JPRS ID: 9165 TRANSLATION FUNDAMENTALS OF ERGONOMICS BY V.P. ZINCHENKO AND V.M. MUNIPOV
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
Release Decision:
RIF
Original Classification:
U
Document Page Count:
261
Document Creation Date:
November 1, 2016
Sequence Number:
51
Case Number:
Content Type:
REPORTS
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4.pdf | 17.19 MB |
Body:
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
~ - ' ~
~T
~ i~ t~~I~i~ it . ~ 11`i~ ~#~i'iiti ~I ~~'fi'~i.~ i~ . i-i~I~i 1 ~ 1 _
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
_ FQR OFFICIAI. USE ONI.Y
JPRS L/9165 _
27 June 1980
Translation _
FUNDAMENTALS OF ERGONOMICS
_ By
V.P. Zinchenko and V.M, Munipov
~B~$ FOREIGN BROADCAST' INFORMATION SERVICE ~
FOR OFFICIAT USE ONLY ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
NOTE
JPRS pubiications contain information primarily from foreign
newspapers, periodicals and books, but also from news agency
transmissions and broadcasts. Materials from foreign-language
sources are translated; those from English-language sources -
are transcribed or reprinted, with the original phrasing and
other characteristics retained.
Headlines, editorial reports, and material enclosed in brackets
are supplied by JPRS. Processing indicators such as [Text]
or [ExcerptJ in the first line of each item, or following the
last line of a brief, indicate how the original information was
processed. Where no processing indicator is given, the infor-
mation was summarized or extracted.
Unfamiliar names rendered phonetically or transliterated are
enclosed in parentheses. Words or names preceded by a ques-
tion mark and enclosed in parentheses were not clear in the
original but have been supplied as appropriate in context.
Other unattributed parenthetical notes within the body of an
item originate with the source. Times within items are as
given by source.
The contents of this publication in no way represent the poli-
cies, views or attitudes of the U.S. Government.
For further information on report content
call (703) 351-2938 (economic); 3468
(political, sociological, military); 2726
(life sciences); 2725 (physical sciences).
COPYRIGHT LAWS AND REGULATIONS GOVERNING OWNERSHIP OF
MATERIAT,S REPRODUCED HEREIN REQUIRE THAT DISSEMINATION
OF THIS PUBLICATION BE RESTRICTED FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY.
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFILIAL USE ONLY ~
JPRS L/9165
27 June 1980
~UNDAMENTALS OF ERGOIVOMICS -
Moscow OSNOVY ERGONOMIKI in Russian 1979 signed to press ~
27 Jul 7g pp 3-36, 67-202, 206-224, 243-308
[Chapters 1, 3-5, 7 and 8 from book by V.P. Zinclienko and ~
V.M. Munipov, Moscow University Press, 21,500 copies] -
CON7ENTS
Introduction 1
Chapter I. Ergonomics and Its Place in the System of Sciences 9
1. Subject and Tasks of Ergonomics 9 ~
_ 2. Interdisciplinary Relations of Ergonomics 18
_ Bibliography 31
Chapter III. Principles and Methods of Ergonomics 36
1. Methodological means of Ergonomics 36
2. General Description of Ergonomic Research and desearch
Methods 39
3. Methods of Observation and Interrogation 48 -
4. Methods for the Study of Productive and Cognitive Activity 55
5. Methods of Evaluating Functional States 66
. 6. Modeling in Ergonomics 80
7. Use of Computers in Ergonomic Research 82
Bibliography 86
Chapter IV. Principles in Ergonom:tc Analysis of Work Activity 93
- 1.. Classification of Working Occupations 94
2. Functional Structure of Executory (Perceptual and Motor)
~ Action 101
3. Functional Structure of Cognitive Actions 130
4. Preparation of Information for Decision Ma.king 158
Bibliography :L65
. .
- a - [I - USSR - C FOUO]
' FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
i
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
- Chapter V. Ergonomic Bases of Technological Design 17I
1. Structnre of Ergonomic Properties and Indices of
. Technology 1~2
2. Consideration of Ergonomyc Requirements in Equipment Design 175
Bibliography 187
Chapter VII. Optimization of Information Display Systems and
Means 190
1. Operator Work With Information Models 190
, 2. Spatial c:haracteristics of Visual Information 200
~ 3. BrightnQss CharacteristiWs of Visual Information 205
4. Time Characteristics of Visual Information 20~
5. Coding of Visual Information 210
- 5. Requirements Referable to Visual Indicator Devices 214
7. Integral Displays 221
8. Mnemonics 222 -
� 9. Panels for Group Use 224
10. Methods of Three-Dimensional Diaplay 231
11. Audio Signaling Devices (Nor.verbal Messages) 233
12. Verbal Warning Signals 235
236 -
- Bibliogrsphy
Chapter VIII. Optimization of Work Movements and Controls 23$
l. Uptimization of Work Movements 238
2. General Requirements of Cont-.rol Elements 241 _
3. Requirements Referable to Different Types of Controls 252
Bibliography
a
.
r -
- b ~
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~ PUBLICATION DATA ~
English title . FUNDAMENTALS OF ERGONOMICS ~n
Russian title : OSNOVY' ERGONOMIKI
Authors . Vladimir Petrovich Zinchenko and
Vladimir Mikhaylovich Munipov
- Editor . G. S. Livanova
Publishing house : Moscow University Press
Place of publication . Moscow
Date of publication . 1979
Signed to press . 27 July 79 _
Copies . 21,800
COPYRIGHT: . Izdatel'stvo Moskovskogo universiteta, -
1979
- c -
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
INTRODUCTION
Socialism is the first in history to pose the question of working man not
simpZy as a worker, but as an individual whose comprehensive development
~ becomes a mandatory prereqiiisite for growth of productive forces and
progress of society as a whole. This a~proach also expresses the ob-
~ective tendency toward drastically increasing the role of the human
factor in national production, as determined by the scientific and techno-
logical revolution.
It is being a pressing need for economic development of the Soviet State
to create conditions that favor comprehensive development of skills and
creativity of the Soviet people, of all workers, during this period of
building of communism. Meeting many of the vital demands of the working
people is directly or indirectly related to the specific industry, in
which they are engaged. The Soviet State is concerned ahout improving
- w;orking conditions and safety, scientific organization of labor, reducing
and subsequently eliminat~ng entirely heavy physical labor, on the basis
of complex mechanization and automation of industrial processes in all
sectors of the national economy, and this is spelled out in Article 20
of the first new basic law, the C~nstitution of the USSR.
Development of physical production on the basis of increased efficiency and
quality is the chief route toward reaching the basic and long-range goals
of the economy of a developed socialist society. High efficiencp of
production is an exceptionally multifaceted problem. There is a wide
range of factors related to scientific and technological progress proper,
to refinement of the system of socialist management, continued development
of socialist democracy, growth of professional and ideological-tlne~retical
training of workers upon which depends attainment of this goal.. Al1 these _
factors are reflected in the immediate work process, in the labor of
people, in some specific form or other in various branches of the national
- economy and in some specific combination.
Increasing the efficiency and quality of labor is one of the most important
means of attaintng high efficiency of production. In our country, the
- movement tocaard high efficiency and quality of work has become genuinely "
1
FOR OFFICII~sL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
~
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
nation-wide. It is imperative to cot.duct theoretical scientific studies
of problems of hw-nan labor and its roie in modern industry for continued
and better solution of this problem.
Labor is a soci.al phenome.non. But since any labor is based on mental and
physiological processes, the disciplines dealing with human performance
and functions play a large role in solving problems of increasing the
productivity of labor.
With the development of industry~, changes occur~in conditions, methods
and organization of human labor; there are substantial changes in func-
tions, role and place of man in the labor process and, accordingly,
different aspects of scientific research on human labor move to the
fore at different stages of history. Predominantly an energy-related
- approach to the study of this process, due to prevalence in the past
of manual labor, is inherent in studies dealing with physiology of
labor, which appeared in the 19th century. At the present time,
industrial physiology, which has undergone a certain evolution, deals
with the patterns of physiological processes and distinctions of regula-
tion thereof in the course of human labor, and it backs up witr, the
appropriate data the means of organizing the labor process that would
be instrumental in maintaining human performance on a high level for a
long time. Industrial hygiene, a preventive discipline that deals with
the effects of the labor process and industrial environment on workers
for the purpose of elaborating sanitary-hygienic and therapeutic-
preventive measures to provide optimum working conditions, good health
>;tatus and efficiency of man, is closely linked with industrial physiology.
At the start of the 20th century, when technological progress resulted in
the appearance of complicated types of labor (driving a car, operating
a locomotive, etc.), which made some serious demands for high speed
reactions, perception and other mental processes of man, there was a
strong impetus for development of indua.trial psychology. This scientific
discipline deals with the psychological distinctions of human labor f~r
the purpose of increasing productivity of labor and forming personality
traits of professional importance.
Differentiation of disciplines dealing with labor has played and continues
to play a beneficial role in development ~i our knowledge about it. In
the course of such differentiation, research methods were developed and
refined, some important patterns were demonstrated, the principles were
formulated of rational organization of different aspects and elements
of the work process.
At the same time, as knowledge accumulated, contacts between disciplines
inevitably occurred. Industrial hygiene was compelled to turn to the
- data in 3ndustrial physiology and psychology, industrial psychology had
to turn to hygiene and systems analysis, etc. And this is understandable,
2
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
since in reality labor is not the sum of scattered elements, but something
whole. In actual performance of labor, the ~sychological components are
not separated from phys~tolog!.cal or social ones. Moreover, one cannot
_ unders tand human endeavor without studying it in its interaction with the
operation of technical devices whicli man uses to solve a given labor
problem.
In the late 1940's and early 1950's, a need arose for an integral system
of conceptions about working man, his labor performance, his correlations
with ma chines and the environment, on the basis of the accumulated
data about labor. Without such a system it was difficult to further de-
velop special disciplines and to ~ake effective use of the accumulated
knowled ge in practice.
But, of course, it is not only a matter of the logic of development of
discip lines. The objective processes that were induced by the scientific
and technological revolution played a decisive role in the inception
of the systems approach to the study of working man and his labor
- activities. Qualitative changes are taking place in the content of
labor, and the structure of occupations established over the centuries is
undeigo ing a transformation. Automation of industry, which is radically
altering the content of human labor, synthesizes many labor functions
in man's work, which were previously separated. Truly human creative
functions are demonstrable more and more in labor activity. The present
era of revolutionary transformations, the era of formation of a new
communist system, internally linked with revolutionary changPS in science
and tec hnology, fills with new meaning the thesis, according to which
"in the historically distant future, we are dealing with one of the
most radical transformations, of transformation of the entire existing
means of human endeavor" (see [44, pp 152-153] in bibliography for
Chapter I).
The contradic ti.on of scientific and technological progress consists of the
fac~ that, along with enormous positive results, it also brings certain
negative social consequences (see [1]). In modern industry, which is
extens ively sv.pplied with complex technological systems, drastically
greater demands are made of man, which compel him to sometimes work
at maximum psychophysiological capacity and under extremely complicated
working conditions. And man is responsible for the efficient function
of large systems for the control of production, transport, communications,
space f lights, etc., and an error on his part could lead, in some cases,
to very serious consequences. Technological progress has raised the
"man-machine" problem most acutely. Man's capabilities are expanding
_ because of development of tools of labor, but the tools often turn out
to be so complex or unrationally designed that it is difficult to use
them. With the development of technology, th~ problem arose of
conforming the design of machines and tlieir operating ~:onditions fio
3
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL U5E ONLY
the characteristics of working man. The machine must be convenient in
all respects for tha man who services it; it must correspond to his
psychophysiological characteriatics.
