JPRS ID: 9722 JAPAN REPORT

Document Type: 
Collection: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3
Release Decision: 
RIF
Original Classification: 
U
Document Page Count: 
52
Document Creation Date: 
November 1, 2016
Sequence Number: 
18
Case Number: 
Content Type: 
REPORTS
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3.pdf3.32 MB
Body: 
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 JPRS L/9968 9 September 1981 USSR Re ort p LIFE SClENCES BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES CFOUO 1 1 /81) FBIS FOREIGN BROADG~AST INFORMATION SERVICE APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/42/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400054418-3 NOTE JPRS publications contain information primarily from foreign newspapers, periodicals and books, but also from news agency transmissions and broadcasts. Materials from foreign-language sources are translated; those from Englisii-language sources are transcribed or reprinted, with the original phrasing and other characteristics retained. Headlines, editorial reports, and material enclosed in brackets are supplied by JPRS. Processing indicators such as [Text] or [Excerpt] in the first line of each item, or following the last line of a brief, indicate how the original information was processed. Where no processing indicator is given, the infor- mation was summarized or extracted. Unfamiliar names rendered phonetically or transliterated are enclosed in parentheses. Words or names preceded by a ques- tion mark and enclosed in parentheses were not clear in the original but have been supplied as appropriate in context. Other unattributed parenthetical notes within the body of an item originate with the source. Times within items are as given by source. The contents of this publication in no way represent the poli- cies, views or attitudes of the U.S. Government. COPYRIGI~T LAWS AND REGULATIONS GOVERNING OWNERSHIP OF MATERIALS REPRODUCED HEREIN REQUIRE THAT DISSEMINATION OF THIS PUBLICATION BE RESTRICTED FOR OFFICIAL USE OYLY. APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02109: CIA-RDP82-00850R400440050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY JPRS L/9968 9 September 1981 USSR REPORT LIFE $CSENCES BIOMEDICAL AND BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES (FOUO 11/81) CONTENTS ~ ~ BIONICS Ctiaracteristics of Movement hy Fem~ile Gryllus himaculatus Crickets To~tard the Source of a Calling Son~, and the Dependence o~ Orientat3on Accuracp on Signal Spectrum.....~..... 1 Iiandbook of Sensory Physiology 11 BIOTECHNOLOGY Role of Biological Factors in Formation and Development of Man.... 13 PHXSIOZOGY Effect of Artificial Gravitatior~ in Space Flig.h_~ on Retention o� Motor Habits by Albino R.ats 19 Review of Intervuz Collection 'Physiological Bases of Human Fatigue' 26 RADIATION BIOLOGY ~ Conclusion From Book on Ra3iation Biology 29 HUMAN FACTORS Simulator Systems 37 - a- [III - USSR - 27.a S&T FOUO] FnR nFFTf ie r i 1.cF, nN1.Y APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 a vn va� r a..ar~u v.aa:, v...... PSYCHOLOGY . Meeting of Scientific Council on Psychiatry of USSR Academy of _ Medical Sciences Presidium 41 Book on Higher Neuron Activitty Republished 45 Socialist Countries Aold Youth Conditioned Reflex Activitq Seminar 47 - b FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONLY BIONICS QDC 591.185.5:595.729 CHARACTERISTICS OF MOVEMENTS BY FEMALE GT+yZZt48 bimaeuZatus CRICKETS TOWARD THE � - SOURCE OF A CALLING SONG, ?,ND THE DEPENDENCE OF ORIENTATION ACCUR~CY ON SIGNAL SPECTRUM Leningrad ZHURNAI, EVOLYUTSIOT~I1~i0Y BIOIQ~iIMII I FIZIOLOGII in Russian Vol T7, No 1, Jan-Feb 81 (manuscript received 23 May 80) pp 25-32 [Article by J. Rheinlaender, V. F. Shuvalov, A. V. Popov and K. Kalmrin~, Institute of Evolutional Physiolo~y and Biochemistry imeni I. M. Sechenov, USSR Academy of Sciences, Leningrad, and Ruhr University, Bochum (FRG)] [Text] The orientational behavior of female crickets in relation to the source of male acoustic calling songs was studied in a round arena with a diameter of 140 cm, located in a sound~roof chamber. The nature of movements made by females depends strongly on their candition, and it may be irregular or continuous. In both cases the females periodically deviated from their course as they moved. These deviations are limited to a�50� sector, they occur irregularly, and they are corrected by turning motions. _ Correcting turns are made both after halts and during movement, such that females could determine the position of the sound source - during movement as well. The greater the error angle, the greater the probability of turning in the direction of the source. The orientation error at the edge of the arena (within 30 cm of the emitter) does not exceed an average of several degrees. Experiments with simulated calling songs differing in the content of high-frequency harmonics showed that presence of high-frequency components in the signal (10 and 15 kHz) does not improve orientation accuraay, and it does not generally hasten phon.otaxis. The characteristics of phonotaxis are defined com- pletely by the low-frequency component, 5 kHz. The results are compared with the properties of low-frequency ascending auditory neurons that transmit information to the brain. The orientation accuracy discovered in females may be the result of evaluation of the difference in the activity of symmetrical pairs of these - neurons. 1 FOR OFFICIAL USE OIVLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 ruK vrr~~.~AL u~r. ~ivL~ Introduction _ Female crickets find males of their species by orienting on the acoustic calling signals emitted by the latter (1-7). Research on crickets of two species, one of which uses a calling song consisting of rarely repeating bursts (Scap82pedu8 mczryinatus),while the signal of the other is similar in structure to a trill (TeZeogzy ZZus oeeanicus), showed that movement of females to the source is obviously discontinuous--that is, it consists of alternating short, usually straight runs, and halts lasting 250-400 msec. It is only during halts that females determine the position of the source, make correcting turns, and then begin their runs. During their movement, females da not perceive information on the position of the source, and they cannot purposefully correct their course. Before starting movement toward the source, females make scanning movements from side to side. Thus the orienta- tion system of females may be described in general as a system with an open feedback _ (4,6). The orientation accuracy of S. marginatus females is astoundingly low. They can determine only the side on which the emitter is located, but they cannot detezinine the necessary turning angle. T. oeean2eus females can determine the required caurse direction angle rather accurately when the source is located in the forward sector from 0 to 30-40�; however, outside this sector the turning angles are weakly corre- lated with the error angle. In this case the orientation accuracy does not improve as sound intensity increases. However, there are grounds for hypothesizing that the described orientation mechanisms are not universal to all crickets, the signals of which may possess different properties. No one has examined ttie dependence of the accuracy and effectiveness of female orientation on the spectral composition of the signal. But at the same time we know that cricket calling songs have a harmonic spectrum consisting of several components (8,9), and that there are several frequency channels in their auditoxy system which may evaluate these components relatively independently (10-13). We posed the following objective in our