JPRS ID: 9748 TRANSLATIONS ONMAJOR USSR RIVER DIVERSION PROJECTS

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4
Release Decision: 
RIF
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
77
Document Creation Date: 
November 1, 2016
Sequence Number: 
44
Case Number: 
Content Type: 
REPORTS
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4.pdf5.06 MB
Body: 
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY JPRS L/9994 18 September 1981 U SS R Re o~t p L1FE SCIENCES BIOMEDI~AL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES CFOUO 12/81) FBIS FOREIGN BROADCAST INFORMATION SERVICE FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-04850R000400050044-4 NOTE JPRS publications contain information primarily from foreign - newspapers, periodicals and books, but also from news agency transmissions and broadcasts. Materials froc: fr~reign-language sou_ces are translated; those from English-language sources are transcribed or reprinted, with the original phrasing and - other ct-~aracteristics retained. Headlines, editorial reports, and material enclosed in braclcets are supplied by JPRS. Proces:,ing indicators such as [Text] , or [Excerpt] in the first line of each item, or following the last line of a brief, indicate how the original infarmation was processed. Where no processing indicator is given, the infor- mation was sums.arized or extracted. Unfamiliar names rendered phonetically or transliterated are enclosed in parentheses. Words or names preceded by a ques- tion mark and enclosed in parentheses were not clear ii~ the original but have been supplied as appropriate in context. Other unattributed parenthetical notes within the body of an item originate with the source. Times within items are as given by source. The contents of this publication in no way represent the poli- cies, views or attitudes of the U.S. Government. , COPYRIGHT LAWS AND REGULA,TIONS GOVERNING OWNERSHIP OF MATERIALS REPRODUCED HEREIN REQUIRE THAT DISSEMINATION OF THIS PUBLICATION BE RESTRICTED FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY. APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICiAL USE ONLY JPRS L/9994 18 September 1981 USSR REPORT LIFE $CIENCES BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES (FOUO 12/81) CONTENTS BIOCHEMISTRY - Recombinant Origin of the Epidemic Influenza Virus Strain A/USSR/90/77 1 BIOTECHNOLOGY Method of Simulating Hemodynamic Effecta of Weightleasness and Refinement of the Chibis Protective Vacuum Suit Used in This Method 5 PHYSIOLOGY Sleep and Mental Fitness 9 Sensibility of Auditory and Tactile Sensory Systems When Stimulated Separately and Together 18 Dynamics of Psychophysiological and Cardiovascular Parametere During Operator Work in the Anticipation and Tracking Mo:~e 21 Evaluating Operator Stress on the Basis of Statiatical Characteristics of an Electrocardiogram 27 Comparative Characteristics of Man's Resistance to I'rolor.ged E:cposure To Accelera~:ions Varying in Gravity Gradients 35 FunctionAl Mobility ~f the Visual Analyzer 39 Coordination of Eye-Head Movements in the Gaze Fixation Reaction. 44 _ PSXCHOLOGY Eighth Transcaucasiau Conference of Paychologiets 51 - a- [III - USSR - 21.a S&T FOUO] APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 Methoda for Analyzing Psychophysiological Cauaes of EXroneous Actions T'hat Violated the Regulations for Flying and Operating Aviation Equipment Causing Flight Accidents, and Preconditions Thereof 53 Psychology of Cognition Discussed at the Twenty-Second International Psychological Congress 64 26th CPSU Congress and Tasks for Psychological Science 68 - b - ~ APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 - FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONL.Y BIOCHEMISTRY UDC 578.2 RECOMBINANT ORIGIN OF THE EPIDEMIC INFLUENZA VIRUS STRAIN A/USSR/90/77 Nbscow DOKLADY AKADEMII NALTK SSSR in Russian Vol 257, No 3, 1981 pp 721-724 [Article by Yu. V. Kozlov, A. A. Shilov, A. G. Kurmanova, V. G. Gorbulev, Ya. M. . Selivanov, V. M. Zhdanov, and Academician A. A. Bayev, USSR Academy of Sciences Institute of Molecular Biology and USSR Academy of Medical Sciences Institute of Virology imeni D. I. Ivanovskiy, M~scow] (Text) Unusually high variability is typical of the influenza virus, and it is mainly this unique feature that makes control of this infection's spread :~.~ficult. Trro basic forms of antigenic variability of influenza virus are distinguished--anti- genic ~rift and antigenic shift. Insignificant and gradual change in the antigenic properties of proteins making up the viral membrane--hemagglutinin (HA) and neuraminidase (NA)--is commonly referred to as antiger.ic drift. The pressure of antibodies in a partially immune host population is apparently the mechanism that defines the selection direction for a virus that has undergone alteration in the course of drift. Arisal of a virus having new antigenic properties is referred to - as antigenic shift. Appearance of such a"new" virus elicits the periedically arising pandemics of influenza. It is believed.that the arisal of new pandemic virus strains is associated with recombination of different viruses. Inasmuch as influenza virus has a segmented genome represented by eight separate RNA fragments coding different virus-specific proteins, recombination is interpreted in this case as simple redistribution of fragments (genes), responsible for coding surface anti- - gens, between different viruses circulating�in the same host . There is now sutf.icient information demonstrating such a mechanism for arisal of new serotypes of influenza virus (1,2). In the last few decades the serotypes of circulating human influenza virus have included H1N1 (1946-1956), H2N2 (1957-1968) and H3N2 (from 1968 to the present). In 1977 an epidemic of serotype H1N1 virus arose, embracing the entire Northern Hemisphere (3). Arisal of this epidemic strain, one having the same serotype as the virus that appeared in 1946-1956 and which had already circulated in this - generation, was a surprise, inasmuch as a large proportion of the population more than 20 years old had already had antibodies to this virus. It was dem~nstrated by serological methods (5) and by means of oligonucleotide analysis of total viral RNA (6,7) that the influenza virus str?~.ns revealed in 1977 (A/USSR/90/77) and a number of Chinese strains have more similarity to strains isolated in 1950 (A/FW/1/50) than to l~ter (A/FM/1/47) or more .recent strains (A/C/1/56, A/Denver/1/57) of the same serotype, H1N1. Nevertheless the question as to the mechanism behind arisal of the new epidemic strain USSR/90/77 remains unclear. 1 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2447/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400454444-4 FOR OFFICIAL USF, ONLY Our objective was to reveal the genetic kinship of the new (A/USSR/90/77) and old (A/FM/1/47 and A/FW/1/50) strains of serotype H1N1 influenza virus by comparing the structure of individual genes by the method of oligonucleotide analysis. This method permi.ts highly precise analysis of insignificant di_fferences in the struc- ture of compared RNA molecules, and it does not require significant quantities of material. Fragments of viral genome RNA were separated by electrophoresis in 1.6 percent agarose or 2.8 percent acrylamide gel. We were unable to acrieve sufficiently good separation of genes P1 and P3, and for this reason they were analyzed together. RNA fragments eluted from gel were processed with T1 RNAase and alkaline phosphatase. The obtained oligonucleo tides were labeled at their 5'-ends with ~-P32-ATP and polynucleotide kinase. Labeled T1-oligonucleotides were subjected to double electro- phoresis in polyacrylamide gel, and to autoradiography. The position of the oligo- nucleotide on the fingerprint depends strictly on its length and on its nucleotide composition; this is why any substitutions in the RNA under analysis are easy to determine from change in position, disappearance, or appearance ~f a new oligo- nucleotide. The double electrophoresis system we developed provides for optimum resolution of oligonucleotides more than 10 members long, and thus it permits us to analyze a sizeable sequence of each gene. Because oligonucleotides are distri- buted statistically in the analyzed RNA, it may be presumed that they reliably represent the entire sequence of the analyzed RNA. We obtained oligonucleotide maps (fingerprints) from the individual genes of three influenza virus strains: USSR/90/77, FW/1/50, and FM/1/47. F:igure 1[figures not reproduced] snows the fingerprints of M genes from the three studied strains, and a diagram representing the distribution of the analyzed