At the present time, technical equipment and technological processes are
become more complex (structurally and functionally), and control of
large complexes is undergoing centralization. Analysis of the efficiency
of automated control systems shows that it is expressly o~.erator error
that often is the cause of malfunctions in the system. The trends in
development of modern industry are such that the main designing difficul-
ties in the next decades will probably be related to determination of the
ways and means of optimizing interaction between man and machines,
rather than research on the characteristics of equipment. In the course
of designing complicated complexes, the problem arises of predicting
the performance of man (group of people), and it cannot be solved on
the basis of the rule: "let us build the machine, then we'll see why it
does not work," as we have seen from the sad experience of building
certain expensive systems.
Previously, every version of a work toel could have been tested, literally
for centuries, through human endeavor and constantly refined. But now,
society does not have the time for this (for example, in the last few
decades, there have been three successive generations of computers). At
the same time, the cost of technical equipment and the "price" of human
error in controlling complex systems have increased dr3stically. For this
reason, when designing new technology and updating existing technology,
it is imperative to take into consideration in advance and as thoroughly
as possible the capabilities and distinctions of the people who will be
using it.
The parameters of the physical industrial environment must also conform
with the characteristics of man, and only then can one expect high
efficiency of his labor. In some types of industry, in the course of the
work day man is compelled to stay in rooms with artificial light, in
the presence of a certain chemical composition of air that is required by
technology. Sometimes he has to work at high atmospheric pressure and
sometimes at low pressure. Some occupations involve the necessity of
enduring high accelerations, altered gravity, noise, vibration, etc.
Development of new machines and development of new technological processes
mean that a new environment is developed for man. Sometimes, this en-
vironment is a combination of natural and art-~ficial conditions, and
sometimes it is entirely artificial. For this reason, whsn creating a
new machine, one should not deal simply with a machine as such, but
with the "man-machine-industrial environment" system.
A complex, systemic approach to the study of the above-mentioned problems
was the methodologica~ basis for the birth of a new scientific discipline,
- ergonomics. Of couxse, the above problems had been raised to some degree
4
FOR OFFZCIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
.
or other before, and some degree of solution was obtained through studies
in industrial psychology, physiology and hygiene. In the course of
these studies, there was intensification of interaction between these
disciplines, and it became necessary for some of them, for example, -
industrial physiology and psychology, to penetrate into one another.
Studies and design man-machine-industrial environment systems created
the prerequisites for combining the engineering disciplines, human ,
sciences and those pertaining to his work activity, and caused the appear-
ance of a new psychophysiological problem. Ergonomics was formed on
the borderline between psychology, physiology, industrial hygiene and
engineering sciences. All of them, with the exception of engineering
sciences, study the same ob3ect, buti they consider working man from
different points of view and use different methods.
The set of human sciences is playing an ever increasing role in studies
of problems of development and control of modern industry, of increasing
its effectiveness. Formation of ergonomics reflects the need for social
production to syntheaize the achievements in socioeconomic, natural and
engineering sciences as applied to the tasks of studying and planning
work processes. It was stressed at the 25th CPSU Congress that "New
opportunities for fruitful research, both general theoretical, basic,
and applied, are opening up on the boundary between different sciences,
in particular natural and s.ocial sciences. Full use must be made of
them" [3, p 87j.
While previously the development of technology was implemented primarily
by advances in physicomathematical, chemical and engineering sciences,
at the present time data from biological, psychological and socioeconomic
sciences are being used more and more to solve problems arising in
engineering [21]. "It is not technology by itself and not man as the
subject of production that are becoming the sub~ect of scientific research
in the area of work activity, but conformity of his ph,ysical and mental
capabilities,aesthetic taste and other social qualities with the proper-
ties of modern technological systems" [43, p 62.
The inception of ergonomics is related to development of a contradiction
within a real object, namely technology which, as a phenomenon with a
natural foundation, has a relatively independent logic of funct3on and
development, but as an element of the work process it functions in the
same system with man and develops in accordance with the laws of his
labor. "The existence of technology outside the body creates the
possibility of infinite technological progress, free of the limitations
of the human body. But no matter how much technology develops, it will
always remain as the "continuation" of man's riatural organs, his hands
and brain. The infinity of technological progress and the possibility
in principle of "relegating" the work f~snctions of the individual to
technology are limited by man's goals to its purpose as a means of human
labor" [40, p 55].
5
FOR OFFICIA;. USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
~
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Certain problems o� quality of labor, which is interpreted rather broadly,
are developed in ergonomics. Quality is an integral characteristic of
a given form of labor, in which determination is made of indices of qua-
~ lity and quantity of products as related to labor performed, the psycho-
logical and physiological t0price" of work, as well as health and
development of the worker's personality. The correlation and interdeter-
mination of all the above components form an integral system of quality
of a certain form of labor, which has a multilevel structure.
Ergonomicatly oriented work is referable to the category of applied
research that implements integration of science and industry. Develop-
ment and implementation of ergonomic principles and recommendations
are becoming a component of a broad program of ineasures directed toward
creating new and updating existing technology, toward further alleviation
of labor and ameliorating conditions from the standpoint of health, as
well as increasing its efficiency and quality. Ergonomics is making
a certain contribution to implementation of a multilevel and long-term
program for the transition from safety practices in engineering to
safe engineering. At the same time, use of the achievements in ergono-
mics makes it possible to increase substantially the attractiveness of
labor. "Man spends a significant and moat active part of his life at
work. Hence, the specifics of the requirements made by various social
- groups of his labor activity. These are requirements of the content of
labor, as well as of opportunity for self-expression and self-a ssertion
these are requirements of working conditions and schedule, which would
permit preservation of health and performance of various roles and func-
tions aside from work and, finally, these requirements refer ta a
certain material reward. The attractiveness of work is determined by
the extent to which these requirements are met" [6, 84].
In these times of faster and faster renewal of the existing store of ~
knowledge and, accordingly, engineering and technology, the ergonomic ~
refinement of different aspects of production must be included in the ,
program of man's education (in the broad sense) as an internal condition ,
for its implementation, and this would mean not only more effect3ve
solution of problems of adapting technology to man, but active formation
of man's capabil3ties in accordance with the requirements imposec~ upon him by
technological progress and the opportunities offered to ~im with the
development of technology.
The trends of development of ergonomics are making it necessary to apply
the methods and criteria it develops to any area of human endeavor, both
in industry and everyday life. The sub~ective area of research and
planning in ergonomics is also expanding as a result of inclusion of
various objects that form the ob~ective [ob~ect-related] and spatial
environment of human life, including the elderly and phygically handi-
capped. Today, one of the newest areas of application of the results of
ergonomic research is the design of technically intricate products
6
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
that improve living conditions. Ergonomics, in close cooperat3.on with
aesthetic styling in engineering, provides for the high consumer quality -
of these prodticts, their attractive appearance and convenience in use.
At this time of scientific and technological revolution, ergonomica is
acquiring increasing social and economic significance, aiding in the
most effective use of its achievements in the interests of man and
society. Ergonomics is called upon not only to help create optimum
working, living and recreational conditions for man, but to form new
cultural values, to create conditions for comprehensive development of man.
This textbook was written on the basis of our 20 years of experience in _
the field of industrial psychology, engineering psychology and ergonomics.
We used the course of lectures delivered at Moscow State iJnirersity imeni
M. V. Lomonosov. This book also reflects the re~ults of numerous studies
in general and experimental psychology conducted in the department
of industrial and engineering p~ychology of Moscow State University and _
department of ergonomics of the All-Union Scientific Research Institute of
Aesthetic Styling in Engineering [VNIITEJ, under the USSR State Committee
for Science and Technology.
We have summarized here material from publication's that we h~ve written
or written under our guidance: "Engineering Psychology," Moscow, pub-
lished by Moscow University, 1964; "Human Engineering Specifications for
Control Systems," Moscow, published by VNIITE, 1967; "Ergonomics. Works
of VNIITE," issues 1-17, Moscow, published by VNIITE, 1970-1979; "Ergo-
nomics. Principles and Recommendations," issues 1-7, Moscow, published
by VNIITE, 1970-1975; "Ergonomic Bases for Organization of Labor," Moscow,
Ekonomika Publishing House, 1974; "Microstructural Analysis of Executive
Performance," Moscow, published by VNIITE, 1975; "Psychometrics of Fatigue,"
Moscow, published by MoSco~t University, 1977; "Pressing Ergonomic Problems.
Human and Animal Physiology," Vol 21 ("Advances in Science and Technology,"
VINITI [All-Union Institute of Scientific and Technical Informa.tion], USSR
Academy of Sciences), Moscow, published by VINITI, 1978; "Current Status of
and Trends in Development of Ergonomics," Moscow, published by VNIITE,
1978. We also used the set of standards in "Man-Machine Systems" and
"Intersectorial Specifications and Standards for Scientif ic Organization
of Labor to Be Considered in Designing New and Remodeling Existing
Enterprises, in Developing Technological Processes and Equipment," Vol 1, -
Moscow, 1978, published by the Scientific Research Institute of Labor,
USSR State Committee for Labor. We were directly involved in preparing
these materials on engineering standards.
This book also deals with some of the greliminary results of scientific
and technological collaboration of CEMA member nations in the area of
ergonomic problems, which was implemented with the cooperation of the
Coordinating Center opened at VNIITE.
7
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
- We were �ortunAte enough to w~rk on the problems with which this book
deals under the guidance of and in collaboration w~,th a number of out-
standing Soviet scientists: S. G. Gellershteyn, F. D. Gorbov, P. I.
Zinchenko, A. N. Leont'yev, V. D. Nebylitsyn, D. Yu. Panov and E. G.
Yudin. They all served science selflesaly, were instrumental in the �
creakion and development of new directions in the aCudy and planning
of work activity= and they made an en~rmous contribution to elaboration
of new methodological principles for analysis thereof .
We wish to express our deep appreciation to the many coworkers and
~ colleagues whose personal participation, advice and critical comments
were of substantial help in working on this book.
There is as much responsibility as difficulty in preparing a textbook.
The difficulties are multiplied when dealing with a textbook on
an interdisciplinary, complex and new area of scientific and practical
endeavor. For this reason, these authora will b e grateful for criticism,
' comments and wishes pertaining to future improvements in this textbook,
~ and they should be addressed to the Moscow State University Publishing
' House.
i
~
_ i
_ 8
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USL ONLY
CHAPTER I. ERGONOMICS AND ITS PLACE IN THE SXSTF�M OF SCIENCES
1. Sub~ect and Tasks of Ergonomics ~
Problems of defining the ob~ect of ergonomics and the aspects of this
object that it studies, i.e., demonstrat~,on of the sub~ect of its studies,
are particularly important for a developing scientific discipline. Such
definitions are usually constructed on the basie of empirical data and,
to a significant extent, from the actual scientific contribution made
by specialists involved in this discipline [5, 13, 23, 28, 49, 51, 53, 54].
Ergonomics is a scientific discipline that studies man (group of people)
in a complex way, under concrete conditions of his (their) work, which
- is related to the use of machines (technological means). Man, machine and
the environment are considered in ergonomics as a complicated functional
- entity, in which the leading role belongs to man. Ergonomics is both a
scientific and planning discipline, since its task includes development of
methods of taking into consideration of human factors when updating existing
machines and technology and developing new ones, as well as appropriate
working (activity) conditions.
Interest in "man-machine" systems emerged in the middle of the 20th
century; it was attributable to the fact that diverse complex systems
for the control of industry, transport, communications, space flights,
etc., the eff.icient function of which is largely determined by the
performance of man, included in them as the leading element. The combina-
tion of human capacities and mach3ne capabilities (or capabilities of
an aggregate of technological means) improves substantially the efficiency
of control. In spite of the joint performance of control functions by
man and machine, each of the two elements of this complex system is
governe.d by the rules [patterns] inherent in the system, and the
efficiency of operation of the system as a whole is determined by the
_ degree to which the distinctions inherent in man and machine, including
limitations and potential capabilities, were determined and taken into
consideration when the system was created.
The concrete activity of man (or group of people) using machines (tech-
nological means) is the subject of ergonomics, while the "man (group of -
9 -
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
people)-machine (technological means [equipment]) system" is the object
of its study."* Optimization of such systems requires a complex approach.