work: 1) to study the nature of orientation by female GryZZtts b2maeuZatus De Geer crickets, the signal of which consists of frequently repea~ing bursts; 2) to evaluate the influence of different spectral components of the signal on orientation accuracy; 3) to compare the available neurophysiological data with the characteristics of orientational behavior. Materials and Methods The experiments were performed with G. bimac�uZatus females from a laboratory popula- tion in the last larval instar phase. Each female was placed in an individual cage and raised until sexual maturity and arisal of well-pronounced positive phonotaxis toward the male calling song. The selection *_nethod was described earlier (4). The experiments were performed in a round plywood arena with a diam~ter of 140 cm, located in a soundproof 5x5x5 meter echoless chamber at 20�1�C. ~ao loudspeakers (MSD-100, Japan) were located 30 cm from the edge of the arena 120� apart (Figure 1). A cage into which the female was placed before the start of the experiment was located in t~e center of the arena. Thirty seconds after the female was placed in the cage, an acoustic signal began to be reproduced through one of the emi.tters. 2 - FOR OFF'ICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ~ONLY The female's movements were recorded by amovie camera mounted above ~he arena. ~he ~ filming rate was two frames per second. The location of the.female and the orienta- tion of its body relative to the cage-emitter line were measured on each.frame. Drawing the trajectory, we used arrows to designa'ce�the orientation of th~ long axis of the female's body at each moment in time. The effectiveness of feanale orientation was assessed on the basis of the following indicators: Tp--reaction , time from the moment of emergence from the box (cage) to the moment the female reached the edge of the arena; ~8--angle of deviation from the line extending from the cage to the emitter at the edge of the arena; -maximum angle of deviation from the course while en route from the cage to the edge of the arena (the first 20 cm of the route from the cage were not counted, because some fem~les wandered randomly prior to the start of purposeful motion toward the emitter); a~--maximum error angle between the long ay;~ of the body and the bearing to the emitter throughout the entire time o~ travel. ~ C F ' ~ 7oO _ R ~ ; 920 ~ _ ` JO ~ 0 N Figure 1. Experimental Arena and an Example of the Trajectory of a Female, With the Measured Angles Labeled: Arrows indicate the trajectory (the bearing of the arrow corresponds to the orientation of the long axis of the female's body at each moment in time); H--emi.tter, C--c3ge. Linear dimensions are given in centimeters. See the text for further explanation. = In the greatPr part of the experiments the intensity of the signal at the cage was _ 70 db relative to 0.00002 N/m~. It increased to 82 db at the edge of the arena, which corresponds to the intensity of a song produced by a male in natural condi- _ tions from a distance of 0.2-1.0 meters (9). In two series of experiments the signal intensity was increased by 16 db. In this case the intensity of the signal at the edge of the arena attained a level corresponding to that of a signal heard several centimeters away from a singing male. r The signals consisted of computer-synthesized sounds with an envelope corresponding to the envelope of the male's calling song at the same temperature; the intensity of harmonic components was arbitrari.ly varied. The spectrum of the calling song contains three harnonic components--4-5, 9-10, and about 15 kHz (Figure 2). At a signal intensity of 25-40 db the level of the second and third harmonics is below the level of the main camponent. ~ 3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 (1) Q6 40 . 20 � ~ ~ 0.2c~3) - p 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 K~u(~C~ ~ ~ 6 , Figure 2. Calling Signal of a Male Crieket (a) and the Acoustic Model (b) Used in the Experiments: A spectral cross section of a calling song pulse is shown on the graph Key: ~ 1, ~ 3. Sec 2. kHz Inasmuch as the high frequency components are often below the noise level in the natural conditions of biotopes experiencing a hiqher noise level (9), one of the models contained only the 5 kHz component (model 5/0--the digit following the slash.line indicates attenuation in decibels)� To reveal the role of high fre- quency components, their leve~. was increased in two other models. The second model contains the S.and 15 kHz (5/0 and 15/-10) while the third contains all three _ components (5/0, 10/-20, and 15/-10). The acc~ustic models were recorded and played back with a Nagra IV-s tape recorder. Thirty females participated in the experiments. Each female was tested once a day for 10-14 days.` Results The reaction of females to the test models did not differ in general characteristics fr~m the reaction to a natural calling song. Orientation of females in relation to the sound source may be divided into three phases: 1) activation phase, 2) initial o.rientation phase, and 3) phonotaxis phase. The activation phase begins the moment - the sound is turned on, and it continues until the moment the female exits from the box. Its duration varies within broad limits, from several seconds to dozens of seconds, apparently due to differences in motivation of the females. After leaving the cage, in the ovestivhelming majority of cases the females halted at the bottom of the chute for several seconds and then.turned in the direction of the emitter (the initial orientation phase). In this case we never noted "sca.nning - movements" prior to the start of the purposeful run, as described for Seapsipedus _ marginatus (4) meaninq that for G. bimacuZatus they are not a mandatory component of orientation. The initial orientation phase.wa~ vezy short for some females, which headed for the source almost immediately, while individual specimens wandere~ randomly around the cage on leaving it, resolutely heading toward the emitter only after this. 4 FOR OFF[CIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02109: CIA-RDP82-00850R400440050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY The phonotaxis phase, or the phase of purposeful movement toward the soyarce n~ the cal.ling song, proceeds differently for different females. Young, timi~i f-~m~-ales, especially when in the arena for the first time, travel the distance i~? short, often straiqht runs alternating with halts. After the sound is turned cn~f~, the nature of the movements does not change, meaning that the halts were x~~t n~cessarily elicited by a need for course correction. As a rule the direction of mo~e~ent changed somewhat after a halt even when the f~male was already facin'rg th~e emitter. These turns far from always occur in the direction of the emitter, and ~onsequently they are not necessarily correcting turns, bei~g mor.e likely a reflectian of some sort of perturbations in the movement control system. The larger tlae e~ror angle at the time of a halt, the greater is the probal~i~.ity of turning in, the direction ~ of the emitter. Calmer, more-strongly motivated females move signif.icantly more confidently, with fewer halts or without any at a11; in this case their rate of movemerit gradually increases as they come closer to the emitter. Runs are not always s~rictly linear. - On the contraxy the female is frequently seen to periodically deviate from side to - side in relation to the true course by an insignificaa~t angle. Such oscillations about the true course are limited to a�30-50� sector (see the va'lues for angle a~ in the table), while their amplitude is insignificant, inasmu.