTE, Vol 15, 1950, pp 213-217. 55. Marler, P. "The Voice of the Chaffinch and Its Function as a Language," IBIS, Vol 98, 1956, pp 231-261. 56. Idem, "Inheritance and Learning in the Development of Animal Vocaliza- tion," in: "Acoustic Behaviour of Animals," (ed. R. G. Busnel), Amsterdam, 1963, pp 228-243, 794-797. '. 57. Melzar_k, R., and Scott, T. H. "The Effects of Early Experience on the Response to Pain," J. COMP. PHYSIOL. PSYCHOL., Vol 50, 1957, pp 155-161. 58. Messmer, E., and Messmer, I. "Die Entwicklung der Lautausserungen and Einiger Verhaltensweisen der Amsel (Turdus Merula Merula L.) Unter Naturlichen Bedingungen and Nach Einzelaufzucht in Schal.ldichten Raumen," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 13, 1956, pp 341-441. 59. Miller, L. "The Biography of Nip and Tuck, a Study of Instincts in Birds," CONDOR, Vol 23, 1921, pp 41-47. 6U. 1ysl.ivecek, J. "Electrophysiological Parameters in Undernourished Developing Brain: Dog and Rat," in: "Postnatal Development of Phenotype," (S. Kazda and V. N. Denenberg, eds.), Prague, 1970, pp 85-94. 61. Myslivecek, J.; Safanda, J.; Chaloupka, Z.; and Semiginovsky, B. "Effects of Postnatal. Influences of Conditioning and Some Functional and Structural Brain Parameters in Rats," ACTA NEUROBIOL. EXPER., Vol 34, No 9, 1974, pp 99-111. 62. Nice, M. M. "Studi.es in the Life History of the Song Sparrow and Other Passerines," New York, Vol 6, 328 pp. 3~+ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 35 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY 63. Petersen, B.; Lundgren, L.; and Wilson, L. "The Development of Flight Capacity in a Butterfly," BEHAVIOUR, Vol 10, 1957, pp 324-339. 64, Riggs, G.; Winter, P.; and Mayer, W. "Effects of Deafening on the Vocal Behaviour of the Squirrel Monkey," FOL. PRIMAT., Vol 17, 1973, pp 404-417. 65. Rosenzweig, M. R.; Bennet, E. L.; and Diamond, M. C. "Brain Changes in Response to Experience," SCI. AMER., Vol 227, No 2, 1972, pp 46-52. 66. Ross, S.; Denenberg, V. N.; Sawin, R. B.; and Meyer, P, "Changes in Nest Building Behaviour in Multiparous Rabbits," BRIT. J. ANIM. BEHAV., Vol 4, 1956, pp 69-74. 67. Safanda, J.; Myslivecek, J.; and Franli.ova, S. "Biochemical Character- istics of the Rat Brain After Low Protein. Diet," ACTIV. NERV. SUPER., Vol 13, 1971, pp 102-].03. 68. Sauer, F. "Die Entwicklung der Lautausserungen vom Ei ab Schalldicht Gehaltener Dorngrasmucken (Sylvia c. Communis, Latham) in Vergleich mit Spater Isolierten and mit Wildlebenden.:Artgenossen," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 11, 1954, pp 10-93. 69. Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. "Weitere Beitrage zur Social and Individual- psychologie des Haushuhns," Ibid,, Vol 82, 1923, pp 60-87. 70. Seitz, A. "Untersuchungen Uber Angeborene Verhaltensweisen bei Caniden. III. Beobachtungen an Marderhunden (Nyctereutes Procyonoides Gray)," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 12, 1955, pp 463-489. 71. Severaid, J. H. "The Natural History of the Picas Mammalian Genus Ochotona," Berkeley, 1958. 72. Singh, S. D. "Urban Monkeys," SCI. AMER., Vol 224, No'6, 1969. 73. Spalding, D. A. "Instinct, With Original Observations on Young Animals," in: "Readings in Ecology" (ed.: E. J. Kormondy), New Jersey, 1965,_49 pp. 74. Spigelman, M. N., and Bryden, M. P. "Effects of Early and Late Blind- ness on Auditory Spatial Learning in the Rat," NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, Vol 5, 1967, pp 267-274. 75. Stadler, D. "Die Vogelstimmenforschung als Wissenschaft," in: "Proc. 6th Intern. Orn. Congr..;~1929, p 338. 76. Thielcke-Poltz, H., and Thielclce, G. "Akustisches Lernen Verschiedenen Alter Schallisolierter Amseln (Turdud Merula L.) and die Entwicklung Erlernter Motive Ohne and mit Kunstlichem Einfluss von,Testosteron," Z. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 17, 1960, pp 211-244. Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4 77. Thorpe, W. H. "Bird Song: The Biology of Vocal Communication and Expression in Birds," Cambridge, 1961. 78. Valzelli, L. "Aggressive Behaviour Induced by Isolation," in: "Aggressive Behaviour" (eds.: S. Garattini and E. G. Sigg), Amsterdam, 1969a, pp. 70-76. 79. Idem, "The Exploratory Behaviour in Normal and Aggressive Mice," PSYCHOPHARMACOL., Vol 15, 1969b, pp 232-235. 80. Idem, "The 'Isolation Syndrome' in Mice," PSYCHOPHARMACOL., Vol 31, 1973, pp 305-309. 81. tdeiss, G. "Beobachtungen an Zwei Isoliert Aufgezogenen Hauskatzen," 7. TIERPSYCHOL., Vol 9, 1952, pp 351-462. 82. Winter, P.; Handley, P.; Ploog, D.; and Scott, D. "Ontogeny of Squirrel Monkey Ca11s Under Normal Conditions and Under Acoustic Isolation," BEHAVIOUR, Vol 47, 1973, pp 230-239. 83. Yerkes, R. M., and Learned, B. W. "Chimpanzee Intelligence and Its Vocal Expressions," Baltimore, 1925. 84. Yerkes, R. M., and Yerkes, A. W. "The Great Apes," New Haven, 1929. 85. Young, W. C. "Genetic and Psychological Determinants of Sexual Behaviour Patterns," in: "Hormones, Brain Function and Behaviour" (ed.: H. Hoagland), New York Acad. Press, 1957. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", 1977 10,657 CSO: 8144 END Declassified and Approved For Release 2012/05/10: CIA-RDP88B01125R000300120007-4