"Ergonomics is science plus technology [engineeringJ. The subject of
ergonomics as a science is the activity of man the worker and man the
consumer. The ob~ectivz of ergonomics as technology is to optimize
korking conditions" [16, p 14].
Ergonomics considers the technological and human aspects in their
inseparable relationship. Ergonomics could, probably, exist and make
cer.tain achievements at the ~uncture of industrial psychology, physiology,
hygiene, and anatomy; however, genuine progress and its practical va7.ue
are determined by the level of synthesis in it of the hwnan and techno-
logical aspects. Moreover, the desire to disclose the patterns of this
synthesis characterizes ergonomics as a new discipline of a special type. _
To solve applied problems of ergonomics implies movement in two direc-
tions simultaneously: from the requirements of man to technology and condi-
tions of its function, and from the requirements of technology and its
operating conditions to man. Both these directions are interrelated,
and optimum solutions are usually at the points of their intersection.
In order to find such optimum answers, it is not enough to make use of
individual recommendations of industrial psychology, physiology and
hygiene, anthropometry and others. It is imperative to coordinate these ,
recommendations with one another, to subordinate and tie them in a
single system of requirements for a certain type of specific activity
and conditions under which it is performed. It is knowledge (or concep-
tions) of man's activity as a whole that is important, rather than
separate functional capacity to perceive, think and act, and it is
imperative to take into consideration a11 of the circumstances upon which _
depends the success of man's activity.
The ergonomic nature of technology is an integral feature, the most
general indicator that blends the hierarchic structure of properties
and indicators of lower levels. This integral characteristic evolves
from a number of ergonomic properties of technology:.capacity to be
contrelled, serviced, learned and inhabited. The first three are
- characterized by the limitations on inclusion of technology in the
corresponding fo~ns of activity of man (group of people). Habitability
of technology is characterized by approximation of conditions (the ,
environment) of its operation with biologically optimum environmental
parameters, with which a working man is provided with normal development,
good health and a high degree of fitness for work, and which also
minimize or eliminate the deleterious consequences of its operation to
the environment. Every ergonomic property of technology, in turn,
emerges from a number of complex indices which represent different, but
*Hereafter, for the sake of brevity, we shall refer to the man-machine
system (MMS).
10
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
interrelated aspects of these properties. The complex ergonomic indices
are formed on the basis of group indices, which are an aggregate of homo-
geneous, single ergonomic in djces: sociopsychological, physiological,
- psychological and psychophysiological, anthropometric and hygienic. T~is
hierarchic structure includes different levels of integration of ergonomic
indices. The study of interchanges from some levels to others constitutes
- the specific research task of ergonomics.
A distinction is made between two stages (phases) in development of ergo-
nomics and, accordingly, two types of ergonomics, corrective and pro~ective
(preventive), which are respectively related to problems of updating exist-
ing machines and systems and designing new ones. The approach used in
corrective ergonomics implies optimization of activity in each instance,
in order for each factor: psychological (when the significance of the an- -
thropometric, physiological and hygienic factors is considered beforehand
to be optimal, or else is not considered at all), physiological, hygienic,
etc. (with the same stipulations). Then the different data are summed
up. It is quite obvious that such a sum of idealized one-dimensional _
models does not conform with actual conditions of work, where all of the
factors are interrelated and interwoven. If the role of scientific
basis of complex planning of work were not attributed to ergonomics,
such idealization, which is widely accepted in scientific research,
would be not only permissible, but logical, since it permits achievement
of particularly rigfd~ results and reduction of time spent on studying
different aspects of work.
Corrective ergonomics plays!a rather important role, uniting specialists
_ in different areas of knowledge to solve important and pressing problems.
In corrective ergonomics, efforts are made to bring together, be it
often mechanically, facts obtained by different disciplines dealing with
labor. Corrective ergonomics has a definite positive influence on
planning practice, and it is instrumental in accumulation of different
facts about labor.
Formation of projective ergonomics implies not only accumulation of data
about human factors, but development of special studies of typical
forms and types of human work, creation of inethods for analysis and for-
malization thereof, detecticn of factors that determine its efficiency.
In turn, these objectives make it necessary to analyze factors that affect
performance of different types of work, to tabulate the ergonomic typology
of different types of work, to develop its own, specific research
methods for projective ergonomics.
Planners need a scientifically substantiated tool for planning labor
activities, which would permit optimization of MMS. Pro3ective ergonomics,
the formation of which permits neutralization of the overtly existing
- trend of "prescription" ergonomics that harbors the danger of limiting
the role of creative analysis in such a complex and important matter as
humanization of technology, working and living conditions, is called upon
to provide such a tool.
11 -
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
.
In view of the increr~sing trend toward technologization of life, specialists
in ergonomics are devoting increasing attention to the study of human fac-
tors as related to problems of planning, developing and assessing
technically complex products for cultural and personal use. However, only
the first steps have been taken in this direction.
Ergonomics is playing an ever increasing role in solving the complex prob-
lem of rehabilitation of individuals who have become disabled to some
extent or ather. Such rehabilitation is a system of government, socio-
- economic, medical, professional, pedagogic, psychological and other
measures directed toward returning the sick and disabled to society and
socially useful labor. One of the important directions of ergonomics is
the study of the capabilities and distinctions of different categories
of disabled people in order to take into consideration the results ob-
tained when planning equipment for public, administrative and residential
buildings, as well as different rooms, work places and different
industrial products. For the same purpose, ergonomics studies the psycho-
physiological capabilities and distinctions of the elderly.
With investigation of the MMS as its main ob~ect, ergonomics studies :
some of their specific properties. These properties were named human
factors; they constitute the integral characteristics of the relation- �
ship between man and machine (technological means), manifested under
specific conditions of interaction when the system is in operation,
which is related to achievement of concrete goals.
According to this definition of human factors, they cannot be reduced
to the characteristics of man, machine (technical means) or environment
per se. Obviously, basic knowledge about each of these system components,
which is available and obtained in the relevant disciplines, is used to
single them out and define them. From the above definition it also
follows that the characteristics and properties fixed in the concept
of human properties do not constitute individual, isolated features of
MMS components, but are its aggregate, systemic qualities. "A new
quality [trait], which arises as the product of integration, union ;
of many elements into a single whole, yields something more than the
sum of the elements; it reflects certain general cooperative properties
of a given set of phenomena, and it represents supraindividual certainty"
[19, P 83]� '
In relation to the properties-traits of MMS com~sonents, human factors
are secondary traits, which arise as a result of integration, embodiment
in one whole of innate traits characterizing the environment, objective
traits characterizing the machine (technological means), functional
traits, including social ones, charactr:rizing man. Ergonomics is not
concerned with all possible "primary" traits of man, machine or
environment, but merely with those that are determined by the position
and role of man in a MMS, and for expreasly this reason they are called
12
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
human factors. Evidently, the most perfect and efficient models will be
those in which the quantity of human properties and traits, set in a func-
tional relationship with his place in the system, with its innate and ob-
~ective qualities, will be at a maximum. For expressly this reason,
to optimize human work and assure the efficiency of the MMS, the competence
of individual disciplines dealing with various aspects of work is not
suf f icient .
Ergonomics must provide the necessary and sufficient nomenc~.ature of func-
tional relations between MMS components, since it is only in this case that
the MMS can acquire the status of a system with the specified efficiency
and meeting specific criteria. The nomenclature of functional relations
must be constructive (and not infinite), and it must meet a number of
criteria of rating MMS's, both technical (stability, re"liability,
resistance to interference).and socioeconomic. Concret:ely, this means
that ergonomics does not simply operate with various seis of base proper-
ties, traits and indices (hygienic, physiological psychological, socio-
economic, technological, ecological, etc.) obtained in the relevant dis-
ciplines, but transforms them into systemic traits by establishing the
necessary number of functional relations between them.
Human factors, construed as integral MMS characteristics of utmost im-
portance, thus constitute a certain superposition of base indices and,
accordingly, fixed (or dynamic) functional ties between MMS elements and _
components. Since the MMS repx,esent$ a.ce~ta~n~fu~et~onal stru~tuxe, from
tae standpoint of ergonomics the human factors emerge as the chief,
system-forming elements, or taxonomic units of analysis of the functional
structure of the system. Of course, the functional structure of the MMS
is characterized not only by human factors, but others as we11: organiza-
tional, informational, territorial, etc. For this reason, singling out the
human factors as units of analysis, i.e., elements of the functional struc-
ture of the system, does not, of course, preclude aingling out taxonomic
units of another type, depending on the ob~ectives of analysis of such
systems.
Human factors are not homogeneous. It is a rather complex and special
task to single them out and classify them. It is important to mention
that human factors themselves emerge as structural elements differii~g
in complexity. When interpreted in precisely this manner, they repre-
sent a certain temporary combination of forces capable of a specific
achievement. _
Such an interpretation of ergonomics and human factors disagrees with
the popular view of ergonomics as a set of disciplines dealing with work
or a sort of inetadiscipline that integrates others. The status of ergo-
nomics is determined by the fact that it operates with data obtained in
other sciences, transforms them, developing its own initial conceptions
and resources, and pursu3ng its own goals and objectives, which are
13
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
related to organization and planning of the conditions and methods of
man's work in the system.
As we have already noted, expreasly human endeavox serves a~ the basis for
singling out the human factors that must be taken ~nto consideration in
determining the functional relations between MMS components. There is
equal validity to the fact that the presence of such functional ties is
the necessary basis for organizing successful performance by man in the -
system. This means that the human factors are not given from the
start. They are something that is to be sought, something that can be
found only on the basis of preliminary analysis of MMS tasks, human func-
tions in the system, the nature and type of man's work.
Such analysis is a mandatory prerequisite in planning MMS's. That it
is done with greater or lesser professionalism, on an intuitive or scien-
tific level, empirically, experimentally (on appropriate prototypes,
mockups, experimental stands or actual devices) on the basis of
practical experience, is another matter. As a result of such preliminary
analysis of work, determination is made of the list of human factors,
consideration of which is necessary for efficient operation of the MMS.
The thoroughaess of human factors singled out s:~d, accordingly, the
abundance and fullness of potential functional ties between MMS compo-
nents that could be determined at the stage of planning the system allevi-
ate substantially introduction of these systems, including formulation of
occupational screening requirements, personnel training, coordination
of external means of work and methods of performing it, etc. A properly
determined and designed system of human factors is largely involved in -
determining MMS planning, providing for greater efficiency of its
operation and aiding in successful performance of tasks given to it.
Thus, man's work is the beginning and end of an ergonomic study, ergonomic
evaluation and ergonomic planning. The concept of work [activity~ thus
services also as the theoretical basis for the above interpretation of
human factors. For this reason, new conceptual schemes of work are
formed in ergonomics, as well as new methods of analyzing it which, in ;
turn, stimulates development ~f general theory of labor activity. In
this respect, the problems of ergonomics overlap those of praxeology,
whose task is to study the general laws of any activity and to determine
the most general rules of its organization.
On the basis of Marxist teaching on objective activity, its development
and forms, there has been an ob~ective spin-off of ergonomic research
into the area of theory and methodology of studying working activity.
Ergonomic problems can be effectively solved only "if synthesis of social
and natural sciences will not be headed on the road of inechanical
combination of the data from some disciplines or other into a certain
summated system or conglomerate of knowledge and not on the road of
- 'subordination' thereof, but rather if it bases itself on general theory
of working activity" [43, p 63].
i
14
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Ergonomics solves problems of rational or~anization of human work in the
MMS, purposeful distribution of functions between man and machine, defini-
tion of criteria for optimization of MMS with due consideration of the
capabilitiea and distinctions of a working man (group of people), and it
develops the typology of such systems. Ergonomic problems include those
of defining ~he methods of evaluating the dynamics of the functional state
of working people and optimum parameters of the MMS environment. ' ~
Systematization aad analysis of publications in journals provide some
idea about the content of ergonomic research. The English abstract
journal, ERGONOMICS ABSTRACTS classifies all work on ergonomics in the
following way (listed in abridged form):
I. Work of a general nature.
II. Man as a component of the system: 1) Perceptive (input) processes
(sight, hearing, etc.). 2) Central processes (short- and long-term
memory, decision making, attention, etc.). 3) Main motor processes (track-
ing, motor skills, manual skill, etc.). 4) Characteristics of perceptive- _
motor activ~ty and factors affecting it. 5)� Main physiologica~. processes.