~c3n as the turns in different directions alternate with one another quickly. Clwing to these the oscillations do not have a significant effect oii the fors~~rd tra~j'ectory as a whole, appearing instead as oscillations in the orientation of tYse b~dy relative to the principal direction of movement. Course correction occurs in these cases while on the move or after a halt, meaning that halts are not manda't~ry to a female's determination of the locai:ion of the sound source. Evidence of this can also be found in data from experiments in which the sound was produced first from one emitter and then ~during the time of the female's movement to~ard it) through the other located opposite the former. In these cases at leasit some of the females ~ turned quickly 180�, without halting their movement or with a few halts. These data also indicate that females confidently distinguish sounds c~rning from the rear from sounds coming from the front. The trajectory of feniales toward the sound source varied within broad limi.ts (Figure 3a). In most cases females headed toward tl~e sou~ce w~th sufficient accuracy from the very beginning, wit3.iout noL-iceable deviations to the s~ides (Figure 3b). Some females occasionally deviated significantly from the course to the emitter, later correcting their movement by means of one or several turns (Figure 3e). Certain females ran relatively straight, though at a certain angle to the source, and then corrected their course at the ~exy edge ot t.~e arena (Figure 3d). The possibility is not excluded that pathologica~. alterations were present in at least some of these insects, leading to interaural asxmmetry. On the whole the orientation accuracy of females, evaluated oa~ the basis of their location at the end of the route (beside the.edqe of the arena), was sufficiently high. The averages for angle ~p do not usually exceed 6-7�. When the emitter was brought close to *he edge of the arena, a11 females approached it,, correcting small deviations in the final leg of the r~ute. Statistical treatment of the obta.ined material (see table and Fic1ure 4) showed that presence of high-fre~uency compone~~ts in the signal does not significantly improve orientation accuracy, and it does not accelerate phonotaxis. The contrazy is more 5 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2447/02/09: CIA-RDP82-44850R444444454418-3 run w�raa..~na, voa: v~~a..a Effectiveness of Orientation by Female Crickets During Movement Toward the Source uf a Calling Song M~del T(sec) ~m~~ a~ N,/sec� � r~ 5/0 20.65�13.33 6.86�6.07 25.28�18.44 44.61�12.41 5.35�3.66 44 5/0 15.-10 16.21�7.38 T0.36�7.36 27.76�20.92 54.31�18.15 4.49�1.05 39 5/0 10/-20 15/-10 26.6t23.8 6.42t7.46 22.22�17.73 42.02�15.85 4.84�1.27 53 5/0+16 ~ 15.72�5.48 6.0�5.36 15.33�9.41 39.00�11.0 5.17�0.78 14 5/0 15/-10+16 db 38.25�15.73 33.00�25.03 47.75�25.16 86.67�40.1 2.52�1.56 5 Note: The values given for each variable are x�6; N--arbitrarily selected measure of the effectiveness of phonotaxis, which is a function of orienta- tion accuracy (~p) and running time (Tp). N= ln 1/Tp��p. The larger N is, the more effective is phonotaxis; n--number of ineasurements. See text for further explanations. a v d ~ c 6 0 ,-.~..-f 4 1 ~ ~ % ~ . . ~ ~ 1 ~ . . f ~ ! . ~ , . . ~ ~ ~ ~ Figure 3. Trajectoxy of Females During Phonotaxis: see text for explanation 6 FOR OFFIC[AL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY o c N e H !5 ~SPo~=b.42*7.46 ~s ~~001=6.86!6.01~ I ~ ~v ~ ~o ~ s s 0 f0 20 90 40 50 0 ~0 20 30 I~a l� N ' 6 15 ~ amQ=I = 42.02: 15 . 85' ~ ~ a~~~ ~44.61; 72 .4!' tp !0 . ~ S S 0 20 40 60 60 f00 ~ a��~� 0 20 40 6a 8Y) ~00 ~rs~~~� Figure 4. Distribution of Angles ~p and a in th~ ~our.s~~af Phonotaxis Toward Models 5/0, 10/-20, 15/-10 (a,~v) a~d 5~/0 (e,d) : a--n = 53, D~p = 2�; b--n = 50, ~am~ =10� j e-�-rc = 44, p= 2�; d--n = 42, ~am~ =10 N--numbex~ ca f runs likely true. Angle a is significantly greater with m~odel 5/0,15/-10 than with - the other models. The differences in the rest of ~t'~e parameters are insignificant. The accuracy of orientation on model 5/0 does not Eliffer from that of orientation on a natural signal. Raising signal intcnsity by 16 db significantly increased the effeativenesS of phonotaxis toward model 5/0+16 db and, on the other hand, significantly decreased-~ ~ the accuracy of orientation c+n the m4de1 containing the high-frequency component (5/0 and 15/-10+16 db). In the latter case the females began rushing from side to side, as if searching for the source. This was accompanied by an increase in all orientation indicators (Tp, ~p, ~~X, a; see table). Discussion The obtained data shaw that the orientation behavior of G. bimaeuZatus females differs dramatically from behavior described in the literature for S. marginatus ~ and T. oceanicus (4,6) in having the capability for evaluating the position of the . source and correcting course n~t only during halts but also while moving, and by absence of scanning movements before the start of phonotaxis. Wendlar et al. (7) recently came to similar conclusions in research on the orientation behavior of the field cricket, GryZZus eampestris, the calling song of which is very similar in temporal organization to the signal produced by G. bim~zculatus (8,9)� The oscillatory deviations of the long body axis and the trajectory from the direction 7 FUR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000404050018-3 rvn vra�a~.~hi, a~oc vi~L.i to the sound source may be the result of random shifts or noises in the systein con- trolling the movement of right and left legs. The randomness of these deviations - and their significant individual variability argue in favor of such an interpretation. Yn any case, inasmuch as these deviations were altogether absent from many femalee, ~ it may be hypothesized that oscillations are not a mandatory component of the guidance mechanism. Course correction compensating for random deviations of the long axis of the body from the direction to the target is apparently achie~ed by feec~ack signals. These signals may be dependent on the error angles. Obviously, the greater the error angle, the larger is the probability of a correcting instruction and its in- tensity; this probability approaches 1 as the error angle reaches its critical value. Our data showed that the maximum error angle for phonotaxis does not usually exceed 30-50�. Within the �30-50� sector, the intensity of the correcting signal may be commensurate with the level of noises within the system, and this may explain the randomness and unpredictability of "minor" turns along the course to the goal. Deflection of the long axis of the body from the direction to the emitter is accompanied by appearance of asymmetry in stimulation of the right and left auditory channels. Information on sound in different frequency bands is processed separately in the auditory system of crickets, beginning at the peripheral level and ending in the highest associative center in. the protocerebrum. The calling signal is perceived by G. bimacuZatus by means of receptors narrowly tuned to a frequency of 5 kHz and by second-order ascending neurons (LF-neurons), which transmit information on the signal _ to the protocerebrum and apparently play the decisive role in control of pkionotaxis. . The functional properties of these elements have been described earlier (11-14). There is one LF-neuron on each side, and the relationship between the discharges contains information on the position of the sound source in s~ace. Each of the LF-neurons is maximally sensitive to sound coming from tY~e ipsilateral side (Figure 5). The difference in their discharges is obviously a function of the error angle. We can see