oligonucleotides. It is easy to see that the distribution patterns of the oligonucleotides of M genes are extremely similar for strains USSR and FM, and that they differ significantly from the pattern for the M gene of strain FW. A different situation is abserved for NP genes. fihe fingerprints of NP genes from strains USSR and FW are very similar, and they differ significantly from the fingerprint of the NP gene from strain FM (Figure 2i. The fingerprints of other genes, which were obtained in similar fashion, are not presented in this paper. Table 1 was compiled from an analysis of the fingerprints of individual genes from the the analyzed influenza virus strains. For the convenience of analysis, the virus strain under analysis, USSR/90/77, was compared in pairs with earlier isolates of the virus--FW/1/50 and FM,/1/47. It follows from the table that the number of oligo- nucleotides common to strains USSR/90/77 and FW/1/50 is significantly greater than that common to USSR/90/77 and FM/1/47. This law extends to all genes, with the exception of genes coding M proteins. Inasmuch as the proportion of a genome repre- sented in analyzed oligonucleotides differs for different genes, the percentage of substituted bases in the analyzed part of the gene must be assessed on the basis of additional calculations. This magnitude is a stricter reflection of the degree of structural similarity (or divergence) of compared genes, inasmuch as it reflects the number of base substitutions for every 100 nucleotides of the compazed sequences. Because this magnitude is minimal for genes P1 + P3, P2, NP, and NS of USSR and FW isolates, these genes have significant structural similarity. Genes P2 and NP of these two strains have maximum structural similarity--their. percentages of base substitution are 0.35 and 0.33 respectively. Genes HA and NA of tZese two strains diverged somewhat more greatly, which is well consistent with published data on the high rate of variability of these two genes, which are responsible for antigenic drift of the virus (9) � 2 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007102/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 - FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY . Table 1 Minimum No. of RNA Analyzed Distribution of Analyzed Proportior.of Gene Base Substitutions Frag- Pairs of Oligonucleotides (No. Represented In in Oligon~_cleotides - ment Virus of Oligonucleotides) Analyzed O~igo- Percent- (Gene) ~trains USSR~* USSR-** Total nucleotides, ~ No.*** age**** P 1+ P 3 iJSSR/FW 12 6 72 28 15 1. 05 USSR/FM 29 22 55 40 2�8 P2 USSR/FW 1 3 56 3 0.35 USSR/FM 12 15 45 34.5 21 2.5 USSR/E~V 7 10 28 29.5 13 2.3 HA USSR/FM 17 13 18 23 4.0 USSR/F~nl 3 '8 41 42.5 9 1.5 NH USSR/FM 11 11 33 16 2�6 USSR/FW 2 1 36 38 2 0.33 NP USSR/FM 23 12 15 29 4�8 M USSR/EW 15 8 10 48 19 4'8. USSR~'FM 4 4 21 6 1.5 USSR/EW 5 3 24 66 6 1.4 NS USSR/FM 8 9 21 13 3.0 * USSR --number of oligonucleotides four~d in the given gene of strain L'SSR/'90/77 but absent in the corresponding gene of strain EW/1/50 or FM/1/47. **USSR'--number of oliqonucleotides found in the given gene of strain FW/1/50 ~ or FM/1/47 but absent from the corresponding gene of strain USSR/90/77. � ***The minimum number of base substitutions was computed in accordance with the the procedure described by Palese (6). ****The percentage of base substitutions was calculated on the basis of the propor- tion of sequences of the given gene represented in the analyzed oligonucleotides. This magnitude typifies the number of base substitutions for every 100 bases of the analyzed sequence, and it reflects the degree of divergence of the compared genes. The calculations were made on the basis of information on the molecular weight of influenza virus genes, provided in (12). The dimensions of oligo- nucleotides were determined by means of incomplete alkaline hydrolysis of eluted oligonucleotide, followed by electrophoretic separation of the products _ in polyacrylamide gel. Comparing the percentages of base substitutions for the M gene of USSR strain in relation to strains FW and FM, we can see that it is significantly closer to that of the 1947 isolate--FM/1/47--than to FW/1/50. Thus all genes, except for the M gene of influenza virus strain USSR/90/77, have structural simitarity with strain FW/1/50, and they cli ffer significantly from those of strain FM/1/47. ~ Obviously the new virus arose as a result of recombination of the earlier strain EW/1/50, or its drift variant, wi+-..h another virus having an M gene structurally closer to that of strain FM/1/47. As a result of such recombination (exchange of genes), the new virus, which retained seven genes of the parent strain FW/1/50, 3 ~'~R OFF[CIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400450044-4 FOR aFFICIAL USE ONLY obtained a new M gene. Thus our data unambiguously indicate that epidemic influenza virus strain USSR,/90/77 came about as a result of recombination (exchange of genes) between earlier strains FW/1/50 and FM/1/47. 'I'he sort of selective advantages the recombinant virus obtained as a result remain - unclear. The biological significance of recombination in relation to the M gene of - this virus is not clear. 'I'hese questions require further research. BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Laver, W. G. and Webster, R. G., VIROLOGY, Vol 51, No 2~ 1973, p 384. 2. Desselberger, U., Nakajima, K., et al., PROC. NAT. ACAD. SCI. USA, Vol 75, No 7, 1978, p 3341. 3. Center for Disease Control, Atlanta Georgia, MORBIDITY AND MORTALITY WEEKLY REPORT, Vol 27, 1978, p 24. 4. Zhdanov,.V. M., Lvov, N. A., et al., LANCET, Vol 1, 1978, p 294� 5. Center for Disease Control, Atlanta, Georgia, MORBIUITY AND N10RTALITY WEEKLY REPORT, Vol 47, 1~`78, p 16. 6. Nakajima, K., Desselberger, U. and Palese, P., NATURE, Vol 274, 1978, p 334. younq, ,7., Desselberger, U. and Palese, P., CELL~ Vol 18, 1979, p 173. 8. Chanok, R. M., Cocbutn, W. C., et al., BUI~L. WORLD HEALTH ORG., Vol 45, 1971, p 119. 9. Vebster, R. and Laver, U. G., "Virusy grippa i gripp" [Influenza Viruses and Influenza], Nbscow, Izd-vo "Meditsina", I.978� 10. Efstratiadis, A., Vournakis, J. N., et al., NUCL. ACID. REB., Vol 4, No 12, 1977, p 4165. 11. Pedersen, F. and Haseltin, W., N~THODS IN ENZYMOLOGY, Vol 65, 1980, p 680. 12. Desselberger, U. and Palese, P., VIROLOGY, Vol 88, No 2, 1978, p 394. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", "Doklady Akademii nauk SSSR", 1981 11004 . CSO: 1840/257 ~ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPR~VED F~R RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL U.ciE ~NLY BIOTECHNOLQGY UDC: 523.612-1 METHOD OF SIMULATING HEMODYNAMIC EFFECTS OF WEIGHTLESSNESS AND REFINEMENT OF THE CHIBIS PROTECTIVE VACUUM SUIT t7SED IN THIS METHOD Moscow IZVESTIYa AKADEMII NAUK SSSR: SERIYA BIOLOGICHESKAYA in Russian No 3, May-Jun 81 (manuscript received 24 Jun 80) pp 443-445 [Articl.e by E. V. Lapayev, G. I. Pavlov, V. G. Voloshin and V. Ye. Grishanov] [Text] The acute period of adaptation to weightlessness is associ- ated with shifting of blood to the upper half of the body. A method of simulating this condition is described in this article, which involves creation of excess pi~?G~!!re in the Chibis [or Chybis] protective vacuum suit [PVS] after it was improved. It is known that there is redistribution of blood and body fluids to the upper half of the body in weightlessness, and this causes a number of adverse reactions in a cosmonauts, including vestibular disturbances (Bryanov et al., 1975). It is necessary to simulate these phenomena on earth to develop the means of preventing these reac- tions and to study the phenomenology thereof. One usually uses antiorthostatic body position (head-down tilt) at different angles and for different periods of time for this purpose. However, it is impossible to - combine such a technique with vestibular studies, since there are no tables with a sufficient angle of inclination, and one cannot investigate such parameters as, for example, resjstance of the vestibular analyzer to Coriolis accelerations. Use of an anti-G suit (AGS) for redistribution of blood to the upper hal~ o~ the body also fails to yield the desired ~esults, since the area of compensation in the garment constitutes SO% of the body surface and there are open sections (buttocks, perineum, etc.) in which blood is deposited, rather than shiftit~g upe . We propose a method of simulating the hemodynamic effects of weightless~ess that does not have such flaws. It consists of placing the lower half of the subject into a protective [or prophylactic] vacuum garment (PVS), the Chibis (Genin et al., 1973; Barer et al., 1975), in which one creates graded positive ai~ .Fressure. Since the human body in the PVS is surrounded by a single air pillow, this pressure is transmitted uniformly to all parts of the body and causes migr:~tio~i of blood to the upper half of the body. At the same time, the Chibis PVS (stff,~, airtight "troiisers") is small and permits placing the sub~ect on various research stands, for example, vestibulometric ones, in any position. 