6) Working conditions (static and dynamic loads). 7) Anthropometric and
biomechanical data. 8) Main data referable to physiology of sense organs
and factors influencing their physiological and biomechanical functions
(fatigue, stress, etc.).
III. Planning the means of interaction between man and machine: 1) Visual,
auditory, kinesthetic and tactile indica~ors. 2) Control organs and
specialized input devices. 3) Planning of work area. 4) Design of equip-
ment, instrwnents, mechanisms and machines, special safety gear. 5) Phy-
sical environmental factors (lighting, noise, vibration, temperatur~,
atmospheric conditions, etc.).
IV. Planning and organizing systems: 1) Distribution of functions between
man and machine. 2) Planning and organizing work (pace, work shifts, etc.).
3) Training. 4) Screening. 5) Motivation and attitude toward work.
V. Methods of research and experimental techniques in ergonomic studies:
1) Methods and instruments .for measurement~, analysis and evaluation of
data. 2) Training program, screening procedures, testing, methods of
interrogation, etc. 3) Modeling, including.on computers. 4) Statistical
data processing and experiment planning, including the use of computers.
The above list shows that there is some superfluosness of information,
due to the requirements for data retrieval.
Ergonomics not only studies, but plans purposeful variants of specific
types of human work related to the use of new technology. On the basis
of the work plan developed in accordance with the main objectives of the
15
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
MMS that is being developed, specifications are formulated far the techno-
logical means used in the work and, at the same time, for professional
screening and training, as well as the training equipment. Not only
do exogenous means determine the methods of work, but the external means
are also "transformed" in accordar..:e with work methods, as a result of
which there is optimization of conditions that are instrumental in
manifestation of human work.
Under socialism, ergonomics is involved in developing conditions, tools
and processes for work and life that provide for a better solution to a
triple problem: increasing t~e ~fficiency of work, preserving human
health and comprehensively developing the personality. "Even now, one must
apparently proceed from the idea of secondary, servicing function of
machines in working oiit a technological assignment and, consequently, one
n~ust consider first of all the positive traits of man as the effective
sub~ect of labor, i.e., that which makes up his advantages over machines,
rather than deficiencies. Basically new reserves are disclosed on this
route for increasing the efficiency of labor.... In store is the change
from solving urgent problems of organization of labor, refinement of
existing technology, man's adaptation to already existing technological
norms to planning new types of human work on the basis of complex theoretical
research on the potential physical,~psqchological and intellectual capa-
- biliCies of man, with which ergonomics is already concerned" [43, p 63].
Planning of human work is based on fundamental psychological research and
modeling higher mental functions: perception, memory, thinking (graphic
and conceptual). These functions are in essence the actual means or
psychological tools for work. These means (methods) include experience,
knowledge, programs and systems [schemes] of behavior, operator skills
that together make up his professional image. On the basis of the actual
means of activity, there is formation of permanent and operational
graphic-conceptual models which are the basis of the decision making
process and control of human activity, which is subject to special
formation and training.
Working man, who uses the arsenal of psychological work tools, bases him-
self on external means that are provided by the designers of machines and
systems. The external means of activity include information models
- rendered on information display devices (screens, panels, mnemonics,
indicator instruments or in the form of a document), computer software
(when solving problems together with a computer) and other ancillary
means of preparing a decision, control organs and communication devices.
Under different conditions, the emphasis of planning may be referable
to either external or inter_nal [human] means of activity.
Planning of specific types of human work, both in relatively simple wnd
extremely complex man-machine complexes, requires the performance of the
most diverse studies. Special publications in the field of ergonomics
16
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
are saturated with experimental anthropometric, physiological and psycho-
logical~studies of cognitive and executive work. Moreover, ergonomics has
left its imprint on the problema, theories and methods of research in the
- above disciplines. The findings obtained in the sciences dealing with
man now perform more than the function of illustration and proof of some
theory or hypothesis. They also acquire independent practical signifi-
cance. Knowledgeable planning of exogenous means of work and formation of
methods of performing it require not only theoretical knowledge about the
structure of human activity, but of a large number of quantitative data
pertaining to accuracy, speed, stability and immediacy [ongoing nature]
of performance of different types of activity. Such data can be obtained
only on the basis of development of new methods of experimental research
and special experimental stands. Their complexity (and cost) are some-
times commensurate with the actual complexity of the technological equipment
that operators of modern automated control systems have to deal with.
With this scope of experimental work and their orientation toward the
solution of practical problems, there is particularly great importance
in methodological studies.
Ergonomics is faced with two classes of inethodological problems. The
first is related to the fact that ergonomics is a field of interdisci-
plinary research. For this reason, the most important task is to develop
the methodological means of recording and synthesizing results obtained
in different fields of knowledge, upon which ergonomics bases itsel�.
One should not conclude from this that ergonomics cannot develop with
success until there is a solution, for example, to the age-old problem
of correlations between the psychological and physiological elements. In
ergonomics, this methodological problem is posed in a rather concrete
form, and it acquires the appearance of a problem of compatibility in
one experimental study of both various methods and means of interpreting
the results of obtained. Even now, some positive experience has been
accumulated in psychological studies of various types of objective
cognitive activity, with concurrent nolyeffector recording of physio-
logical functional structures and systems involved in performance of
these types of activity.
Ultimately, data are obtained which permit characterization not only of
overall effectiveness of activity, for example, in the mode of
detection, data retrieval or informational preparation of a decision, but
also the energy expended by the phsyiological functional systems
involved in its performance. On the basis of such integration in
a single experimental procedure of inethods that were developed in differ-
ent disciplines, in particular, in psychology and physiology, it becomes
possible to make more realistic forecasts about the efficiency and
possible functional states in the planned types of activity. Analogously,
experience is being gained in concurrent study of anthropometric, bio-
mechanical and psychological aspects of executive activity, for example,
17
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE QNLY
in the mode of sensorimotor tracking and operation of controls, inclduing
those with 2-3 degrees of freedom.
The second class of inethodological problems confronting ergonomics could
be called intradisciplinary. In each of the disciplines, upon which ergo-
nomlcs relies and the resuits of which it uses, there are various metho-
dological appr~aches, conceptual systems and methodological procedures
for studying and describing the same phenomena. These conceptual
schemes emerged in their day to ~olve spECific scientific and practical
problems, which do not coincide always by far with the problems of ergo-
nomics. But this does not by any means signify that the young scientific
discipline must manifest "methodological rigorism" and necessarily .
expound its own methodological conception, so to speak, in a voi d, so -
long as this methodology is original, ergonomic. It is another matter that,
when testing the applicability of some previously formed conceptual
systems and methodological approaches to ergo mmic problems,it becumes
necessary to partially revise, enlarge upon and alter them. Work on prob-
lems of inethodolog y and methods of research aids in expounding theory of
ergonomics and thereby enriches the practice of specific research.
2. Interdisciplinary Relations of Ergonomics
At the present stage of development of ergonomics, the question of correla-
tion between its subject and those of allie3 disciplines acquires special
significance. This is considered important, not only from the standpoint -
of defining the conceptions of ergonomics as a science and demonstration
of the constructive routes of its formation, but from the standpoint of
solving practical problems of organizing the relevant scientific research
and effective use of its results in various areas of endeavor.
Ergonomics, which deals with complex studies of man under specific condi-
tions of his activity with the use of machines (technological means), is
governed by the basic principles of Marxist-Leninist philosophy on
social and objectively active essence of man; about the integral and
concrete-historical understanding of man; about the essence and content of
the labor process as human objective activity, which transforms nature
and produces the entire world of human culture; about technology as a
system of material, artificially developed means of human activity;
about man and his comprehensive development as the highest criterion of
the value of scientific, technological and social progress. The theory _
of integrity of man, which is being expounded in philosophy, constitutes
the methodological basis for complex studies of interaction between man
and machines, man-machine systems and the environment.
As it studies the patterns of formation of a new society, scientific
communism serves as the methodological basis for defining the goals and
tasks of ergonomics under socialism. Being directly related to the
creation and development of technology, the study and refinement
18
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICYAL USE ON~Y
of human labor, ergonomics is governed by the theses of political economy,
the discipline dealing with laws that control industry, distribution and
exchange of necessities of life in a human society.
Ergonomics interacts with social, natural and engineering sciences. The
continuing process of formation of ergonomics takes place in contact with
- many areas of scientific and practical endeavor, and it enables us to
refer to basic sciences in relation to ergonomics, to a set of scientific
disciplines that are specially involved in ergonomic research and,
finally, to ergonomics proper as an area of scientific and practical
endeavor.
The logic of development of ergonomics links it more and more with
sociology and, first of all, industrial sociology, which is given the
leading role in implementation of a complex approach to the study of
labor (nature and content of labor, correlation between various
incentives and factors of being satisfied with the work, social aspects
of rational organization of labor, et~.). In industrial sociology, an ~
important place is given to the study of "man-technology" systems [40].
Studying the social aspects of work activity and patterns of functioning
of groups of workers, industrial sociology is working on a wide range
of problems that are the methodological starting point for many ergonomic
studies. Sociological studies .permit more specific integration of the
principle of objective determination of endeavor with the principle of
subjective activity. As it applies to work, the source of human activity
lies in the correlation between interests, since the individual always -
performs.this activity as a member of some social community, and it is _
directed toward satisfying the needs and interests of a specific social
group, class or society as a whole [47].
Using the results of studies in industrial sociology to study many applied
problems, erognomics in turn begins to have an increasing influence on
the former. The general tendency in development of sociology, whose
task is "not only to describe the present state of social ob~ects,
not to limit itself to a general forecast of their future state and
preparation of practical recommendations as the 'by-product' of studies,
but it m~~st specially elaborate a system of new ways and means that
would permit prompt and accurate exertion of influence on the objects
of social control as a whole and intensify their development" [20, p 166],
is significantly instrumental in strengthening these correlations.
Links with sociology is a mandatory prerequisite, but not the only one,
for use by ergonomi:,s of a complex approach to the study of man under -
specific conditions of activity. Links with social psychology, which -
deals with the patterns of human behavior and activities determined by
the fact that they are in social groups, as well as psychological charac-
teristics of these groups, are important to ergonomics. If these links
are overlaoked, it usually leads to a situation, in whicYi the researcher -
19
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
becomes incapable of seeing a live man behind the mean statistical data
and schemes, with all the wealth of his sociopsychological relations [42J.
Studies by social psychology of the cooperative joint activity of group
members define its most signif icant ties with ergonomics. Theory of
activity-mediated intragroup relations [35] is of basic importance to
ergonomic studies and planning of group (team) work. At the same time,
the activity-oriented approach in ergonomics also enriches this theory. _
Studies of informal relations of all types in groups are also important to
ergonomics: horizontal, vertical and "diagonal" (relations between indivi-
duals with different job status, but not under immediate subordination).
Of particular interest is the approach that emphasizes the role of
business relations (including those of responsible dependence) in the
formation and development of a work team. The study and planning of
specific f orms of labor activi ty imply consideration of sociopsychological
factors, which have a direct influence on the nature and results of
activitp. In this respect, two main directions of studies of the socio-
psychological climate, which refers to the prevailing and relatively
stable spiritual atmosphere, mental set of the group manifested by both
peoplets attitude Coward one�ar?other and the group~~s atitude towaxd a
common cause appear to be the most important.
Sociopsychological studies of attitude toward work and, first of all,
satisfaction with work, involvement of the individual in the work,
occupational and social ad~ustment, etc., also have a direct or indirect
bearing on the range of problems of ergonomic research.