from Figure 6 that the difference in th s number of impulses produced in re- sponse to a calling song burst grows dramatically as the source shifts from foxward position (0�) up to 80-90� to either side. At the critical error angle for phono- taxis (50�), tl~e difference in the number of impulses contained in a discharge by these neurons in response to a calling song burst is 15-20 at average and high intensity. This ccrresponds to a difference in stimulation intensity on the order of 18-25 db, which greatly exceeds the differential intensity threshold of hearing which, judging from behavioral experiments, is about 2-3 db for G. bimacuZatus (157. A difference in the number of impulses on the order of 2-4 per burst, which is what arises at error angles on the order of 3-7�, corresponds to the differential intensity _ threshold. This amount is well consistent with angle ~p (about 6�). At larger deviation angles a feedback signal should arise, the intensity of which is proportional to an error angle within up to 80-90� (if its intensity is governec~ by the difference in stimulation of LF-neurons). _ LF-neurons react almost identically to saund from the front and from the back, and consequently there should be additional elements in the auditory system of crickets having difFerential sensitivity toward the back and toward the front. Similar elements have been discovered in locusts only in the protocerebrum (16). The acuity of their directional sensitivity grows as sound intensity increases. If crickets also have such elements, then our data indicating growth in orientation accuracy with increasing sound intensity receive a fully natur.al explanation. 8 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY n D' ~l~ 32 r~y /I NyMeupoH ~r'' ~ lTHY�HeupoM ~ ~ Z4 ~ - - ~ i � - /-10db~~~ 16 ~ -lOd6 Od6 I ~ ~ ~ . Od6 / i 1 8 ~ ~ (3) / ~ ) ~ 0 270 ~ 90 ~ \ \ ~ ~ \ ` ` _ i s ~ _ ~ ~ ~ ' ~ e ~ _ \ / 1~~ _ _ / ' 180� . Figure 5. Characteristics of the Directional Sensitivity of a Symmetrical Pair of LF-Neurons of the Cri~ket's Ventral Nerve Chain in Response to the Action of a Calling Song Containing Bursts of Five Pulses: n--number of impulses contained in neuron responses to a calling song burst, recorded at the level of the protocerebrum from axons of LF-neurons (from experiments conducted in Bochum, the basic results of which were published earlier (11)). An intensity of 0 db corresponds to a signal intensity of 95 db relative to 0.00002 N/m2. Sc.e text for further explanation. Key: 1. Left LF-neuron 3, cm 2. Right LF-neuran dn 30 ~ - ~O,q 6 . ~ 20 ~ ~ . C1~ . C ~ 90 ' O,q6 ~ 0 ~ ~ . , ~ ~ , . 90 ~ / . C ~ v . C ZO ~ ~ ` ~ i . ~ J L 1 I ~ 1~I ~ I I ~ I I I 180� 270� 0� 90� 180~ Figure 6. Dependence of the Difference in Responses (~yy) by the Same Pair of LF-Neurons on the Er:.or Angle, Given the Same Signal Intensity as in Figure 5(Evaluated on the Basis of the Number of Impulses in Response to a Calling Song Burst): �~ee text for further explanation Key: 1. db 9 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 . �'V~\ Vl'l ~~.IAV v?V VL\1.~� Experiments with models having different spectral compositions showed that the characteristics of phonotaxis depend entirely on the first low-frequency maximum of the signal (5 kHz). High-frequency components influence neither the effectiveness of orientation nor the rate of phonotaxis, even if their level is significantly higher than in the natural signal. Sharp disturbance of orientation in the presence of a high-intensity signal with high-frequency components may be a reflection of a transi- tion by the female from phonotaxis to a search for the male. In natural conditions the high-frequency components achieve their highest level only in direct proximity to a singing male. The cricket orientation system may be of interest from the standpoint of bionics as an example of a guidance system used to assist movement. BIBLIOGRAPHY ~ � 1. Regen, I., PFLUGERS ARCH., Vol 155, 1913, p 193. 2. Regan, I., S.B. AKAD. WISS. WIEN., Vol 132, 1923, p 81. 3. Walker, T. J., ANN. ENTOMOL. SOC. AMER., Vol 50, 1957, p 626. - 4. Murphey, R. K. and Zaretsky, M. D., J. EXP. BIOL., Vol 56, 1972, p 335. 5. Popov, A. V. and Shuvalov, V. F., J. CON~. PHYSIOL., Vol 119, 1977, p 111. . 6. Bailey, W. J. and Thompson, P., J. EXP. BIOL., Vol 67, 1977, p 61. 7. Wendler, G., Dambach, M., Schmitz, B. and Scharstein, H., NATURWISS, Vol 67, 1980, p 99. , 8. Popov, A. V., ENTOMOL. OBOZR., Vol 51, 1972, p 17. 9. Popov, A. V., Shuvalov, V. F., Knyazev, A. N. and Klar-Spasovskaya, N. A., ENTOMOL. OBOZR., Vol 53, 1974, p 258. 10. Popov, A. V., ZH. EVOL. BIOICHIM. I FIZIOL., Vol 9, 1973, p 265. 11. Rheinlaender, J., Kalmring, K., Popov, A. V. and Rehbein, H. G., J. COI~. PHYSIOL., Vol 110, 1976, p 251. 12. Elsner, N. and Popov, A. V., ADV. INSECT PHYSIOL., Vol 13, 1978, p 229. 13. Popov, A. V., "The Insect Acot~stic Communication System," Dissertation Abstract, Leningrad, 1979. 14. Shuvalov, V. F. and Popc~v, A. V., ZH. EVOL. BIOKHIM. I FIZIOL., Vol 9, 1973, p 177. - 15. Shuvalov, V. F. and Popov, A. V., DOKL. AN SSSR, Vol 246, 1979, p 247. 16. Adam, L. J., ZOOL. JB. PHYSIOL. [sic] Vol 81, 1977, p 250. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka" "Zhurnal evolyutsionnoy biokhimii i fiziologii", 1981 11004 CSO: 1840/254 10 - FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY HANDBOOK 0~' SENSORY PHXSIOLOGY ~ Leningrad ZHUTINAL EVOLYUTSIONNOi~ BIOKHIMII I FI.ZIOLOGII in Russian Vol 17, No 1, Jan-Feb 81 pp 103-104 ~ , [Review by F. Gribakin of book "Rukovodstvo po fiziologii organov chuvstv" [Handbook of Sensory Physiol~gy], Volume VII/6A, "Vision in Invertebrates", P~rt A: "Inverte- brate Photoreceptors", edited by Prof Kh. Autrum, Springer-Verlag, Berlin-Heidelberg- New York, 1979, 729 pages, 344 figures] ~ � [Text] This book is the first part (A) of the next volume in a handbook of 5ensory physiology known the world over, containing basic information on the general physio- logical mechanisms of invertebrate photoreception. Special emphasis is placed on the fact, sometimes unfor�:unately disregarded, that invertebrate photoreceptors not only serve as models for studying the basi,c photoreception processes, but also owing to numerous special adaptations they have significant value to expanding our knowledge of ecology and evolution. This is reflected in the full title of the volume--"Compara- tive Physiology and Evoluticn of Invertebrate Vision". All 12 chapters are written by top-class specialists. Readers of our journal would be interested to learn that the "Introduction" (Chapter 1, Prof IQz. Autrum) discusses, in particular, Ya. A. Vinnikov's idea that microvillous receptors are phylogenetically older than ciliary receptors; of course the editor of the volume himself feels that this supposition must be supported by further research. Chapter 2(B. D'yen) is devoted to the physiological mechanisms of photoreception in the Protista. Chapter 3 (U. Miller) exami.nes the optic properties of the eye's filtering media (aromatic amino acids, shielding pigments, and so on). Chapter 4, which is devoted to the physiology of invertebrate vi.sual pigments, is extr.emely interesting. This chapter was written - by Prof K. Khamdorf, who discovered that photoregeneration is the principal means of regeneration of rhodopsin in cephalopod mollusks and arthropods, while rhodopsin in vertebrates is regenerated by enzymatic mechanisms. Chapter 5, "The Physics of Vision in the Compound Eye", contains all of the basic information on the optics of photo- reception by the invertebrate eye. The author of this chapter, prFSently a professor of applied mathematics at the University of Australia, A. U. Snayder, is