5 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED F~R RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY , ~ ~ A ~j U .9 3 Air pus?ped ~n ' ~ B ; _ -~--p ~ ~ or out - - ,\6 i ' '"__b" _ ?7V i i - - ~ i 5 ~ ~ ~ S1ow-up of A G O 2 _ Figure 1. General diagram of the Chibis garment, elements of improvement of s}stem of connecting micropump to the sealing shutter / ~ A O BOZ Z ' M M- ~ J 3 to atmos h. to atnosphere , ~ , - ~vs rv~ ~ pumping out pumping in Figure 2. Diagram of manometer connection to measure excess pressure The proposed method involves refinement of the Chibis PVS so that both positive and negative pressure (in relation to ambient level) can be regulated in it. _ The purpose of refinement is to provide for the following: operation of the 1~tD-2G micropump in two modes--pumping air in and out; to render the garment airtight [seal it] at the subject's waistline when there is excess or negative pressure; ability to monitor the variable pressure using the same instrument. ~ Figures 1 and 2 illustrate the essential elements of the improvement. Figure 1 illustrates the general scheme of operation of the garment, as well as elements of improvement of the system of operating the micropump and sealing shutter. When turned on in the regular mode, micropump (1) only pumps air out of the PVS.~ In our improved version, it is turned on by switch (2), which makes it possible 6 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR t~FFICIAL USE ONLY , to alter the polarity on the motor ~ coils and thereby cause air to be _ ~ either pumped in or out of the PVS. f~ In the position it operates in ti ~ ~ , ~ the pumping out mode and in the position in the pumping in mode. In the regular variant, sealing the ' y`" Chibis garment around the subject's body ' (3) is provided by a rubber shutter (4) ~1 . ~ ~ 1r~ ~ executed in the form of a valve that II.~V~~. - is pressed to the body when negative pressure is created in the PVS; it ~ allows excess amounts of air to pass _ , ~ freely into the garment. To render j the PVS airtight in the presence of ~ excess pressure, an additional circular ! valve (5) is pasted over the perimeter of the inner edge of shutter (4) . When ~ excess pressure is created, when the Figure 3. gas rea~hes space (6) between valves (4) and (5) it presses valve (S) to Subject wearing Chibis garment in a the body and reliably seals the PVS. vestibulumetric chair Valve (7), which permits smooth change or delivery of air from the atmosphere (when pressure is negative in the PVS), or release thereof into the atmosphere (with excess pressure), is used to regulate the levels of excess and negative pressure in the PVS. Manometer [pressure gauge] (8) is used to monitor the negative pressure in the regular variant. In order to allow us to measure excess pressure with the same gauge, we installed valve (9), the connection of which is illustrated in detail in Figure 2. We installed a series produced instrument as manometer M in the PVS, which has an airtight compartment that is connected with the inside of the PVS by means of coupling (1). The inside of the membrane chamber (2) opens into the atmosphere through coupling (3) in the regular variant. When negative pressure is created in the PVS, the pressure drops in the cavity of M and the membrane chamber (2) ex- pands, and this is indicated by a pointer. With our refinement of the instrument, M is connected to the PVS by valve (4), which has two independent pneumatic channels. In position A(-), the valve provides for the regular mode of connecting the mano- meter: coupling (1) is connected to the PVS, coupling (3) goes out to the atmosphere and the gauge measures negative pressure. In position B(+), couplinb (3) is connected to the inside of the PVS, while coupling (1) and tlie interior of M goes into the,atmosphere. When excess pressure is created in the PVS, the membrane chamber (2) expands and the gauge measures excess pressure. FOR OFFICCAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2447/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400454444-4 FOR OFFtCIAL USE ONLY ; - The proposed method of simulating the hemodynamic effects of weightlessness by means ~ ~f a modified Chibis PVS has been used with success in our experimental tests - ~(~i~ure 3), which enabled us to obtain a number of theoretically important data can- cerning the dependence of vestibular analyzer function on altered hemadynamicti. ~ BIBLIOGRAPHY i 1. Bryanov, I. I., Matsnev, E. I. and Yakovleva, I. Ya., KOSM. BIOL. I AVIAKOSM. - MEDITSINA, No 3, 1975, p 85. 2. Genin, A. M., Goryushev, V. P. and Pestov, I. D., BYULLETEN': OTKRYTIYA, IZOBRETENIYA, PROMYSHLENNYYE OBRAZTSY, TOVARNYYE ZNAKI [Bulletin: Discoveries, Inventions, Comanercial Samples and Trademarks], No 6, 1973, p 10. 3. Barer, A. S., Savinov, A. P. and Sever.in, G. I., KOSM. BIOL. I AV~AKOSM. MEDITSINA, No 1, 1975, p 41. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", "Izvestiya AN SSSR, seriya biologicheskaya", 1981 10,657 CSO: 8144/1374 8 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400054444-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY PHYSIOLOGY UDC: 612.741 SLEEP AND MENTAL FITNESS Moscow FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA in Russian Vol 6, No 6, Nov-Dec 80 pp 1094-1101 [Article by I. S. Kandror and V. S. Rotenberg, All-Union Scientific Researcn Institute of Railroad Hygiene, Moscow, and First Moscow Medical Institute, submitted 13 Jul 79] [Text] The dependence of the level of man's fitness for work on integrity, duration and quality of sleep is not only common knowledge from self-observations, but it is confirmed by numerous experimental studies of the effect of sleep depriyation [1-4]. However, the mere statement of such a dependence is not enough to solve many problems of industrial physiology and hygiene. It is imperative to identify the specific physiological processes in the organism that occur during normal nocCurnal sleep and have an effect on man's overall functional state and, in parti- cular, his fitness for work during the subsequent waking period. = Until recently, it was not quite clear as to what sleep is as a state of the brain = and what processes are involved in restoring the fitness of a tired man after proier sleep. We do not know how various changes in sleeping and waking schedule affect these processes. Yet these are questions of basic significance to industrial physiology and hygiene, since without answers to them it is impossible to determ~ne which changes in the schedule are permissible without detriment to ~qan's heal,th ar~d fitness for work. At the present time, realistic opportunities have emerged for investigation of these matters, since conceptions are already beginning to fornt about some factors that affect human fitness. At the same time, experi~ental data about the structural organization of sleep and functional significance of its differer.t phases and stages are accumulating rapidly. ~ A number of psychological studies have demonstrated that both physical and mental fitness of a healthy man are largely determined by t.he force of a subject's motiva- tion (interest) for a given activity, his level of wakefulness (i.e., degree of activity), direction and stability of attention, mnestic capacities and capacity for solving problems creatively. The relati.onship is .f.ound to be complex and nonlinear between motivation and level of activity, oti the one hand, and fitness, on the other: f itness for work diminishes, not only when the wakefulness level and motivation are low, but when ther~ is excessively high motivation and excessive (unproductive) activity. This function is sometimes c.allea the "law of Yerkes-Dodson." ~ This happens not only ~n a state of marked neurotic an:ciety, but in healthy indi- ~ viduals in a state uf. severe emotional excitement. Psychological stress disorganizes - occupational performance, does not permit concentrating on its subject, leads to fai:lure or error, r!s well as autonomic changes which, in turn, become a hindrance f ~ _ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-04850R000400050044-4 FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONLY to normal performance. All these circumstances ultimately diminish significantly