Ergonomics needs to establish firm ties with industrial economics, the
sub,ject of which is labor in its historically defined form, social
and national economic organization of labor.
- At the present time, there is a tendency toward interpenetration of
psychology and economic science due to the need for obj ective development
of productive forces, change in nature of labor in the course of the
scientific and technological revolution, need to improve personnel
screening and training, increasing significance of rationalization and
_ organization of labor for eff ective use of the "human f actor," etc. [45].
Approximation of ergonomics and economics occurs under the influence of
the same processes. Determination of the socioeconomic effectiveness of
new technology, which has become a pressing problem of economic science, -
is an area where th? interests of economics and ergonomics overlap.
Without the back-up of ergonomic knowledge, it would hardly be possible
to obtain a productive solution to this problem. "The possibilities of
increasing labor productivity and, consequently, economic effectiveness
are found in the newest technology as such. But implementation thereof
does not occur automatically; rather it does when technology is creatively
used by man in the production process. The extent to which the potential
economic effect is realized depends on how technology influences man,
20
FOR OFFICIAL USE~ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02148: CIA-RDP82-00850R040240090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
his living conditions in society and working conditions in industry in
the course of using this technology" [25, p 12]. In turn, the possi-
bilitie3 of continued development of ergonomics and, particularly, of _
using its findings in different sectors of the national economy will
~ depend appreciably on the work on the range of problems related to
determination of the socioeconomic effectiveness of new technology.
- As we have already mentioned, the status of ergonomics is determined by _
~ the fact that it operates with data obtained in other disciplines and
transforms them, developing its own base conceptions and means. Develop-
ment of the general concept of MMS and appropriate language for describing
it as an integral whole, coordinating descriptions of this system in the
languages of different disciplines is considered te be one of the important
tasks for ergonomics.
Ergonomics is primarily concerned with the functional structure of the
MMS, which is determined by the place and role of man in the system,
internal relations of this system and interaction with the environment,
whereas industrial psychology, physiology and hygiene concentrate their
scientific interests on the study and modeling of different components of
this system and their interaction with other of its elements. "The
complexity of studying man engaged in production is related to solving two
parallel and interrelated problems in each of the disciplines forming
a complex: 1) studies of each element of such a system, distinctions
and patterns existing within this element; 2) studies of correlations
between elements, of existing relations and f eedback" [24, p 27].
Ergonomics cannot fail to be concerned with the study of individual
elements of the system, just like industrial psychology, physiology and
hygiene cannot overlook the relations between the elements of the system
they study and other components, and the system as a whole. Consequently,
it is important to study the relationships existing within this complex
system, no.t only from the standpoint of ergonomics, but of the discip-
- lines, at the ~unction of which it emerged. Moreover, it is only
by studying these relations that it is possible, for example, to solve
the theoretical and practical problems put to industrial physiology.
The main ob~ective of industrial physiology is to study the patterns of
physiological processes and distinctions of their regulation in the
course of work activity, i.e., demonstration of the distinctions charac-
terizing the functioning of physiological systems and the entire body,
as related to the existing relations between the above elements in the
system [24].
Ergonomics does not rule out or replace research that is conducted in the
field of industrial physiology, hygiene and psychology, it is based on
them and synthesizes their achievements. Ergonomics uses the results
and stimulates definition of optimum characteristics of the work process,
which permit achievement of high efficiency of labor, studies of changes
21
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR (.1i~'FICIAL USE ~NLY
in functional state of the hwnan body under the influence of his work,
which are conducted wirhin the framework of industrial physiology.
Ergonomics is guided by the data in industrial hygiene, which studies the
efFects on the human body of work processes and factors in the industrial
environment surrounding man, and develops hygienic standards and measures
to provide good working conditions and prevent o ccupational diseases.
Ergonomics could not exist and develop without the support of the entire
- set of studies in industrial hygiene, since the goal of the latter is
to provide scientific substantiation �or the bio logical optimum, to
which the environment must conform in order to assure normal development,
- good health, high efficiency and longevity of man [18]. The internal
logic af research dealing with determination of optimum parameters of
~ microclimate, lighting and other f actors of the industrial environment
' demonstrates the most fully the vectors of the required relations between
industrial hygiene and other disciplines, including ergonomics. By
studying the effects of factors in the industrial environment of man on
_ the quality of his prof essional activity, ergonomics gives impetus to
work on specific problems of industrial hygiene. Moreover, ergonomics
makes a substantial contribution to setting standards, developing ways .
and means of preventing the deleterious effects on man of various
fa~tors of the industrial environment.
It is very important to ergonomics to establish close ties with psycho-
- hygiene, which deals w3.th scientific bases of ameliorative measures
pertaining to mental health of people in order to prevent diseases.
The link between ergonomics and psychoneurology, which permits disclosure
of the genesis and pathophysiological mechanisms of neurotic states that
occur, in some r.ases, among workers in the cour se of their work and, in
particular, in stress situations, is equally important. ;
. ~
Ergonomics is called upon to supply the missing link in interdisciplinary ;
studies of working man. Ergonomic research, which is instrumental in ~
� developing a complex approach to the study of work activity, causes I
development of certaiu ",junctions" between different disciplines that ~
deal with working man. Problems are solved that have been posed by
the very logic of development of these disciplines and attributable to
changes in the nature of the practical tasks put to them. In this regard, ~
we can mention that formation of a discipline, related to industrial
p sychology, such as industrial psychophysiology [41], is becoming a
pressing need.
Complex studies of working conditions, hygienic evaluation of new techno- -
logical processes and equipment, psychophysiological studies of certain
- forms of labor, further development of scientifically substantiated
measures to control monotony, hypodynamia and hypokinesia--all this
permits fuller use of the achievements of scientific and technological
22
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
progress to improve working conditions. "In solving these problems,
industrial hygienists and physiologists must devote more attention to
cooperation with technological and planning institutes for evaJ>>~tion of _
new processes and equipment at the planning stage and alteratio~~ thereof
in accordance with hygienic and ergonomic requirements" [14, p 2J.
The complex approach in disciplines that deal with working man ia
implemented not only on the level of interdisciplinary interaction, but
within the framework of different disciplines in the course of studying _
various ob~ects using ~ one or several methods of a specific discipline.
"As a rule, in complex studies of working conditions, a hygien~ic evalua-
_ tion is made of the technological processes proper, equipment, instruments,
raw material, by-products and intermediate products, as well as the
final product, sanitary engineering installations, general and personal
safety equipment, architecture, construction~and planning of induatrial
- buildings and rooms, natural and artificial light in them, as well as
physiological and hygienic evaluation of organization of labor and
individual labor operations" [17, p 141].
Ergonomics could not develop without a link with human anatomy, the dis-
cipline that deals with the shape and structure of different organs and
the body as a whole. Functional anatomy, which defines the correlations
between structural distinctions of human organs and systems, on the one -
hand, and the nature of their functions, on the other, is one of the
scientific disciplines, on the boundary of which ergonomics emerged.
Studies of correlations and mutual determination of morphological bio-
chemical and psychological characteristics of man are of particular
interest to ergonomics. Ergonomics makes use of and further develops
- the aggregate of inethodological procedures inherent in anthropometric
studies, which are used to measure and describe the human body as a
whole and different parts thereof, as well as to determine the quanti-
tative characteristics of their variability.
The complex approach to the study and planning of human activity causes
close and multilevel relations between ergonomics and psychology [33], in
the subj ect of which activity is not included as a special "part" or
"element," but as a special~function of the individual's [sub~ect~s]
belief in ob~ective reality and its transformation into a form of
- subjectivity [22]. It is not only that the psychological factor is an
element of human factors in technology. Ergonomics is linked with many
branches of psychology: industrial psychology and engineering psychology,
aviation and space psychology, social psychology and psychology of the
personality [individual], military and pedagogic psychology. Ergonomics
~ makes full use of the methods established and formed in psychology for
studying cognitive and executive activity and, in some cases, it develops
and creates new ones.
23
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Being related to industrial sociology, occupational pedagogics, industrial
physiology and hygiene, functional anatomy and esthetics of engineering,
industrial psychology has laid a broad scientific foundation for the
emergence of ergonomics [36]. This was significantly aided by the pro-
ceas of solving a dual problem, which is considered pressing for the
d present stage of industrial psychology: 1) analysis and evaluation of
' relevant conceptions of disciplines dealing with labor for the purposes
of industri~al psychology and 2) concurrent determination of the signifi-
cance of psychological conceptions to disciplines dealing with labor j48].
Establishment of multifaceted relations between industrial psychology and
other scientific disciplines, including ergonomics, is largely determined
by Che synthetic nature of Che psychological factor in the labor pro-
cess, which is reflected, in particular, in psychological analysis of
activity. At all stages of development of industrial psychology, it has
concentrated on the psycholugical factors, quantitative characteristics
of work activity, which acquired special scientific and practical signi-
ficance in our times. Development of the psychological conception of
quality of labor is one of the most important problems of industrial
psychology. Creation thereof will stimulate further work on many
problems of industrial psychology. At the same time, its ties with
allied scientific disciplines and, first of all, with ergonomics, for which
the problem of quality of labor is among the most important, will be
strengthened and developed.
Engineering psychology is a branch of psychology that is closest to
ergonomics with regard to conditions under which it was conceived and,
mainly, its tasks and methods. In recent times, the idea has been
voiced, not without justification, that engineering psychology, being
historically related to industrial psychology, is forming nevertheless
as an independent branch of psychology characterized by profoundly
specific experimental methods, theoretical conceptions and approaches [27].
This is attributable, first of all, to the specifics of the subject of
study, in the capacity of which the performance of an ASU [automated
control system] operator was singled out, already at the early stages
of development of engineering psychology, with physical ~,o~e1s of control
and substitutes for real controlled sygtems [10].
Being a branch of psychology, engineering psychology examines only aome :
specific aspects of man-machine interaction, and in this respect it also
emerges as one of the sections of ergonomics, the task of which includes
complex studies of different aspects of interaction between man and
machine, the MMS and environment. "Engineering psychology could actually
- be viewed as one of the disci~lines that contributes to a broader area of
research on human factors" [50, p 699].
Engineering psychology directly preceded the appearance of ergonomics in
our country. It also strived to keep a complex record of human f actors,
24
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
and it outgrew quite rapidly the confines of actual psychological analysis
of work. At the start of its development, engineering psycholegy solved
the most acute and pressing problems pertaining to organization of the
work of MMS operators with automation devices. They included, first of
all, problems of sensorimotor tracking, detection and discrimination of
useful signals in the presence of noise on cathode-ray tubes, upgrading
of mnemonics (controls), etc. For this reason, the problems of engineer-
ing psychology began to be formulated in more general terms: develop-
ment of principles of planning infortnation models, studies of processes
of information retrieval, information preparation and decision making;
finally they became even broader: organization of informational inter-
action between man and machine. Of course, there was also transforma-
tion of research sub~ect matter at each stage of development of eng~neering
psychalogy. Responding to practical demands, engineering psychology
proliferated into an ever increasing range of tasks and problems, for
the solution of which the competence of psychologists was no longer
sufficient. Anthropologists, bioengineers, physiologists, hygienists,
designers and other specialists began to be called upon to join with
teams that had to solve problems of engineering psychology, and this
resulted in dzvelopment of the appropriate ways and means of complex
research. Expansion of the sub~ect matter of research and planning in
engineering psychology �l~d to the~:natural transformation of the
engineering psychology service in industry into ergonomic, although
the name remained the same for some time. Appeals for "psychologization"
_ were the distinctive reaction to this process of Soviet engineering
psychology. In essence, this meant awareness of the need for stricter
definition and narrowing of the area of research in engineering psychology
for the purpose of its effective development as a branch of psychology,
a leading section of psychology of man's labor.