essentially the "father" of photoreceptor optics, which grew up into an independent branch of knowledge in the last 15 years. Chapter 6, "The Receptor Potential of Invertebrate - Visual Cells" (M. Yarvilekhto), contains contemporary data on the regeneration mechanisms of ear~:y and late receptor po~ential in arthropod photoreceptors. It goes on to examine the physiological mechanisms behind arisal of a false pupil, or a pseudopupil, in the compound eye. A purely optical phenomenon, the pseudopupil is 11 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 usually seen as a dark spot formed out of omanatidia oriented strai~ht at the observer, and consequently not reflecting and not scattering ~ight in this direction; the possibilities for capitalizing on this phenomenon as a valuable tool for studying the optic qualities of the ele and the properties of its photoreceptors and pigment cells are demonstrated (Chapter 7, D. Stavenga). Modern ideas are presented on the superposition and apposition of compound eyes (P. Kuntse, Chapter 8); the first subdivisions of the chapter rather extensively explain the classical viewpoints of Johannes Muller and Sigmund Ecksner. Chapter 9, "Spectral Sensitivity and Color Vision of Invertebrates" (R. Mentsel')r i.s a most extensive review summar- izing, in one place, all that is known today about the spectral sensitivity of the photoreceptors of invertebrates, beginning with the Protista, and about the central mechanisms af color information processing, to include the behavioral aspects of the problem. Chapters 10 (M. Ioshida) and 11 (Miriazn Bennett) are devoted to extraocular photoreception and its role in triggerinq and maintaining diurnal rhythms. Finally Chapter 12, "A Genetic Approach to the Visual System" (M. Geyzenberg), deals basically with research on vision of a classical object in genetics, mainly various eye mutants of DrosophiZa, since in the author's opinion only research on DrosophiZa fits within the framework of the term "genetic approach", even though the genetic literature on vision Presentlyincludes organisms from halobacteria to the white tiger. Evaluating the content of the volume as a whole, it should be stated that it is a superior source of contemporary data having fundamental significance today. Never- theless a number of problems still rema.in outside the field of vision of the author collective (the ion mechanisms of generation of receptor potential, the genetic approach to studying ,.vision in the honeybee, analysis of the electric field of a compound eye, and so on). We would hope that these aspects of the physiology of invertebrate vision would be reflected in the second part of Volume VII/6. which is now being prepared for publication. Unfortunately, the iridex of cited works by Soviet authors is not very large: As a rule only those works which are published in English in international journals are cited, which once again emphasizes the need for publishing in fo~eign sources. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka" "Zhurnal evolyutsionnoy biokhimii i fiziologii", 1981 11004 CSO: 1840/254 12 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY BIOTECI~TOLOGY ROLE OF BIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN FORMATiON AND DEVELOPMENT OF MAN Mosc~w VESTNIK AKADEMII NAUK SSSR in Russian No 6, Jun 81 pp 42-47 [Text of statements made by Academician A. D. Aleksandrov and Academician N. P. Dubinin] [Text] As was already reported on the pages of VESTNIK AN SSSR [Vestnik of the USSR Academy of Sciences] (No 4, 1981, p 107), there was a session of the General Meeting of the USSR Academy of Sciences on 21 November 1980. At this session, Academician A. D. Aleksandrov spoke on the role of biological factors in the formation and development of man. Academician A. P. Aleksandrov, - president of the USSR Academy of Sciences, then gave the floor to Academician N. P. Dubinin to respond to his points. In accordance with the r~quest of the speakers, the text of their speeches is published below. Academician A. D. Aleksandrov - Man is, to use the terms of philosophy, the carrier of two forms of movement of matter, "biological" and "social." But it is expressly the latter that distinguishes man. Within the same biological species and even the same race, people demonstrate an immense diversity of forms of a.ctivity, which they create and transmit to one another and to subsequent generations by means of direct and indirect communication. Since the brain is the source of the programs of these forms of endeavor, we can refer to the cerebral evolution of man in contrast to genetic evolution. The enormous area of what is specifically human, human history, develops, of course, - on the basis of genetically determined human distinctions and, first of all, the _ distinctions of the human brain. There, the same chief law common to all living organisms is in effect, according to which genes ~'-etermine the possibility of development of traits of an organism, but this possibility is realized depending on conditions. However, ~specially man genetically represents merely a possibility ~ that changes into reality through interaction between the genotype and conditions, ~ mainly social ones. These conditions themselves are created and changed by the activities of people. For this reason, it can be stated that man (in the collective ' sense~ cxeated a~d continues to create himself on *_he basis of his specific genotype. ~ TYius, three factors affect the formation of man: genotype, living conditions and tY~e t~ansfo~zning, creative endeavors of man himself The extretnely complex *'~Creati;ve endeavor" is used to construe any activity that is not performed accord- ing to a pxeset program. ~ 13 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 _ correlation between them constitutes a fundamental and difficult problem about which the most varied v~iews are expressed. Representatives of different special fields often exaggerate expressly what is closer to them, disregard{ng what is "alien" to them. Geneticists tend to underestimdte the social factor, while sociologists do so for the genetic factor, as well as the creative fdctor in ~ some cases, interpreting, for example, the personality as the passive product of existing social conditions. ~ ~ The colossal extent, to which geneticists sometimes disregard the social factor, is indicated, for example, by an article by the well-known American geneticist, re:cipient of the Nobel prize, J. Lede~berg, which was published at the time in LITERATURNAYA GAZETA [Literary Gazette] (No 20, 1969). There, among others, the following question was discussed: "Do the poor have proper [perfect] genes?" The answer being: "Perhaps genes do play some part in man's economic failures. For this reason, the excessive proliferation of the stratum of the poor could be eugenically harmful. But birth control among the poor will not yield results very soon.:.. Of course, it would be easiest to suggest another solution: let not - the poor have any children at all. But if we were to take this route, we could go even farther: fight for eradication of poverty by means of physical extermination of all the poor. Although, perhaps, this would be ~nore humane than to doom them - to a hungry death." This opinion is extremely instructive in many respects, and it should be discussed in detail, but unfortunately, we do not have space here to do it. We shall merely call attention to the blindness of this authoritative scientist who does not see _ that poverty is a social phenomenon and that for this reason it is simply absurd to discuss "eradication of poverty by means of physical extermination of all the