an individual's fitness and reliability. Thus, for each type of productive acti- vity there is apparently its own optimum of activity, with reduction of which drowsiness and boredom appear and surpassing of which causes errors due to hasti- ness, and both are dangerous to production. In order to discuss in the most general form the question of a link between the above factors and sleep, we must briefly describe the current conceptions on the structure and function of sleep. Studies of the last few decades have shown ttxat nocturnal sleep is divided into several phases or stages [5], and that it by no means constitutes a passive state of the brain, rather it is an active one. On the whole, the number of actively functioning neurons in the brain during sleep is not smaller than in a waking state, and it is even greater in some phases of sleep (rapid sleep) than in a tense waking state. As we know, two distinct phases are clearly differentiated in sleep, so-called slow and rapid (paradoxical or REM). Both phases differ, not only in their electro- _ physiological manifestations, but psychological ones, and they alternate during . the night, forming 4-5 cycles. We still do not have an exhaustive idea about the functional role of the different sleep phases and cycles. However, there are some rather convincing data to the effect that there are differences between the functional significance of different phases of sleep. Thus, many researchers [6, 7] mention the important role of delta sleep in organization of mnestic functions (memory functions). It was demonstrated that this sleep phase has a beneficial effect on recall of material memorized before sleeping. Symptoms of selective deprivation thereof are also indicative of such a function of delta sleep: tired ["jaded"] feeling, increased fatigability, diminished attention, worsening of mood, well-being and slee~iness [8]. These symptoms can be interpreted as the result of being overloaded with information, which makes it difficult to perceive and screen new information. In the opinion of a number of researchers, the function of rapid sleep is to psychologically stabilize the personality, eliminate neurotic anxiety, elRaiid emotional adaptation to unusual and highly significant information (9]. p - sleep is considered to be among the main mechanisms of psychological defense, which permits resolution of intrapsychic conflicts (struggle between motives) and thus is instrumental in reducing excessive emotional stress [10-12]. ~ In view of this role of rapid sleep, its significance to psychological adaptation depends largely on personality distinctions. This phase of sleep most probably does not play a particularly important role and is often reduced in highly active, optimistic and sthenic subjects, who tend to overlook negative information, who are free of internal conflicts and actively overcome external barriers. This may also be associated with decrease in total duration of sleep, which does not have an adverse effect on their fitness for work [13]. Conversely, rapid sleep is particularly necessary for sensitive personalities, who tend to fix on complex psychological shadings of interpersonal relations, who are vulnerable to negative information, profoundly emotional about conflicts, anxious 10 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPR~VED F~R RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY and are subject to frequent worsening of mood. As a rule their rapid sleep is longer and saturated with dreams [13]. In the presence of functional inadequacy of the "rapid sleep--dreams" system, the risk of development of neurotic states grows higher for such subjects [14]. Even an insignificant, chronic reduction in total sleeping time diminishes fitness of these individuals. The above data enable us to now turn to concrete analysis of correlations between changes in sleep and fitness in the presence of various functional states and changes in work, rest and sleep schedule. - Let us first discuss the results of experiments involving total deprivation of sleep for several days and gradual reduction of total duration of sleep. In the former case, weakening of higher mental functions was observed [4, 15, 16), _ capacity to concentrate attention, orientation in a new situation and adaptation to it. This is largely related to unsurmountable sleepiness, when.reactions to exo- genous stimuli are delayed are lacking entirely. With longer complete deprivation, one observes riisorders in the affective area as well: groundless irritability, aggressiveness, suspicion, etc., which diminishes work fitness drastically, in par- ticular due to diminishe~ interest in solving the presented problems. It is import- ant to indicate that while sleepiness and diminished attention are more or less euqally inherent in differenr subjects, the thresholds of onset of affective disorders and their intensity are quite variable, and they apparently depend on the role of sleep, particularly its rapid phase, in processes of emotional stabiliza- tion in a given subject. Thus, individuals with a high level of self-control perform work involving detection of a signal in the presence of noise better after sleep deprivation than individuals who are very impulsive. The latter also present more marked mnestic disorders, i.e., impaired ability to retain and recall [17]. During the first night of recovery, emotionally stable individuals show an increase in delta sleep and, only later, of rapid sleep, while in those with emotional in- stability there is often compensatory increase at f irst in expressly rapid sleep [18]� While fitness for work can diminish very drastically with total sleep deprivation, gradual reduction of overall sleeping time (to a certain minimum level) does not, according to some authors [19, 20], necessarily lead to a significant decline of fitness during the waking period. Reduction of sleeping time to 5.5 h per day is critical. In the case of a 5-h and particularly 4-h sleep schedule, the subjects present an appreciable deterioration of mood, they tire rapidly, soon become irritable and they are unable to concentrate on their work. It becomes very diffi- cult for them to sustain a waking state. The results reported in the literature of polygraph studies revealed that, in the case of 5.5-h sleep, there was a re- duction in duration of all sleep stages, as compared to the background period, but especially of the rapid sleep stage. With the change to a 4-h sleep period, rapid sleep diminished even more, there was drastic reduction of the second stage, but rel.ative increase in duration of the fourth stage. After changing to a normal sleep schedule, fitness and well-being reached their initial levels as early as the third day, a:~though total duration of sleep was still shorter than before the experiment for a long time (because of falling asleep 1-1.5 h later). A check of sleep structur.e on the lOth recovery night revealed that total duration of sleep was still somewhcit sharter and there was reduction of the rapid sleep phase. It appeared that the subjects were indeed able to change to a shorter sleep schedule. FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY However, one must take into consideration the following circumstances: when sleep is reduced to 5~nd 4 h, there is relative compensatory increase in delta sleep due to reduction of rapid sleep and, as we mentioned above, this is far from being always harmless for many subjects. Moreover, it is apparent that one cannot excessively generalize data obtained on a small number of subjects. Finally, one must take into consideration the data of researchers [21, 22] who report that, altnough there was an effect of initial "return" of delta sleep in the recovery night after a reduced 5.5-h sleep period, there was a decline of fitnes~a in the daytime during the period of re.duced sleep schedule even when the reduction of the fourth stage was quite negligible, as compared to the background period. This is apparently indicative of the fact that with artificial reduction of sleep the most urgent need is to satisfy the delta sleep requirement. For this reason, we cannot rule out the possibility that restoration of impaired balance after long-term gradual reduction is also a long-term process and the lOth recovery night is not yet informative enough. The opinion has also been voiced [23] that even if - sleep reduction does not cause diminished fitness for work in the immediate period, it may be deleterious to health at a later time, and if chronic adjustment to a S-h sleep schedule per day is possible, one cannot yet determine the "price" that the body pays for such