Concurrently, there was a transformation in problems handled by engineer-
ing psychology, in the true sense of the word. It was and continues to
be induced by the rather rapid change in automation hardware: appearance
of neaer generations of computer5, il.i~l~ai.iuu display devices and
controls. Refinement of these work tools leads to a growth in scope of
MMS's, which become hierarchic, and there is a considerably larger volume
of information in computer memory, greater accessibility thereof to
operators, possibility of requesting the same information in different
forms--textual, symbolic, graphic, etc., drastic improvement in quality
of displaying information, broader possibilities for utilizing color _
and coding of information. The possibility of adjusting display systems _
to users no longer appears fantastic. In the near future, the user will
be able to select not only the brightness, contrast, angular dimensions
of an image and elements thereof that are convenient for him, but also
the method of writing letters, digits, etc.
All this results in the fact that the subject matter of engineering
psychology is shifting more and more to the area of research on
25
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
decision making, organizing dialog interaction with computers. Operators
are changing into users of computers and other automatic equipment. At
the same time, refinement of the latter is putting some qualitatively
new tasks to engineering psychologists. The volume of data stored in
computer memory is so great that they cannot all be displayed simul-
taneously, and when displayed successively they are difficult to
visualize. Nor does a general information model save the day, since
generalization of information can be made on many bases. In other words,
it is not so much the information model transferred to information dis-
play devices as the information model stored in the computer's memory
that becomes the object of operator (user) activity; not infrequently, the -
latter is called the information block, the aggregate of data bank bases.
When referring to these data bank bases, the operator must himself
form the dynamic information model, which requires utterly different
- training, knowledge about the aggregate of data bank bases, ability to
inCerpret them, knowledge of the possible levels of data processing and
different bases for classification thereof. The most difficult task is to
construct a logic3l order of data bank bases on the basis of complex,
heterogeneous and diverse information models, transfer thereof to display
equipment, followed be evaluation, correction, modification and decision
making. This is only one example of the new range of problems of engineer-
ing psychology, which requires that it develop intensively in even
closer contact with general and experimental psychology. Of course, as
ergonomics prepares the requirements for consideration of human factors
in engineering, it is governed by and uses the results of both prior and
the most recent research in engineering psychology. This applies to the
same extent to other disciplines dealing with labor and work activity.
It is premature to maintain today that all problems of correlation between
engineering psychology and erogriomics have been resolved. Some difficul-
ties still arise, and they are related primarily to the fact that the
formation of engineering psychology and ergonomics is a continuing
process. The subject of both disciplines is being defined and, in some
cases, even re-emphasized. Nevertheless, some clarity has been brought
in the main and substantial point that characterizes the unity and
difference between these two areas. Whenever differences are not
demonstrated between ergonomics and engineering psychology and they
are equated, fruitless debates occur, the cause of which is not a
difference in views, but terminological misunderstandings [52].
At the present time, the development of psychological science is stimu-
lated, in many respects, by the tasks of ergonomics, which introduces
into its context some forms of work, new means of performing it and of
studying it. In the near future, we should expect further deepening and
broadening of the ties between ergonomics and psychology.
When planning man's activity in control systems, usually prob~ems of
occupational screening, education and training are resolved at the same
26
FOR OFFICIAL USE OPTLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
time. Combining in an integral system the planning of work activity,
professional screening, education and training makes it possible to
perform each of these tasks on a qualitatively different level than is
done in other cases. This dynamic system is called upon to solve prob-
leme, not only of optimum adaptation of machines to man, but active
~ development of human skills to conform with the requirements made of man
by technological progress and the capabilities that are disclosed to him
with the development of technology.
Ergonomics, along with pedagogics and pedagogic psychology, is called
upon to aid in the process of refinement of polytechnical education in
the schools, in order to provide a certai.n vocational guidance and
appropriate training of young generations for work with the new
technology that is being planned and developed. In the course of poly-
technical education one can start acquainting students with ergonomics
as an element of general, industrial and labor culture.
One of the most important problems is to establish the relative significance
of psychophysiological functions act~vated by professional work to develop-
ment of polytechnical sets and skills. By synthesizing the achievements
referable to a number of disciplines dealing with work activity and
engineering, ergonomics can aid in establishing appropriate intersub3ect
relations and better organization of the teaching process itself. For -
this reason, there is some validity to statements that it is high time
to create pedagogic ergonomics. The problem of taking into considera-
tion the content and methods of teaching students (future blue-collar
workers, engineers, etc.) in secondary schools when preparing the
ergonomic specifications for machines, work places and the industrial
environment.
Under socialism, scientific organization of labor and ergonomics have
the same goals: .to aid in increasing labor productivity, preserving health -
and development of the personality. There are many common directions of
research, which are related primarily to the study and planning of work
processes, refinement of organization and servicing of work places-
improvement of working conditions. At the same time, scientific organiza-
tion of labor and ergonomics are different levels of study and planning
of work processes, between which there are certain correlations and
transitions from one level to another. At each of these levels, their
own inherent laws are established, and this is reflected in a certain
theory, system of concepts and categories [12, 23, 32, 34].
Ergonomics and scientific organization of labor work with different
units of analysis of work activity, for the definition of which the
same terms are sometimes used. Ergonomics has adopted the scheme of
units [entities] of analysis of work that is heing developed in psy-
chology: specific activity, action and operation [22]. Activity is
directed by motivation, behind which there is always the subject's need.
27
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL iJSE ONLY
Motivation not only prompts activity and creates its direction but, what
is very important, it imparts to activity (and all pr ocesses that imple-
ment it) a certain personal meaning (one could also s ay subjective value).
Operations, procedures, actions and movements are the units for analysis
of the work process in scientific organization of labor. "The operation is
the main element of the work process. It is performed by one or several
workers at the same work place on the same ob~ect of labor. A work pro-
cedure combines several work actions by man's working organs, which are
performed without interruption to perform some elements of the same
operation. Work action is the aggregate of working movements of the
fingers, }iands, legs, as well as body of a worker, which are performed
without interruption. Work movement is the single d isplacement of fingers,
hands, legs, as well as body in the labor process. Work movement can be
broken down into micromovements" [37, p 9].
A comparison of units of analysis of work activity used in ergonomics and
scientific organization of labor enables us to maint ain that we are dealing
with studies of the same ob~ect that differ in content. In the former
case, attention is focused mainly on demonstration o f internal patterns
of activity, whereas in the latter case there is pr edominant consideration
of external manifestations of the same activity. This difference is also
reflected in the investigative methods used, although some of the methods _
are the same in ergonomics and scientific organizati on of labor. Organiza-
tion of labor under modern industrial conditions, characterized by complex
mechanization and automation of production processes, has raised a number
of new problems, the effective solution of which is possible only by
means of scientific synthesis of data pertaining to work activity of man.
The integration of results of studies, which were ob tained in different
- scientific disciplines, which is made by NOT [scient ific organization of
labor], must be enlarged by the results of such synthesis, which is made
in the course of interdisciplinary studies. Ergononomics is one of the
directions in which such synthesis is made.
Ergonomics and NOT are two independent but organical ly interrelated areas -
of scientific and practical endeavor. Ergonomics is making an increasing
contribution to the cause of socialist NOT. The pro cess of continued inter-
action and reciprocal development of ergonomics and NOT is demonstrable
- in three main directions.
The first direction is related to work on theoretical problems, primarily
problems of planning group and individual work proc e sses. One of the
first and foremost problems is socioeconomic effect iveness of introduction
of NOT and results of ergonomic studies in the national economy.
The second direction of application of joint effor~ s of specialists in
ergonomics and NOT is establishing the standards and requirements of NOT
- and ergonomics. The work being done in this direct ion must be expanded
significantly and given scientific substantiation. This direction is
28
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
closely linked with the first one, sin~e the effectiveness of development
of these standards and requirements depends largely on the study of
theoretical problems.
_ Finally, the third direction is related to the direct use in industry of
the achievements of ergonomics. Experience has shown that the maximum
effect is obtained when measures in the field of ergonomics are included
as elements of NOT plans. Ergonomists, together with specialists in NOT,
should become more actively invo lved in processes of technological
re-equipment and remodeling of existing enterprises. "An important
distinction of modern remodeling is that, in addition to technical re-
equipment which provides for an increase in production and decrease in
cperating expenses, it is solving to an ever increasing extent socio-
economic problems, i.e., improvement of working conditions, elimination
of heterogeneity of labor of workers in different occupations, over-
coming the substantial differences between mental and physical labor,
raising the physical standard of living of working people and proteetion"
of the environment" [39, p 22~.
Ergonomics is playing an increas ing role in providing safe working condi-
- tions [7]. Labor safety regulat ions refer to a system of legislative
documents and corresponding socio economic, engineering, hygienic and
organizational measures that assure safety, safeguard the health and
fitness of man in the process o~ working.
Consideration of the requirement s of ergonomics is a mandatory prerequi-
site for creating convenient, reliable and safe technology. Basing
itself on work in the area of labor safety, ergonomics supplements and -
develops it in some respect. It is becoming generally recognized that
the number of accidents ultimately due to dangerous actions is consider-
ably greater than the number of accidents due to dangerous conditions.
In this regard, it is noted that ergonomics opens up new opportunities
to define the latent causes of unsafe actions that could lead to
accidents.
The problem of criteria of asses sing the difficulty and intensity of
labor, which can be solved only by using a systems approach and on the
basis of the advances in industrial hygiene, physiology and psychology,
industrial economics and other disciplines, reflects the most the
need for organic correlation between labor safety and ergonomics. The
ergonomic approach is necessary for the study of difficulty and intensity
of labor, which are manifested by the functional parametars of the
body formed under the influence of physical, mental or emotional load
and factors of the industrial environment.
Use of the achievements of ergonomics permits more effective solutions
to current problems of labor safety. As far back as the 1930~s, there
was discussion of the matter tha t requirements pertaining to improvement
' 29
FOR OFF ICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USi ONLY
of health-related and safety conditions of work must become an organic
element of the process of developing technology, rather than attached to
it as something extraneous and independent. Under modern conditions,
such formulation of the question has become a real p roblem. "Our goal,"
stressed L. I. Brezhnev in a speech at the 16th Cong ress of USSR Trade
Unions, can be formulated as follows: from labor s a f ety techniques
[practices] to safe techniques. We are on this rout e and will advance
persistently on it" [4, p 187]. On this route, clos e relations are
established between labor safety and ergonomics, which is one of the
important areas of work on scientific and methodolog ical problems of
creating safe techniques [equipment].
Many problems and pra~tical tasks are worked on by e rgonomics in close
collaboration with design, which makes it possible to implement the
most fully its principles and requirements. Ergonomics is viewed as
the natural scientific basis of design [30]. In turn, design enriches
the range of ergonomic problems studied by means of including it in
a broader context of development of culture [31]. On the practical level,
consideration of human factors is an inalienable par t of the entire pro-
cess of aesthetic design of industrial products and corresponding trans-
formation of the industrial and objective-spatial environment. In
pricniple, design cannot exist and develop apart f rom ergonomics.
Ergonomics is closely related to engineering and mathematical sciences:
cybernetics, systems analysis, general theory of sy s tems and study of
operations, as well as other disciplines and direct ions of modern
scientific research.
Ergonomics solves a number of problems posed in sys tems analysis: evalua-
tion of reliability, accuracy and stability of work, effect of inental
tension, fatigue, emotional factors and distinctions of the neuropsycho-
logical organization of an operator on efficiency of his performance in
the MMS, adaptive and creative capabilities of man. In practice, the
correlations between ergonomics and systems analysi s refer to the
problem of organizing comprehensive and professional consideration of
human factors at different stages of developing and operating systems.
Consideration of human factors is a mandatory component of development
of structural and functional schemes of both the sy stem as a whole and
its individual elements.
Ergonomics is related in some way or-other to all s ciences the subject
of study of which is man as the subject of labor, cognition and communi-
cation. In solving practical problems, ergonomics must base itself
on the entire system of knowledge about man. As the formation of ergo-
nomics progresses, it is exerting an increasing inf luence on development
of this system of knowledge. "Among the new humani tarian disciplines
of utmost importance to general theory of the scien ce about man, we should
mention ergonomics, which could be defined as a sp e cial science dealing
with the labor activity of man" [5, p 13].