poor" (incidentally, ~et us recall that Schubert and Van Gogh, for example, were paupers and, one would think, their poverty was related expressly to their genius, which compelled them to direct their efforts to an activity that yielded no economic success). There is also a very obvious ethical aspect to such ~udgments. An important dis- tinction of all sciences dealing with man, be it psychology or human genetics, is that it applies, in principle, to everyone, not excluding the researcher himself, and for this reason, in the sciences dealing with man the scientific position is particularly closely related to the ethical [moral] position. Of course, this does not mean that science must subordinate its objectivity to ethical goals. Marx wrote: a man who strives to adapt science to such a point of view that is not gleaned from science itself (:no mazfe~ Rqw ~quch t~e la~,tex ~a~ in exro~'~ but from the outside, toward a point of view that ts dictated by i~te~+e~fis that are alien to science, endogenous in relation ta it�-~T ca~,7, such a-man R~i~~erio~' But one must clearly understand the human meaning of what is said and done in the science of man and there must be a warning signal in front of everyone who talks about man, parttcul,a�r~y tt~ose who do so on behal~ o~ science; "Be c~ze~ult people! Be ob~ective to the utmost!" This app~ies in particular tp statetqen~s made in the broad press. *K. Marx and F. Engels, "Soch." [Works], Vol 26, Pt 2, p 125. 14 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02109: CIA-RDP82-00850R400440050018-3 - FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY But here we read the article of Academician N. P. Dubinin, "Inheritance of Biologi- cal and Social Factors,"* where this warning is not heeded at all. N. B. Dubinin wishes to develop the idea of Marx concerning the social essence of man,and he criticizes geneticists who exaggerate the role of the biological factor in man to the detriment of the social one. But he himself goes to an utter extreme. His own idea is that it is absurd to search for an explanation of normal mental phenomena in any biological functions. He decisively denies that biological, genetic heredity has any influence on man's normal psyche and, in particular, writes: "Vn2gar b fologizational interpretation of inentai functions as genetically _ determined traits constitutes idealism in comprehension of the problem of man. Authors who relate the human psyche directly to genes, attributing the mental properties of man to physiological functions of neurons of the brain, vi~w themselves as materialists of the highest order. However, the human psyche cannot be found in ei.ther the function of genes or the function of neurons, for the simple reason that its level has gone beyond the boundaries of biology. To search for an explanation of inental phenomena in any biological functions is tantamount to attributing spiritual properties of all matter" (p 72). But we know, for example, that sex is genetically determined, that the psyche of normal men and women is not the same, and that such mental differences are attri- butable, at least in part, to biological functions. ThnsL_the main thesis of the article, and one stated so categorically, is an overt contradiction to obvious facts. - The real problem is to examine which mental features, how and to what extent they depend on heredity or social conditions. But N. P. Dubinin closes this issue for normal people, leaving it to medical genetics only with regard to abnormal people. However, it is beneficial to note that the very concepts of normal genotype, normal brain, normal psyche, that Academician N. P. Dubinin uses with such confidence, are not completely definite and imply demonstration of various aspects and gradations of normalcy and abnormality, which depend on the same genetic heredity. For this reason, the desire of Academician N. P. Dubinin to limit the influence of genes - only to an "abnormal" psyche is invalid. Further, we see from the statements quoted above that Academician N. P. Dubinin decisively denies a direct relationship of the psyche to neuronal functions in the brain. But, for example, the well-known effect of alcohol on the mind is - apparently related to its effect on neuronal function in the brain, so that here too there is no validity to the idea of Academician N. P. Dubinin. Indeed, the problem consists of investigating physiological, biochemical, physical structures and mechanisms that determine mental phenomena. N. B. Dubinin "closes" this problem also. Let us recall, however, higher nervous activity, the types thereof established by I. P. Pavlov. He made the direct statement that certain .temperaments correspond to these types, and that they are determined by the genotype. So that Pavlov - See: KOMMUNIST, No 11, 1980. 15 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 maintains there is a direci. relationship of the psyche to the genotype and brain neuron functions and, thus, is among the authors who "directly relate the psyche to genes. Let us assume that Pavlov was m~.staken, but then he should be refuted with scientific arguments, and not disposed of in such a manner. Incidentally, it would appear that Pavlov was not mistaken here.... The problems that are directly related to man require utmost objectivity of judg- ments, utmost caution of conclusions and utmost attention in research. Questions of heredity, congenital or acquired traits, abilities or defects, questions of man's capabilities--they all emerge acutely in life, they affect living people, sometimes they create difficult situations and they merit the most serious dis- , cussion. This applies fully to the problem of abilities. A socialist society is based on the principle of "from each according to his abilities," and for this reason the question of abilities is of basic importance to us, not only theoretically but practically, both on the level of society as a . whole and of each man: how to best demonstrate and develop his abilities, what can be expected and demanded of him. N. P. Dubinin answers the question rather simply: "All normal people are capable of virtually unlimited spiritual develop- ment" (p 68). "Giftedness is the effecti~ve development of essential human traits combining a normal genotype with beneficial conditions for development thereof (i.e., chiefly the "'acquired nature' thereof)" (p 73). In other words, apparently with a normal genotype, the beneficial conditions can make a L~monosov, Marx, Newton, Beethoven, Rafael, or some~ne else out of any child by providing for "virtually unlimited spiritual development." And if there are very few even less gifted people who develop from the multitude of children, it is the fault of . their parents who did not provide the same conditions for them as were present for Lomonosov. Incidentally, Academician N. P. Dubinin stipulated: "i.e., chiefly the 'acquired nature'." And this apparently means that "the inborn factor" also plays a part, if not the leading one. If this is so, does not N. P. Dubinin find himself among the "authors who related the human psyche directly to genes," at least to an insignificant degree? The question of being gifted requires investigation. But N. P. Dubinin, proceeding from his views, sweeps away in-depth studies of gifted- ness, whose authors try to demonstrate the potential abilities of man. 5uch studies have not yet advanced enough, but to abandon work in this direction would be extremely detrimental to the development of our society, as would abandonment in general of all three problems that N. P. Dubinin "closes." Declaring loudly that he is speaking from the vantage points of Marxism, N.'P. Dubinin considers