adjustment. Indee3, there are data [24] to the effect that successful performance of psycholo- ~ gical tasks in the case of chronic shortage of sleep is associated with strain on the body's energy systems, and this is apparently necessary to compensate for the impaired organization of information during sleep. Naiton [23] stresses that each subject has his own individual limit of sleep reduction, and it may be believed that this is largely determined by the individual rapid sleep requirements. The above data indicate that, in a number of cases, the�body tries to compensate, first of all for delta sleep when sleep is reduced, not only at the expense of other stages of slow sleep, but even rapid sleep. However, tY:is is by no means indicative of the low functional significance of rapid sleep. With reference to the correlations between sleep structure and work fitness in the case of both normal and altered sleep schedule, it must be borne in mind that emotional tension of the neurotic anxiety type has the most marked effect on creative activity, on solving problems that require an original approach, when it is not enough to base oneself solely on acquired skills~and knowlEdge acquired pre- viously. In view of ttie role of rapid sleep in compensation of neurotic anxiety, we can assume that the rapid sleep phase plays a particularly important part in solving expressly such problems, particularly among highly sensitive subjects. Indeed, experiments with rapid sleep deprivation [25, 26] revealed that selective deprivation of this phase worsens performance of tasks that require creative (di- vergent) thinking and has virtually no effect on tasks that require a formal and logical approach or simple retention. Our studies [27] revealed that those who react with excessively rigid emotional . excitement to emotional stress in a waking state and cope worse with solving logic problems regain their productivity rapidly after compensatory increase in the rapid sleep phase. In individuals engaged in the intensiye study o� a foreign l,angua~e (by the so-called "immersion" method), an increase in rapid sleep phase, as compared to the base level, was associated with better achievement in as.similating linguistic material FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00854R004400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY - without signs of increased stress, unlike those in whom there was no such increase in this phase of sleep [28]. At the same time, healthy, insensitive people who usually have a short sleep period arid relatively short rapid sleep phase under ordinary working conditions present good fitness for work and cope easily with standard psychological tests [29]. Hence, it should be concluded that the consequences of different changes in the sleep pattern are different for different individuals, depending on their personality - distinctions, current condition and nature of activity. Occupational activity is often related to a specific work schedule, regular or irre- gular alternation of day and night shifts of different duration or working only on the evening or night shift. According to many researcl~ers [30-33), daytime sleep is much shorter than usual when one works on the night shift. A somewhat unexpected finding was that this is associated with a tendency toward reduction of delta sleep, particularly during the first half of the daytime sleep period, and increase in rapid sl.eep phase. Some authors [34] believe that thi~; distinction is determined by circadian rhythm (hence the relative increase in this phase in the premorning hours of sleep), whereas the duration of delta sleep :~s determined by overall ~lura- - tion of wakefulness prior to sleeping [35] (the authors refer to the fact that, even after many years of a distort::d schedule, rapid sleep is particulally frequent and prolonged in the early morning hours). However, our findings are indicative of the no less importance of another factor-- degree of individual adjustment to night work. There is primary compensation of expressly delta sleep and the structure of sleep as a whole is virtually the same as the usual structure in individuals who are well-adjusted to night work and have no tendency toward anxiety reactions, whose need for rapid sleep is relatively minimal. In individuals who are subjectively inadequately adjusted to work on the night shift, the need for rapid sleep is greater, and this is manifested by its prin~ary "return" after night work, at the expense of reduction of delta sleep. The latency peri~d of rapid sleep is reducnd during daytime sleep in cases where the _ night shift is subjectively rated as being more tense. And this is how it is rated mainly by individuals who are not quite adjusted to night work. As was to be exl~~cted, mental fitness and so-called psychoproductivity (stability of attention, cancellation test, etc.) is substantially lower in such individuals than in well- adapted ones, on ttie basis of all of the foregoing. There is a correlation between level of fitness and representation of delta sleep after a decline~of emotional tension and general normalization of sleep structure (for example, as a result of a course of electrosleep treatment). In this case, fitness for work increases and autonomic changes reverr_ *_o normal. Thus, the data submitted above are indicative of the important role of structural organization of sleep in sustaining a normal level of work fitness in man, with due r.onsideration of char.acterological traits and nature of work performed by the subject. However, ii would be wrong to consider on this basis that there is a direct link between duration and quality of sleep, on the one hand, and fitness for work, on the other: the results of experimental studies do not confirm this. In one ~tudy [36], the sleep schedule was excessively changed for 10 subjects: sleep time was extended to 11 h per day (from 2100 to 0800 hours); total sleep time ~aas retained but shifted over the circadian rhythm (sleeping from 2100 to 0500 hours or from FOR OFFICI~CL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00854R004400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY 0300 to 1100 hours) and, finally, it was reduced to 5 h(from 0300 to 0800 hours). The structure of sleep on the usual schedule, sleeping from 2400 to 0800 hours, weiG also studied. Each schedule was kept for 1 week, after which the subjects slept on their usual schedule for 7 days. It was found that the precision and speed of _ performance were considerably lower, while negative affective changes were greater with all of the altered sleep schedules. The diminished work fitness was not due to either the change in total duration of sleep or structure of prior sleep. Worsening of well-being, as well as diminished fitness with increase in duration of sleep, were the unexpected results of this study. Indeed, if we ~onsider that the main function of sleep is for the optimum organiza- tion of information, elimination of information overload and elimination of non- productive emotional tension, it is incomprehensible why excessive sleep can have an adverse effect. The hypothesis has been expounded [36] that the optimum level of effective function in a waking state may depend more on the correct rhythm of the "sleeping-waking" cycle and particularly on bedtime, than on total duration and structure of sleep. The role of circadian rhythm is also stressed by the fact that certain times of - day were observed when changes in sleep schedule had a particular effect on fitness - for work and well-being. Thus, the largest number of mistakes in attention tests were referable to noon and afternoon (about 1200 and 1700 hours), but not in the morning. _ It is known, that a distinct rhythm is observed in many physiological functions, which are re~.ated in some way to f itness for work, when there is a normal sleeping and waking schedule. It is also known that physiological rhythms are found to be somewhat shifted in phase in so-called "larks" and "owls," i.e., individuals whose peak activity is referable to morning or evening hours [37]. It would be logical to assume that there is a close correlation between these physio- logical and psychological rhythms, which determine fitness for work, on the one hand, and the circadian "sleeping-waking" rhythm, on the other. At any rate, it - has been shown [38] that differences in rhythm of secretion of epinephrine, body temperature and performance of "owls" and "larks" level off or disappear entirely with 72 h of sleep deprivation. Consequently, in those cases where such differ- ences are present, they are perhaps mediated by the "slEeping-waking" circadian