30
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The above analysis is indicative of the abundance of interdisciplinary
relations between ergonomics and social, natural and engineering sciences,
of course, primarily to those aspects that are closest to work activity.
As we know, interdisciplinary ties are dual in nature. Not only is ergo-
nomics sub~ect to the influence of sciences related to it, it has
already begun to exert an influence on them in the area of theory, methods
and practice. At the present time~ most obvious is the effect of ergo-
nomics on the last two areas, since applied problems continue to be
prominent in ergonamics. Development of complex [cooperativej research
leads to a certain change in disciplines involved in a given study [38].
This change is not something artificially imposed on sciences dealing
with labor exogenously; it is a logical stage of their development [24].
Ergonomics emerges as a distinctive catalyst of this process.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
1. Marks, K., and Engels, F. "Soch." [Works~, Vol 20, pp 495-496.
2. Lenin, V. I. "Poln. sobr. soch." [Complete Works], Vol 36, p 300.
3. Brezhnev, L. I. "Report of the Central Committee of CPSU and j
Next Tasks for the Party in the Field of Domestic and Foreign Policy," '
Moscow, Politizdat, 1976, p i~7.
4. Idem, "Soviet Trade Unions: an Influential Force of Our Society," in
"Materialy XVI S"yezda professional'nykh soyuzov SSSR" [Proceedings of
16th Congress of USSR Trade UnionsJ, Moscow, Profizdat, 1977, p 18.
5. Anan'yev, B. G. "Problems of Modern Human Sciences~~~ Moscow,
Nauka, 1977, p 13.
6. Antosenkov, Ye. Ye., and Kupriyanova, Z. V. "Trends in Worker
Turnover. Dynamic Aspect of Analysis," Novosibirsk, Siberian Branch
of Nauka, 1977, p 84. ~
7. Blanchard, F. "For More Humane Working Conditions. Working Conditions
and the Industrial Environment," "60th Session of International Labor
Conference," 1975, International Labor Office, Geneva, 1975, pp 32-33.
8. Vaysman, R. S. "Link Between Interpersonal Relations and Group
Efficiency of Performance," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII [Problems of
Psychology], No 4, 1977.
9. Venda, V.; Zinchenko, V.; and Munipov, V. "Projective Ergonomics,"
TEKHNICHESKAYA ESTETIKA [Esthetic Design in Engineering], No 8, 1970.
- 31
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
10. Zinchenko, V. P., and Panov, D. Yu. "Key Problems of En~ineerin~
Psychology," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 5, 1962.
11. Zinchenko, V. P., and Munipov, V. M. "Methodological Problems of
Ergonomics," Moscow Znaniye, 1974.
12. Zinchenko, V. P.; Munipov, V. M.; and Smolyan, G. L. "Ergonomic
Bases of Organization of Labor," Moscow, Ekonomika, 1974.
13. 7.inchen;co, V. P., and Munipov, V. M. "On Theory of Ergonomics,"
TEKHNICHESKAYA ESTETIKA, No 6, 1977.
14. Izmerov, N. F., and Letavet, A. A. "Decisions of the 25th CPSU
Congress and Tasks for Industrial Hygiene," GIGIYENA TRUDA I
PROFESSIONAL'NYYE ZABOLEVANIYA [Industrial Hygiene and Occupational
Diseases], No 2, 1976, p 2.
15. "Engineering Psychology. Theory, Methodology and Practical
Applications," Moscow, Nauka, 1977, p 6.
16. "Interview With Conference Participants," TEI~INICHESKAYA ESTETIKA,
No 12, 1972, p 14.
17. Kokarev, N. P. "Industrial Hygiene," Moscow, Profizdat, 1973, p Z41.
18. Krotkov, F. G. "Gigiyena," BSE [Great Soviet Encyclopedia], 3d -
edition, Vol 6y 1971, p 458.
19. Kuz'min, V~ P. "Systems Principles in Theory and Methodology of
K. Marx," Moscow, Politizdat, 1976.
20. Lapin, N. I.; Korzheva, E. M.; and Naumova, N. F. "Theory and ~
Practice of Social Planning," Moscow, Politizdat, 1975, p 166.
21. Leont'yev, A.; Lomov, B.; and Kuz'min, V. "Psychology and Scientific
and Technological Progress," KOMMUNIST [Communist], No 11, 1971.
22. Leont'yev, A. N. "Activity. Consciousness. Personality," Moscow,
Politizdat, 1975, p 92.
23. Lomov, B. F. "Ergonomics and Scientific Organization of Labor,"
SOTSIALISTICHESKIY TRUD [Socialist Labor], No 8, 1969.
24. Medvedev, V. I. "Theoretical Problems of Industrial Physiology,"
FIZIOLOGIYA CHELOVEKA [Human Physiology], Vol 1, No 1, 1975, p 27.
25. "Methodological Problems of Determining the Socioeconomic Effective-
ness of New Technology," Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
32
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
26. "Methodological Problems of Ergonomics," "Proceedings of First Inter-
national Conference of Scientists and Specialists in Ergonomics of
CEMA Member Nations and Socialist Federated Republic of Yugoslavia,"
Moscow, Izd. VNIITE [All-Union Scientific Research Institute of
Aesthetic Styling in Engineering], 1972.
27. Krylov, A. A. (editor) "Methodology of Research in Engineering
Psychology and Industrial Psychology," Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad
University, Pt 1, 1974.
28. Monmollen, M. "Man-Machine Systems," translated from French, Moscow,
Mir, 1973.
29. Munipov. V. "Ergonomic Bases for Aesthetic Design," TEKHNICHESKAYA
ESTETIKA, Na 10, 1964.
30. Munipov, V. "Ergonomics and Aesthetic Styling in Engineering,"
Ibid, No 7, 1969.
31. Munipov, V. M. "Design [Styling] and Science," VOPROSY FILOSOFII
[Problems of Philosophy], No 9, 1976.
32. Idem, "Ergonomics and Scientific Organization of Labor," in "Trudy
VNIITE, Ergonomika" [Works of the All-Union Scientific Research
Institute of Aesthetic Styling in Engineering, Ergonomics], Moscow,
vyp 9, 1976.
33. Idem, "Ergonomics and Psychology," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 5, 1976.
34. Novozhilov, S. S. (editor) "Scientific Organization of Labor in
Industry," Moscow, Ekonomika, 1978.
35. Petrovskiy, A. V. "Sociopsychological Conception of Group Activity,"
VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 5, 1973.
36. Platonov, K. K. "The Place of Industrial Psychology in the System
of Disciplines Dealing With Labor," in "Metodologicheskiye prohlemy.
ergonomiki. Materialy I Mezhdunarodnoy konferentsii uchenykh i
spetsialistov stran-chlenov SEV i SFRYu po voprosam ergonomiki"
[Methodological Problems of Ergonomics. Proceedings of First Inter-
_ national Conference of Scientists and Specialists in Ergonomics
of CEMA Member nations and Socialist ~'ederated Republic of Yugoslavia],
Moscow, Izd. VNIITE, 1972.
37. Podzhivatov, V. P. "The Rol~ of Progressive Work Methods in
Scientific Organization of Labor," Moscow, Ekonomika, 1972, p 9.
33
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE UNLY
38. Ponomarev, Ya. A. "The Question of Studying the Psychological
Mechanism of 'Decision Making' Referable to Creative Tasks," in
"Probemy prinyatiya resheniya" [Problems of Decision Making], Moscow,
Nauka, 1976, p 90,
39. Smyshlyayeva, L. "An Important Means of Intensifying Production,"
KOMMUNIST, No 5, 1976, p 22.
40. Suslov, V. Ya. "Labor Under Fully Developed Socialism," Leningrad,
Nauka, 1976, p 55.
41. Tochilov, K. S. "The Place of Industrial Physiology as a Science,"
in "Fiziologiy~ truda (Tezisy dokladov VI Vsesoyuznoy nauchnoy
konf erentsii po fiziologii truda)" [Industrial Physiology (Summaries
of Papers Delivered at the 6th All-Union Scientific Conference on
Industrial Physiology)), Moscow, 1973, pp 351-352.
42. Tugarinov, V. P., and Parygin, B. D. "Correlation Between Social
and Psychological Factors," FILOSOFSK'tYE NAUKI [Philosophical
Sciences], No 6, 1967.
43. "To Strengthen the Correlation Between Social, Natural and Engineer-
ing Sciences," KOI~II~IiTNIST, No 1, 1977.
44. "Man--~ience--Technology (Marxist Analysis of the Scientific and
Technological Revolution)," Moscow, Politizdat, 1973, pp 152-153.
45. Chernikov, G. P. "Political Economy and Psychology," VOPROSY
FILOSOFII, No 2, 1972.
46. 5cherrere, J. "Industrial Phy~iology (Ergonomics)," translated fram
French, Moscow, Meditsina, 1973, ~
47. Shcherbak, F. V. "Incentives for Work (Methodological Aspect),"
Leningrad, Izd. Leningrad University, 1976, p 66.
48. Bures, Z. "Industlial Psychology and Its Benefits," Prague,
Prace, 1973.
49. Chapanis, A. A. "Relevance of Physiological and Psychological
Criteria to Man-Machine Systems: the Present State of the Art,"
ERGONOMICS, Vol 13, No 3, 1970.
50. Idem, "Engineering Psycholog~," in "Handbook of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology," Marvin D. Dunnette, editor; Copyright 1976
by Rand McNally College Publishing Co.
51. Faverge, J. M. "Ergonomics as Seen by Ergonomists," LE TRAVAIL HUMAIN
- [Human Work], Vol 39, 1976, p 2.
34
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02148: CIA-RDP82-00850R040240090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
52. Hunt, D. P.; Howell, W. C.; and Roscoe, S. N. "Educational Programs
for Engineering Psychologist: That Depends a Good Deal on WherP
You Want to Get to," HUMAN FACTORS, No 14(1), 1972.
53. Meister, D. "Human Factors: Theory and Practice," New York, 1971.
54. Murrel, K. F. N. "Ergonomics," London, 1965.
35 '
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOg OFFICIAL USE ONLY
CNAPTER III. PRINCIP LES AND METHODS OF ERGONOMICS
'Cfie methodology for complex study of man under specific conditions of
activity with the use of machines (technological means), including the
mel-hodology of ergonomics, is developed on the basis of the philosophy of
dialectical materialism, which conforms with the spirit and trends of
- clevelopment of modern scientific knowledge. One can distinguish three
types of inethodological means in ergonomics corresponding to the three
levels of ineChodological analysis singled out in the literature dealing
with philosophy (77]: 1) methodological means of a general philosophical
nature; 2) general scientific methodological means; 3) special scientific
means.
I. Methodological Means of Ergonomics -
Mc~tt~odological means refers to some knok*ledge or other, but considered in
n S~~ec:ial role or function: the function of principle, method, procedure,
u~nnner. o~ obtaining new knowledge. In research practice, methodological
memis do not emerge in their "pure" form; rather, they are organically
Ul urided, inc].uded in the relevent ergonomic conceptions. The ergonomic .
- metl~odological means can be interpreted by analogy with what is defined
i.n psycliology a~ general principles of studying mental phenomena,
prirticularly since they are part of the methodological arsenal ot ergonomics,
~ind this applies f irst of all to the principle of unity of consciousness
anci aclivity adop~ed in Soviet psychology. In ergonomic knowledge, the
shlrc of the p.hilosophical ["world outlook"] cor,~onent is rather large,
by virtuc.~ of the direct orientation toward man and the industrial-practical `
orientation of ergonamic studies (even if emphasis is not laid on this
component in some specific instance), Ultimately, any change in the work
process, working cond itions, tools and products has certain socioeconomic
goal.s; in modern society it always serves specific class interests, which
are different for socialist and capitalistic methods of production.