this as grounds, from the very first lines of his article, to discredit people of a different mind stating that they are "undertaking an attempt at revision," and even trying to "abolish the Marxist teaching on the integral social essence of man, ad~�ancing the thesis that the genetic program - supposedly controls the social behavior of people" (pp 62, 63). He mentions the late Academician B. L. Astaurov as the first example of those "who did not escape this craze," and quotes an extensive passage where B. L. Astaurov writes about the significance of the law, according to which all of the proper- ties of an adult organism, not excluding man and his psyche, depend on heredity and the environment. But N. P. Dubinin interprets recognition of this law as a "thesis," that supposedly the "genetic program controls the social behavior of 16 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY people." Confusing the concepts of "to be rel.ated jor dependent] and "to be controlled," he attributes to B. I,. Astaurov sometfiing he did not say and, at ~ the same time, involvement in efforts to "aboJ.ish Marxist teaching on the integ�- ral social essence of man." IncidentaLly, N.P. Dubinin also attributes to Marx things that he did not write at all. It is expressly in the "Theses about Feurbach" that Marx states: the essence of man is not an abstraction inherent to a separate individual. In reality, it is the aggregate of all social relations."* N. P. Dubinin writes, with reference to man, that "in the words of K. Marx, he is the aggregate of all social relations" (p 62). But is it not clear that "man" and "essence of man," and in general any object and its essence are ratner different concepts, so that to replace one with the other is to distort the author~s thought. Let us also mention that Marx writes about the essence of man in its reality, of man in the collective sense, rather than an individual; let us compare this to the thesis, according to which genes determine the possibility of development of an individual's traits.... It is obvious that the contradiction that N.P. Dubinin wishes to see does not exist here. However, it is not the inaccurate quotation or inadequate comprehension o~ some o~ the judgments of Marx that is the main point. The main point is that the article of Academician N.P.Dubin~n, with all its claims of a Marxist position, is in reality something that is quite the opposite of Marxism. The spirit of Marxism lies in systematic scientific attitude, concrete analysis, in-depth penetration into the entire aggregate of available facts referable to the problem under study. But in the article in question, only declarations are made with regard to the psyche of a normal man. ~ The spirit of Marxism lies in dialectics and, consequently, consideration of all associations, mediations and mutual transitions, to the extent of the unity of opposites. But in the article under discussion, the very thoughtof interaction between biological and social factors with reference to the psyche of normal man is re~ected. The spirit of Marxism lies in unconditional, specific humanism. But in the article in question, concrete analysis of human problems relatedt~i-heredity (with a"normal" genotype) is replaced with abstract statements as to the unlimited abilities of man. Furthermore, there is the question of principles of scientific research and scienti- fic polemics. The main point is not referable to specific mistakes and absurdities per se. The main point is for something alien to science by virtue of inethods of reasoning and approach to the problem not to replace research and scientific - discussion of the most serious problems. Academician N. P. Dubinin I never denied the importance of problems of human genetics. Science has offered good proof of the effect of biological heredity on physical, neurodynamic and psycho- dynamic distinctions of man. At the present time~ more than 2300 Mendelian traits have been studied. Moreover, questions of damage to biological heredity are being *K. Marx and F. Engles, "Soch.," Vol 3, p 3. 17 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 . . _ raised with exceptional acuity at the present time. According to data for 1977-1980, 10.5% of the children in the world are born with genetic defects. Of these, 3% (according to the UN O.r.ganization) are born with inheri~ed metital deficiency. ~ There is a question of dynamic growth of the genetic burden under the influence of mutagens, which pollute the environment, and other facY.ors. Much work is being done on this subject. I myself am a participant of many All-Union programs and, in particular, I head the section of "Genetic Aspects of the Problem of 'Man and the Biosphere of the USSR State Committee for Science and Technology, and am involved in the work of international committees dealing with the problem of the biological factor in man. And, in the mentioned article, "Inheritance of Biological and Social Factors," in the ~ournal KOMNIUNIST (No 11, 1980), the signif icance of the biological factor is very clearly stated. Of course, each man is a distinctive genetic individual with his own properties, his own anatomical, physiological and biochemical features, with numerous distinctions, such as elements of the psyche, endocrine system, speed of neurodynamic processes, etc. But the point is how this is related to man's social essence: do variations of normal genotypes inherited from parents have a direct, adequate effect on social and human properties, on man's spiritual world? Efforts are being made to relate them in a fatal way. It has even been said that creation of a new man would be impossible without altering the genetic prerequi- sites of modern people. ' ~tatements are being made that one can supposedly "predict" all of the potential spiritual abilities of each neonate on the basis of a study of his genes. It was written in LITERATURNAYA GAZETA (4 July 1980) that one can identify a.future member of the Writers' Union at the embryonic stage, according to the set of "writer" genes received in the zygote after fertilization. . Such statements are beyond the boundaries of science. In the presence of dialectics of social and biological factors, it is imperative to draw a distinct line between them, since the biological part of man is merely the lowest form of movement of matter. The essence of man is social. This idea is the basis for my article in KOMMUNIST. I am deeply convinced that my point of view discloses real possibilities for the spiritual, social and industrial development of each man, while the vew of fatal genetic predetermination precludes these possibilities. Moreover, the latter point of view is scientifically incorrect: we have no data to the effect that man~s spiritual world, his work activities are biologically de- . termined; in fact the specifics of man are created as the product of all social relations. For this reason, we shall fight for the view that offers our people the opportunity of enormous social development on the basis of normal human heredity. The biological basis, being the lowest form of movement of matter in man, serves anl.y as the pre- requisite for him to enter into the social form of movemant of matter. Conscious- ness, the spiritual world and ability to work do not appear in a child from his biology, but as a result of his active entrance into the world of social practice. Herein lie the enormous capabilities of pedagogics and psychology, that chief factor that should serve as the foundation of formation of new man by the condi- tions of the social environment. COPYRIGHT: Izdatel'stvo "Nauka", "Vestnik Akademii nauk SSSR", 1981 10,657 . CSO: 1840/252 18 FOR OFFICIAL