rhythm. However, an artificial change in this rhythm, be it longer sleep, earlier falling asleep or later awakening, could affect the dynamics of physiological and psychological functions and, consequently, fitness for work. In particular, in the case of waking up late, the subject could sleep through the time of day that is optimum for his efficiency, just like early falling asleep could disrupt the natural cycle of physiologica3. processes and reduce efficiency on the following day in an individual with peak activity in the evenings. Several studies [38] did not take into consideration the possible differences in peak of activity between subjects. Yet it is necessary to consider this when studying the effects of the sleep schedule on fitness and general well-being of people. Staying awake during a period that is biologically intended for sleep, like sleeping at a time intended for being awake (daytime sleep when working on a night shift) may not be physiologically satisfactory, even if deviations from the norm are not demonstrable with regard to objective signs, for example, sleep structure. FOR OFFICIA~ USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICtAL USE ONLY Thus, it ~_s mandatory to consider the fact that changes in sleep schedule could have an adverse effect, not only on fitness for work but health (frequency and severity of diseases) because this is associated with discoordination of many other rhythmic functions of the body. But, just as it would be a mistake to overlook this circumstance, it would be wrong to underestimate the functional role of the change in sleep structure described above. The fact remains: when there is selective deprivation referable to different phases of sleep, the overall "sleeping--waking" cycle and rhythm occasionally fail to change, while the effect of such deprivation on fitness for work is significant. In various extreme situations and in the presence of neurotic states, the sleep " schedule may not change as a whole, but its structure could vary over a certain _ range and show a correlation with changes in fitness. Thus, in the presence of neurosis, the shortage of delta sleep is related to rapid progression of asthenic symp[oms. Evidently, not all changes in the sleep schedule have the same effect on an individual's condition during the subsequent.waking period. - For this reason, the conception of the effect of altered sleep schedule on physiolo- gical and psychological functions must not compete with conceptions of the role of altered structure of sleep itself, but enlarge upon them. Such an approach to the physiological study and hygienic evaluation of work in shifts and night work could deepen significantly our understanding of the nature of changes in the entire organism that are associated with such work and enable -::s to develop with better validation the appropriate recommendations. Conclusion Thei-e is every reason to supplement the traditional studies of industrial physiology _ and hygiene with in-depth investigation of the relationship between sleep and the waking state, their reciprocal effect on one another, with the use of modern poly- graphic techniques. Firs: of all, it is int~r~sting to examine these correlations in people engaged in intensive mental labor, operators performing important control functions, workers on alternate morning, day and night shifts and those working only at night for many years. The main question is whether any adaptive changes occur and how they are manifested. Are there individual or other differences in ability to sdjust to shift work or only night work? If so, which is quite probable, is it possible to find methods of vocational screening of individuals who adjust easily and define contraindications for those who react to such a schedule with particular difficulty and morbidity, whose reliability is drastically diminished when they work under such conditions? It is also interesting to investigate the preterred duration of working on the same shift in order to cause the least discoordination of working and living schedule in relation to the natural fluctuations of physiological iunctions. What is tl~e physiological value of brief sleep, frequent naps, ~ai~~n working at night, when this is admissible with regard to working conditions, and how does this affect fitness for work? Finally, what is the link between a change in phases of sleep and coaking state, and the rhythms of many other physiological functions? FOR OFFIC[AL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/42/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~\I3ut one mu~c be fully aware of the fact of the immense difficulties involved in organiz'ng such studies under real living conditions. BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Miles, W. R. and Laslett, H. R., PSYCHOL. REV., Vol 38, 1931, p 10.~ 2. Wilkinson, R. T., QUART. J. EXPTL. PSYCHOL., Vol 12, 1960, p 36. 3. Williams, H. L., Lubin, A. and Goodnow, J. J., J. PSYCHOL., Vol 73, 1959, p 14. 4. Wilkinson, R. T., TRIANGEL, Vol. 8, No 5, 1968, p 162. 5. Jovanovic, U., "Normal Sleep in Man," Stuttgart, Hippokrates Verl., 1971. 6. Latash, L. P. and Manov, G. A., FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA, Vol 1, No 2, 1975, p 267. 7. Grieser, C., Greenberg, R. and Harrison, R. J., ABNORM. PSYCHOL., Vol 80, No 3, 1972, p 280. 8. Agnew, H. W., Webb, W. B. and Williams, R. L., EEG AND CLIN. NEUROPHYSIOL., Vol 17, 1964, ~ 68. 9. McGrath, M. and Cohen, D., PSYCHOL. BULL., Vol 85, No 1, 1978, p 24. 10. Greenberg, R. and Pearlman, Ch., THE PSYCHOANALYTIC QUART., No 3, 1975, p 392. 11. Rotenberg, V. S., in "Mater. 6-go Vsesoyuzn. s"yezda nevropatologov i psikhiatrov jProceedings of 6th All-Union Congress of Neuropathologists and Psychiatrists], Moscow, Vol l, 1975, p 261. 1'l. Arshavskiy, V. V. and Rotenberg, V.S., USP. FIZIOL. NAUK, Vol 9, No 3, 1978, p 49. L3. ~iartmann, F.., "Functiona of Sleep. The Nature of Sleep,"~Stuttgart, Eiippokrates Verl., 1975, p 238. 14. Rotenberg, V. S., in "Fiziologiya i patologiya sna cheloveka" jPhysiology and Pathology of Human Sleep], Moscow, 1975, p 69. 15. Jovanovic, U., Liebaldt, G., Muhl, U., Nippert, M., Sto~ker, G. and Stumkat, E., ARCH. PSYCHIATR. NERVENKR., Vol 214, 1971, p 183. L6. Kjellberg, A., PSYCHOLOGIC. REPORTS, Vol 37, 1975, p 479. 17. Lc:;ter, J., Knapp, T. and Roessler, R., WAKING AND SL]:EPING, Vol 1, No 2, 1976, p 61. 18. Rotenberg, V. S., Shakhnarovich, V. M., Kandror, I. S., Roytenburd, S. R., Goncharenko, A. M., Kosilina, N. M., Tyurin, M. S. and Arenalis-Garsiya, R., FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA, Vol 1, No 5, 1975, p 756. - 16 . FOR OFFIC[AL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-04850R000400050044-4 FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONLY 19. Johnson, L. C., in "Behavior and Brain Electrical Activity," 1975, p 1. 20. Johnson, L. and Macle~d, W., PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR SKILLS, Vol 36, No l, 1973, p 87. 21. Dement, W. and Greenberg, S., EEG AND CLIN. NEUROPHYSIOL., Vol 20, 1966, p 523 22. Webb, W. and Agnew, H., SCIENCE, Vol 150, 1965, p 1745. 23. Naiton, P., WAKING AND SLEEPING, Vol 1, No 2, 1976, p 53. 24. Luby, E. D., Grisell, J. L., Frohmann, C. E., Lees, H., Cohen, B. and Gottlieb, J. S.., ANN. N.Y. ACAD. SCI., Vol 96, 1962, p 71. i 25. Lewin, J. and Glaubman, H., PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Vol 12, No 3, 1975, p 349. 26. Glaubman, H., Orbach, J., Awram, P., Frieder, J., Frieman, M., Pelled, 0. and Glaubman, R., Ibid, Vol 15, 1978, p 75. 27. Goncharenko, A. M., Shakhnarovich, V. M. and Rotenberg, V. S., ZH. VYSSH. NERVN. DEYAT-STI, Vol 27, No 4, 1977, p 831. 2g. DeKoninck, J., Proul, G., Healey, T., Arsenault, R. and Prevost, F., SLEEP RESEARCH, Vol 4, 1975, p 150. 29. Studd, D., Healey, Th. and Broughton, R., in "Second International Sleep Research Congress," Edinburgh, 1975, p 59. 30. Globus, G. C., Phoebus, E. C. and Boyd, R., AEROSPACE MED., Vol 43, 1972, p 266. 31. Kripke, D. F., Cook, B. and Lewis, 0. F., PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Vol 7, No 3, 1971, p 377. 32. Bruden, G. and Goldstook, T., Ibid, Vol 10, No 1, 1973, p 36. 33. Webb, W. and Agnew, H., AVIAT. SPACE ENVIRON. MED., Vol 19, No 2, 1978, p 384. 34. Foret, J. and Benoit, 0., EEG.AND CLIN. NEUROPHYSIOL., Vol 37, 1974, p 337 35. Eiume, K. J. and Miles, Jo N., in "Second International Sleep Research Congress," Edinburgh, 1975, p 278. 36. Taub, J. M. and Berger, R. J., PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, Vol 10, No 6, 1973, p 559. 37. Kleitman, N., "Sleep and Wakefulness," Chicago, 1963. 38. Frober, J., BIOL. PSYCHOL., Vol 5, 1977, p 119. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", "Fiziologiya cheloveka", lyi~U 10,657 CSO: 8144/1482 � FOR OFFICfAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY UDC: 612.85+612.825 SENSIBILITY OF AUDITORY AND TACTILE SENSORY S~STEMS WHEN STIMULATED SEPARATELY AND TOGETHER Moscow FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA in Russian Vol 6, No 6, Nov-Dec 80 pp 1131-1133 [Article by N. Yu. Alekseyenko, Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neuro- physiology, USSR Academy of Sciences, Moscow, submitted 10 Oct 79] [Text] Studies of the significance of motor activity to spatial hearing established that proprioceptive impulsation associated with head movement affects not only per- - ception of the direction of the sound source but absolute sensibility of the auditory system jl, 2]. In this regard, the question arose as to whether this effect on hearing thresholds is specif ic to proprioception, to which the auditory system is functionally closely linked under conditions of integral human and animal behavior, or whether other sensory systems have the same effect on hearing. Pre- viously obtained data concerning the modulating effect of cutaneous sensibility on spatial hearing [3] suggest the possibility of the latter~s effect on auditory thresholds. To check this hypothesis, experiments were conducted to examine the effect of cutaneous sensibility on hearing, in which the subject was presented with audio and cutaneous stimuli of varying intensity simultaneously: threshold, supraliminal and subliminal for both modalities, in different combinations of these intensities, - and determination was made of audibility of the audio signal contained in each combination. Audio stimuli, consisting of 0.2 ms clicks, were delivered in a free acoustic field by an ESU-1 stimulator through a TD-6 electrodynamic telephone. Electro- cutaneous stimulation was delivered by means of pulses of current of the same duration from the same stimulator to the skin of the forearm through silver cup electrodes with electroconductive paste. Both stimuli were used simultaneously. Their intensity was regulated by attenuators. The subject was kept in a dark, sound-proof chamber. At the start of the test, we measured the thresholds of audio and cutaneous stimuli separately, after which they were used together at intensities of -2 to -K+ dB in relation to the estab- lished threshoids, with intervals of about 15 s between combinations. The different combinations were used in random order. Each experiment involved 70 to 150 combina- tions of stimuli with a 5-10-min rest break. After each combination, the subject was instructed to report whether he heard a sound and felt stimulation of the skin ("Sound," "Skin"). In all, we conducted 28 experiments on 9 healthy subjects 19-22 years of age. F'OR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY dB In the vast majority of these tests _Z � � � � � ' ~ - (23 out of 28) , there was distinct - 0~ ~ _ elevation of auditory sensibility under Z the influence of supraliminal (per- ceptible) cutaneous stimulation. Thus, o o~ o~~ J~. when combined with subliminal electro- ~ _ cutaneous stimuli, the clicks deliv- - p~ . � � ered at the intensities we chose were -2~- ~ ~ heard by the subjects in 38% of a total of 690 presentations, whereas in cocn- _ L binations with supraliminal cutaneous l~ � ~ � ~ � � ' ' ' ~ stimulation they were heard in 63%. p . Thus, the increase in auditory 5ensi- ' _Z~ c o n bility was distinct and reliable, although it did not occur with each = Effect of cutaneous sensibility on auditory presentation. sensibility (schematic depiction of frag- ments of three experiments) The Figure illustrates some examples Audio stimuli: black circles--audible, of such changes, where there is sche- white--inaudible; black triangles-- matic rendition of fragments of three perceptible electrocutaneous stimuli, tests with three subjects. We see white--imperceptible. that with the use of clicks of the Y-axis, intensity of stimuli as related same intensity, they were not heard to perception threshold, dB (white circles) in combinations where there was no cutaneous sensation (white triangles), whereas in the combinations where electric stimuli were percep- tible (black triangles) the same clicks were audible (black circles), with the exception of two cases. Thus, it was discovered that the capacity to affect auditory sensibility is not inherec~t specifically to proprioception, but other sensory systems, in this case the cutaneous one. It must be noted thatsome researchers [4, 5] observed elevation of the auditory threshold under the influence of cutaneous stimulation. Evidently, this is re- - lated to the fact that they specially used strong electrocutaneous stimuli that have a masking effect on signals of other modalities. As for the structural bases of the effect we observed, according to data in the ~ literature there are several possibilities. For example, stimulation of the skin of a cat's paw altered neuronal activity in its cochlear nuclei [6, 7], inferior lamina tecti mesencephali [6, 8], internal geniculate body [6], its large-cell part [9] and primary auditory cortex [10]. On the basis of evoked potentials, over- lapping of cutaneous and auditory.projections was demonstrated in the anterior ecto- sylvian gyrus [11]. These two modalities also interact in multisensory cortical regions [12, 13J. However, it must be stipulated that, unlike our psychophysical experiments, these electrophysiological data were indicative of predominantly inhibitory effects of cutaneous stimulation on hearing. Let us also mention that we demonstrated the opposite effect, and an even stronger one at that, in the course of our experiment~, i.e., the influence of audible sounds on cutaneous sensibility. Subjects perceived isolated cutaneous stimuli in 30% of ttie cases (562 deliveries without audible sounds), and in combination 19 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY with audible clicks (325 presentations) they did so in 79% of the cases. These findings conform with previously published data [14]. Conclusion A study was made of the effect of mild tactile stimulation on auditory sensibility. Stimuli of both modalities were delivered either separately or simultaneously. In the case of concurrent tactile (supraliminal) and sonic stimulation, the auditory thresholds were reliably lower than with audio stimulation alone. Thus, mild tactile excitation increased excitability of the auditory system. . ~ BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Alekseyenko, N. Yu. and Kamenkovich, V. M., FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA, Vol 3, No 2, 1977, p 324. 2. Alexeenko, N. Y. and Verderevskaya,N. Y., EXPTL. BRAIN RES., Vol 26, No 5, 1967, p 495. 3. Alekseyenko, N. Yu., in "Problemy fi.ziologicheskoy akustiki" [Problems of Physiological Acoustics], Leningrad, Vol 1, 1949, p 4. 4. Gescheider, G. A. and Niblette, R. K., J. EXPTL. PSYCHOL., Vol 74, No 3, 1967, p 313. S. Gescheider, G. A., Sager, L. G. and Ruffolo, L. J., PERCEPT. PSYCHOPHYS., Vol 18, No 3, 1975, p 209. 6. Buchwald, J. S., Holstein, S. B. and Schwefel, T., ANAT. REC., Vol 163, No 2, 1969, p 160. ~ 7. Naumova, T. S. and Bobkova, R. M., ZH. VYSSH. NERVN. DEYAT., Vol 22, No 2, 1972, p 394. 8. Aitkin, L. M., Dickhaus, H., Shult, W. and Zimmermann, M., J. NEUROPHYSIOL., Vol 41, No 4, 1978, p 837. 9. Khorevin, V. I., NEYROFIZIOLOGIYA, Vol 10, No 2, 1978, p 133. 10. Borgest, A. N., in "Materialy VII Vses. konf. po elektrofiziologii tsentral'noy nervnoy sistemy" [Proceedings of 7th Al1-Union Conference on Electrophysiology of the Central Nervous System], Kaunas, 1976, p 52. 11. Berman, A. L., J. NEUROPHYSIOL., Vol 24, No 6, 1962, p 595. 12. Bignall, K. E., EXPTL. NEUROL., Vol 18, No 1, 1967, p 56. 13. Schneider, A. S. and Davis, J. L., PHYSIOL. BEHAVIOUR, Vol 3, No 3, 1974, p 365. 14. Gescheider, G. A., Kane, M. J., Sager, L. C. and Ruffolo, L. J., BULL. PSYCHONOM. SOC., Vol 3(3A), 1974, p 204. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", "Fiziologiya cheloveka", 1980 10,657 CSO: 8144/1482 20 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY UDC: 612.821 DYNAMICS OF PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGICAL AND CARDIOVASCULAR PARAMETERS DURING OPERATOR WORK IN THE ANTICIPATION AND TRACKING MODE Moscow FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA in Russian Vol 7, No 1, Jan-Feb 81 pp 75-80 [Article by N. I. Sapova and T. A. Pavlova, Leningrad, submitted 11 Nov 79] [Text) An operator's work at a control console is related to the necessity of perceiving and processing information on an extremely tight time schedule and making appropriate decisions on this basis. During performance of operator work, a man can be subject to the influence of a number of adverse factors, which alter the operator`s functional state and efftciency [fitness for work] [1-4J. For this - reason, industrial physiologists are confronted with the task of observing the condition of operators during work in order to detect early adverse signs of changes in them and to prevent such adverse changes. Our objective was to investigate the functional changes in the human body in the course of operator work for 4 h in the mode of anticipation and tracking, with differing density of signal input per watch [work perio~] and under different ambie~it microclimate conditions. Methods A total of 10 people (men) 20 to 33 years of age, who had not been engaged pre- viously in operator work, participated in the studies. Their duties consisted of prompt detection and tracking of an illuminated dot on a television screen. The tests were conducted in a small sound-proof. chamber. The first and second tests were conducted under ordinar�r conditions (temperature 22�1�C, humidity 40-60%), the third at an air temperature of 43�1�C (with th~ =~me humidity). The density of signal delivery constituted 160 in the first test, and 16 signals per watch in the second and third. In the first test, the intervals between signals ranged from 20 s to S min, in the second and third from 30 s to SO min, with tracking time of 20 s. Prior to the tests, the subjects were trained many times in operator work at a console. The well-being of the subjects was evaluated by means of interrogation following a specific outline before and after work. We studied the direct indicators of operator efficiency--number of errors (misses) referable tc si~nals, as well as the quality of tracking (integral tracking error). Specially developed "Search" equipment was used to study the dynamics of inental efficiency of the operators. An instrument generated groups of audio signals for 21 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/02109: CIA-RDP82-00850R040400050044-4 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY several successive intervals each lasting 5 s, at a constant frequency of pulses within an interval and increasing frequency from interval to interval. The duration of the sonic pulse was constant, and it constituted 80 ms. The method of counting signals (CC) under conditions of time shortage consisted of determining the maximum signal frequency at which the subject could correctly count the to*_al number thereof per_ group. The subject was instructed in advance to keep track of the number of signals by mentally listing the sequential number of each of them. He was to call out loud the total number of signals in each group. The maximum number of pulses counted correctly by the subject served as the indi- cator [parameterJ. For the method of dividing attention (DA) under conditions of shortage of time, the subject was asked to keep track of the number of audio signals as in the preceding test. Concurrently with the audio signals, photic signals were delivered in the same intervals. The number of photic pulses per group was set at random (from 3 to 6). The duration of photic pulses constituted 80 ms. In response to each photic pulse the subject had to touch the sensory plate on the instrument with his right index finger right after appearance of the light. Per- formance of this task was rated according to maximum number of audio signals correctly counted by the subject, provided he also reacted properly to the phot~c signals. i tance of the o erative [immediate] memory (OM) method under conditions of I The subs p ~ shortage of time consisted of determining the threshold frequency of two types o~ audio signals alternating in random order, at which the subject was capable o~ correctly counting the number of both types separately. The instrument generated audio si~nals lasting 160 ms for several successive intervals lasting 10 s. In each group, the higher frequency (1000 Hz) signa]s alternated with lower frequency (400 Hz) signals. The order of delivery of high and low signals in each group was set by a transmitter of random numbers. The sub~ect was asked to count the number of high and low signals separately in each group, listing mentally the sequential number of each. The maximum number of high and low audio signals that were counted correctly in 10 s was taken as the threshold value. In the examination of the cardiovascular system, arterial pressure (AP) was measured by the Korotkov method with a Bedmonitor instrument by remote contzol. The electrocardiogram was taken in the D-S lead [5]. Phase an~lysis of cardiac - function was performed by means of concurrent registration of the EKG, sphygmo- gram of the carotid artery and seismocardiogram. The polycardiograms (PKG) were interpreted by the method described by V. L. Karpman j6]. An original design of the Periodometer instrument was used to record intervalocardiography (IKG). This instrument is based on the principle of time and amplitude conyersion of EKG R-R intervals recorded in the form of vertical lines with an automatic tracer. The stationary segments were evaluated according to mean values of R-R intervals, sinus arrythmia (SA), determined as the difference between maximum and minimum ' R-R intervals within the selected epoch of analysis (3 min), as well as analysis of amplitude of respiratory waves of cardiac rhythm (RA), which was calculated for ~ive respiratory cycles. The rheoencephalogram (REG) was recorded in the left frontomastoidal lead and the rheovasogram (RVG) of the lower extremities from the left lower leg. The rheograms were interpreted by the conventional method j7]. ~ Stroke (SV) and ~inute (MV) volumes of the heart were determined by the method ~ of integral rl~eography of the body (TBR) using the formula from the works of a ri, I. Tishchenko j8]. The aboVe-mentioned physiological processes were recorded ~ 22 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050044-4 - FOR OF'FICIAL USE ONLY with nc~~ subj~~ct in seated position. The REG, RVG, IRR and PKG were analyzed in 20 .5uc~~essive. ~:ardiac cycles. Variabi].ity was defined as the difference between maxim~~m and minimum val.ues of parameters within this epoch of analysis. 2 The orttiosr~:tic~ tc~st and Marein's test (20 squats in 30 s), with registration of _ i'r'.C 1nd A1', wi~re pc~rformed before and after the watch. Transient processes in t.}zE�:;~� *ests weY�e evposed khar the concept of quality of regulation, which refers to the rc:,~l}:~roi~al ~~f the 3rea :~f regulation, be applied in biology j9] . However, as shown ~ hv vur ~x.;~E,rience, r~~uuction of the area of regulation involves great strain for ~ t;~e bc~dy sn:i cannot be considered a positive phenomenon, whereas in engineering such a rtdu.~tior~ i.s indicative of better regulation of the system. On the basis of data submictcd i~y D. ti. Menitskiy [10], the reaction to a load characterizes excitabi- ].ity, d:iration of ttte transient process--lability, whereas overregulation is ii~d..cative uf eqiiiiibrium of nervous processes in the regulatory centers of the ar~;an ism. RE>~:ul.ts ~~l:l ot tii~ ~ubjects who participated in the tests noted that operator work is t:.rin~, duc~ tc~ tt~~ cnonotony and uniformity of the surroundings. There were com- ;11r headaci~e, sharp pain in the eyes, which were :aore marked in the third - te~t. The suhjec~ti also reported that it was difficult to sustain the working (}o:, 1 t 2.pn . titudies c~f eEficiency of oper:?tor performance revealed that the number of missed si~;n.+is per watc,h constituted 1.1-1.6 in all of the tests. The largest number of mf.5sc:-ct signals due to distraction was noted at high ambient temperature (2.9 sig- nals). "i'racking error (mo[cir camponent of work) diminished toward the end of the w~~;.cn ,.n :~I1 of Che tests (the "finish-line dash" effect). In orcler t~.~ :~,:_~se~~;z. Ir~ th ie~ rt~^~~zd, tht:~~~ was discussion of the means and algori.thms ' s~i ~:~xL-;-:�v~~ir~~; ;r.~:ivi~'~,~-tl ~1c~cr.~, as we11 as the question of the number and nature of n r~;cr;i,^~~;~z~~:, ~;~;c~c.! t~}~ cx:.~~",.~z~c.:~t ~eaplt~ to assess rhe degree of similari_ty or differ- ~ f'll( _ ~;t.'~l:ii'1: C}:. .,;:1!?14' ..~1!1?l~~tX UU~PC~S~ -:x~~.~ ~.;Ee r~~~.~;ers :!:~l iv~.~red at. the symaasium entitled "Psychophysical Evalua- ~ t;~;n~; :~~,f~ ?=.~rr~~pr~.~;~i ~:z:~~~:~ii i~.3tion" (organizzc? by H. Geisler, GDR), there was ~ v;~i i~::.~r i~~;, ~:~i t_f~r_> r,c~c~~; ~:~r ir~-d~~~th in~.esti~ation of perceptual representation of � :::�r~;~_~::~x r~~~;c..-~i ~srir,n~:? i. ~}~n~tilar~z~~ rrr rli.f.fe