UST ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONL9' , PHYSIOLOGY UDC 612.821.6+613.693 ' EFFECT OF ARTIFICIAL GRAVITATION IN SPACE FLIQiT ON RETENTION OF MOTOR HABITS BY ALBINO RATS Moscow ZHURN~IL VYSSHEY NERVNOY DEYA~Z'NOSTI IMENI I. P. PAVLOVA in Russian Vol 31~ No 2, Mar-Apr 81 (manuscript received 6 Mar 80) pp 261-266 [Article by N. N. Livshits, Z. I. Apanaserilco, M. A. Kuznetsova and Ye. S. Meyzerov] [Text] The goal of this project was to s~cudy the influence of artifical gravita- tional forces on some indicators of tk~e 2~igher nervous activity of albino rats in space flight. Certain changes in thc behavioral reactions of animals following their exposure to space flight factors in biosatellites were noted by us earlier (3,4). Accelerations are known to ha~~e an effect on the central nervous system on earth (6,7, etc.), but there is no information in the literature on the action of these factors during orbital flight. We studied behavior of animals in the first 2 weeks after the landing of a bio- satellite aboard which artificial gravitation was created. Methods The conditions under which animals were maintained in a vivarium and aboard the biosatellite were published earlier, as was the complete program of clinical-bio- logical research (1). In addition to groups of animals studied in accorclance with the complete program, there were a group of rats for which only higher nervous activity was investigated, and a group of rats studied in accordance with an abbreviated program containing only a third of the procedures included in the complete program (Table 1). A maze devised by Ya. Dombrovskaya was used to study rat higher nervous activity. We recorded the number of refusals to run the maze, the time taken by the rat to move from the starting ccmpartment to the feed compartment, and the mi.stakes made en route. We kept count of the number of times neurotic states arose, and of the number of disturbances in the normal dynamics of mistakes in the course of experi- ments. The research methods were described earlier (3). It should be pointed out - that we attached special significance to the number of times rats refused to run the maze in our analysis of the results. Disturbances in higher nervous activity deduced from other indicators could be assessed at less than their true importance in the event of a significant increase in the number of refusals, because this would 19 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 Table 1. G'haracteristics of Experimental Conditions No. of Angular Volume of Group Animals Accelera- Velocity, Experimental Exposure Conditions S ols in Group tion, g Radians Research Aboard b~io- Ddotionless IlH3 5 0 0 Complete (all satellite BIOS [not procedures further identified] 5 1 5.3 " ICosmos-936 Centrifuge ~2 radius ~ 320 mm Mock-up Motionless CH3 5 1 0 on earth BIOS Centrifuge C~2 4 1.4 5.3 Reduced (only . radius higher ner- 320 mm vous activity) Centrifuge I~~ 4 l.l 5.3 Complete (all radius procedures) 98 mm Vivariums Ma~ n group ffiC 6 1 , ~ Additional ffiC~ 18 1 0 Partial - group (1/3 proce- dures) Intact ~3 9 1 0 Reduced (only animals , higher ner- _ group vous activity) (with no sensors implanted) mean that information about those animals which experienced the most significant - changes in higher nervous activity would be excluded from the analyzed material. ~ Concurrently with recording the state of higher nervous activity, we used a five- point system to score the feeding activity of animals in the feed compartment of the maze. The lowest point score, l, meant total absence of a reaction to the feeder. The highest, 5, signified fast eating of all of the feed. Intermediate poin't scores (2,3,4) corresponded to different degrees of taeakening of the feeding reaction (not finishing all of the feed, slow feeding). We began training the rats to find the feed in the rnaze 2 weeks before the flight, and stopped the training 5 days before the flight. The animals were distributed such that each rat in the experimental group had a partner in the control group with similar higher nervous activity indicators. The symbols used to refer to the different groups and descriptions of the effects to which they were exposed are shown in Table 1. " 20 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2407/42/09: CIA-RDP82-40850R000400450018-3 F~DR OFFICIAL USE ONLY 60 CN~ ' ~ ~ x ~ - 40 -x x-x- x ~ , X\ ~u~ X-x-x-x ~~flX ~ ~ \ 20 nNJ \ 8ifa ~ ~ o ~ u2 . O B~" ~ ~-r ~ ~i � �~,~-n~s!? : : _ 3 ,5 7 . 9 f1 >3 Figure 1. Refusals to Run the Maze in the Recovery Period: abscissa-- days after landing; ordinate --refusals to run the mazes, percent. Letters beside the curves are the abbreviated names of the animal groups. See text for e~splanation. dt,~ ~20 ~ ' I~ ~00 I ~1) 6i~adc ea 80 � 6- 60 4 ~ UO 2 ?0 - ~ 8x,~ Qlf BH~ r,y~ q; nH~ nu2 ~ ex,~ el( BRd CNJ uz OHJ lIUZ Figure 2. Mistakes in Recovery Period: Figure 3. Maze Running Time in the abscissa--animal groups (see tPxt and Recovery Period: ordinate--time, sec. Table 1); ordinate--difference between See Figure 2 for other symbols. group average for indicator under analy- sis in the testing period and two addi- tional experiments performed subsequently Key: 1. Absolute units 21 FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/09: CIA-RDP82-00850R000400050018-3 Body temperature sensors were implanted into the abdominal cavity of all animals - (except group HKg) 2 weeks before the flight. The maze tests were resumed 3 days after the biosatellite landed. For 16 days we studied the animals' capability for restoring and reinforcing the previously developed feed-seeking habit. An experiment with a heightened functional load was performed on the 17th-18th days in order to study tiring of the higher divisions of the central nervous system. During this experiment the number of maze runs was increased to 16, and to avoid saturation of the animal, the number of individual reinforcement feedings was re- duced corresporidingly. The results were treated statistically using algorithms for comparison of two processes, calling for dispersion analysis (5), and, in some cases, analysis on the basis of the "phi" and inversion tests. Research Results and Discussion The results of research on rats maintained in a vivarium showed that the behavioral habit previously developed in the maze was easily restored by intact animals, and with somewhat greater difficulty by rats subjected to surgery (implantation of sensors in the abdominal cavity). In this case the greatest difficulties were noted among animals tested in accordance with the complete program. This can be distinctly seen from change in the number of refusals (Figure 1), the maximum increase of which was recorded in group BK, and the minimum of which was recorded in ffiCH (P < 0.05). Changes in the number of refusals in group ffiCA were close to the indicators for group HKx; however, the rest of the parameters underaent significantly greater - alteration than in the intact control (HiCH). Differences in the number of mistakes _ (Figure 2) and time (Figure 3) were significant (P < 0.05). Similar relationships in the change in the number of refusals recorded for control groups subjected to comparison ~aere also revealed in the experiment with the heightened functional load. Research conducted on animals maintained in a m4ck-up of the biosatellite also re- veals certain difficulties in restoring the maze habit. All indicators for animals _ in group CH3 changed for the worse, without exception, in comparison with the same indicators for animals maintained in vivariums (BK). Differences between the groups were significant in relation to both the refusal criterion (P