7'he phil.osophical component of ergonomics is reflected the most fully in
the ar~a of goals of this discipline, in studies of the histor.y of incep-
tlon of the subject of ergonomics and its interpretation of the centr.al
c:~ltegory, ttie category of object-oriented activity. '
36
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
We have already indicated that the general goal of ergonomics is formulated
as the unity of three aspects of research and planning: to increase the
efficiency of activity and, accordingly, operation of man-machine systems,
safeguard health and develop the personality of people involved in the
work process. This conception is based on Marxist-Leninist interpretation
o� the role of labor in a fully developed socialist society, under con-
ditions where labor is changing from a means of sustaining life to a
first and foremost necessity, the main prerequisite for development of
man's skills and creativity.
Acceptance of the thesisof threefold nature of the general purpose of
ergonomics makes it posaible to avoid both a narrow-minded practicism
and rift between ergonomic research and the concrete tasks of developing
socialist industry. This specifies the unity of ergonomic studies,
their systemic nature. Of course, there may be prevalence of a given
aspect in a concrete study, this is permissible and virtually inevitable.
- However, the general and single goal is reached through the aggregate
and mutual complementarity of these aspects.
The next basic theoretical thesis of ergonomics, which is organically
~ linked with the one ~ust discussed and which serves an equally important
philosophical f~inction, is the indication that the "man-machine"
relation is pri~marily a"sub~ect of labor-tool of labor" relation. It
is not man wha is viewed as a simple element contained in a technological
system, but the machine that is viewed as a means included in man~s
' activity [42].
Of immediate significance to ergonomics as a discipline that is directed
toward the study and planning of work activity is analysis of the
category of object-oriented activity, distinction of conceptual schemes
- in which this category is used and studied by various scientific discip-
lines and, first of all, psychology [39]. These conceptual schemes,
which define the ergonomic aspect of conceptions of activity, serve a
methodological function in relation to concrete ergonomic studies. This
function consists of formulation of general philosophical sets through
the methodological means of ergonomics of both the general scientific
and special scientific type. We must mention the philosophical nature
of the theoretical theses of ergonomics that are related to the study of
the emergence of the sub~ect of this scientific discipline. They include,
first of all, the thesis that appearance of ergonomics is attributable to
radical changes in the work process proper, which are related to the
scientific and technological revolution, as well as the thesis of three
stxges in determination of the nature of the rela~ionship b~tween the human
and machine components when solving the corresponding scientific and
industrial problems [3].
At the f irst stage, this link was interpreted as man's adjustment to a
machine, at the second as the machine's adjustment to man (his psychological,
37
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY _
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
physiological, anthropometric, etc., characteristics). Finally, the third
stage is characterized by broader interpretation of the "hwnan factor," as
a factor that forms (makes up) the "man-a~achine" system itself. The dis-
tinction of the stages and definition of the specifics of the latter also
serve as justification of the general go=.ils of ergonomics, which are
interpreted in the conceptiona of its sub~ect. The research scheme is
broken down into details, from a brief indication of two "man-machine"
components to a total "man-group of people-machine-ob~ect of labor-
industrial environment" system. But the important aspects include more
than thia breakdown and expansion of number of MMS components that are
included in the ar~a of ergonomic research. Also important is the start
of overcoming the adaptation-homeostatic approach to the problem of inter-
action between man and machine in two variants thereof:~ mechanocentric
and anthropocentirc.
The methodological content of the theoretical theses of ergonomic~ that
were formed in the course of processing of general scientific conceptions
as they apply to its sub~ect consists of both a special type of vision
of the objects of study and planning a1d combined specification of the
object and means of studying it, i.e., construction of a systems strategy
of ergonomic research. "General scientific" refers to the concepts and
conceptions that are not rigidly related to a given area of scientific
knowledge, but at the same time do not have the status of philvsophical
categories.
- The concepts of the systems approach in the broad sense--as one of the
leading current general scientific trends--determine many of the initial
sets and theoretical theses of ergonomics. They include the following:
integral consideration of man-machine systems, dynamic-systemic view of
their structure, inclusion of man's work in the sub~ect of scientific
- examination, tendency toward scientific synthesis of various aspects of
research, striving to demonstrate the possible consequences of man's
activity. For example, the typical ergonomic interpretation of the
complex and integral nature of MMS's includes a new aspect for technology:
the influence of the human factor. For this reason, there is an overt -
tendency tdward the use of systemic [systems analysis] methods, i.e., a
definite turn toward systemic orientation [41, 42, 55].
Use of the principles of the systemic approach permits different formula-
tion of many research problems. This can be seen particularly well on the
example of how the orientation of research on human actions is changing.
While the early studies of work activity were characterized by inter-
pretation thereof as a process and the main tasks were to single out and
define the stages of this proc~ss, within the framework of systemic
orientation the scheme of the process is no longer the initial and basic
factor. Now, Che structure of activity is in the center of attention,
and this implies a different interpretation of action itself as a multi- `
component formation, each of the components of which has its own functions
38
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
within the same action (i.e., the action construed as a system). This
requires consideration of various systems of action, distinction of
levels thereof, etc. [3].
As we see, in describing the substance of the reorientation that has
occurred in ergonomics, we actually have to operate with the entire
basic "paradigm" of the systemlc conception of objects (concepts of
structure, elemer~t, functions, levels, etc.).
The important theses of ergonomics that characterize its use of general
scientific conceptions is that one can arbitrarily give the name of
"organismic" to the conception of activity, its likening to a func-
tional organ [element]. This interpretation was first expounded by N. A.
Bernshteyn in relation to a motor act [10].
Also of inethodological significance are several theoretical theses, -
expounded either directly in ergonomics or in allied scientific discip-
lines, which have become an organic element of ergonomics. They include:
differentiation between corrective and pro~ective ergonomics, disclosure -
of the content of the concept of the human factor in engineering;
formulation of the problem of complex study and planning of exogenous
ways and means of activity; hypothesis of hierarchic organization of
operator activity with distinction of systemic and dynamic [operational]-
psychological levles; definition of general psychological schemes of
activity with introduction of functional units [blocks] as special
- elements; development of inethods of microstructural and microgenetic
~ analysis; hypothesis of dynamic [operational] image; conception of
"inclusion" [or switching on]; structural-heuristic approach to the
study of information processes, 3ncluding decision making procedures;
mathematical theory of construction of functional structures of man-
machine systems, and a number of other theses of "systems analytical"
ergonomics.
2. General Dascription of Ergonomic Research and Research Methods
The systemic [systems analysis] approach is the methodological foundation
of ergonomics. On its basis, it is also possible to use in ergonomic
resear~h the methods of various aciences, on the boundary of which
qualitatively new problems of research on MMS arise and are solved.
There is some transformation of the methods used, which leads to the
development of new res~arch procedures.
- In ergo~nomics, research methods are used that were formed in sociology, ,
industrial psychology, physiology and hygiene, functional anatomy, cyber-
netics, systems analysis, etc. The main problem is to coordinate various
procedures to solve a given ergonomic problem and synthesize the results
obtained with Chem.
39
FOR OFFICIAL USE QNLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
There are specific features in the ergonomic approach to the study and
optimization of activity. With regard to methods, this is expressed in
the following basic theses: In the first place, the orientation of ergo-
nomics toward planning activity and its components requires the use not
only of experimental but a priori planning methods. In the second place,
the use of generalized indicators of activity, tension and convenience
[comfortJ of work in ergonomics i~aplies the use of procedures for ob-
taining integral criteria on the basis of a system of special indices.
In the third place, an ergonomic study or evaluation must always be
systemic, and this is feasible only by the concurrent use of various
methods reflecting correlations between components and the main properties
of the MMS.
A certain strategy for the choice of inethods to solve specific ergonomic
problems emerges from the above method-related diatinctions.
The research methods of ergonomics can be arbitrarily divided into two
groups: analytical (or descriptive) and experimental. In m st studies
they are closely interwoven and used concurrently, supplementing and
enriching one another. -
~ Virtually any ergonomic problem arises as a result of reformulation of
actual tasks. For this reasan, each practical tasks put to ergonomics
must be first analyzed from the standpoint of demonstration of the
specifics of the influence of the human factor under specified conditions.
A mandatory prerequisite for the professional work of a specialist in
the field of ergonomics is the ability to competently analyze industrial
activity (labor productivity, progressive knowhow, working conditions,
flaws, personnel turnover, typical erroneous actions of working people,
trawnatism, etc.).
Any ergonomic study ~ust start with analysis of man's performance and
function of the MMS. Its objective is to define man's place in solving
problems for which the system studied is intended, general psychophysio-
logical description of man's performance in this system, demonstration
of the structure of human factors that affect the efficienc.y of the system
as a whole and its parts.
The purpose of such analysis can vary, depending on the specific problem
involved. If one has to conduct experimental studies, analysis is needed
chiefly to choose an equivalent model of activity or separate typical
actions, as well as to define the specific objectives of the experiment.
If one needs to make an expert evaluation of the NI~IS, the purpose of
_ analysis will be to demonstrate the system's components, according to
which the ergonomic evaluation must be made. When elaborating the criteria
and methods for occupational screening, analysis will be directed toward
detection of personality traits that have a substantial influence on
performance quality.
40 '
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Refinement of machine (technical means) design in order to have it take
the utmost consideration of the capahilities and distinctions of working
man requires, in the first place, exact knowledge about the causes for
dissatisfaction with the existing design from the ergonomic point of view;
in the second place, it requires a clear idea about the direction in _
which it should be altered. Answers to these questions can be obtained
provided the flaws in organization of interaction between man and machine
are detected in the course of preliminary analysis and requirements are
defined with regard to technological means and psychophysiological
traits of man needed for a given form of work. In the ideal case,
answers to basic ergonomic questions of refining an existing technolo-
gical means and planning a new one should be the result of the analytical
stage.
At the analytical stage of ergonomic research, many modern methods of
cLsign analysis, which have been well summarized by the English scientist
J. K. Jones [26], are found to be useful. Most of them are the result of
"spontaneous" psychological analysis (made by engineers or pro~ect
administrators) of the ~aork of the most talented designers and entire
groups which have made o,itstanding achievements in developing modern
technological equipment [means].
Preliminary functional and structural analysis of activity serves to sub-
stantiate the goaZ to which subsequent ergonomic research is directed. Not
infrequently, reference to the prevailing opinion about the significance
of a specific problem leads to setting false goals that are not warranted _
by the actual state of affairs. The t~srn to the experimental level of
analysis is fruitful only when Che foundation for the experiment has been
prepared by a detailed description of the entire set of factors having a
direct or indirect bearing on the ergonomic problem under study.
Setting up an experiment implies testing the validity of an expounded
hypothesis or system of hypotheses, which reflect in ergonomics certain
conceptions of the nature of relation and interaction of a certain group
of factors in each individual instance. Hypotheses should be expounded
and substantiated in the course of preliminary analyiss of the problem.
- The problems formulated in the course of preliminary analysis are solved
in the course of an experimental study. There are diverse types of experi-
mental situations and concrete procedures used in ergonomic practice. The
concreCe content of some of them will be discussed in detail below. It
should be streased that ergonomic expertmentation has a number of features
that distinguishes it substantially from studies on the analytical level
and traditional laboratory experiments.
First of all, the use of the experimental method is directed toward demon-
stration of the distinctions of organization of interaction between man
and machine that are not directly demonstrable thraugh analysis. Under
41
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
ordinary industrial conditions, rather low demands are often made of man,
and he can readily compensate for the existing flaws of a technical
device. In this regard, an important procedure is to complicate the
work (posing additional problems, simulation of an accident situation,
etc.) as an effective means of demonstrating the advantages of one of
several variants of the technological means in comparative studies [27].
Performance of a second (or additional) task concurrently with the main
work subject to evaluation is used to record the spare time, which refers
to the extra time (over and abovethe required minimum) that the operator
may have to prevent deviations of the controlled parameter beyond a
permissible range [45]. In turn, the amount of spare time, which
changes in accordance with the level of mobilization of the (human)
- operator, serves as one of the prognostic indicators, on the basis of
which one predicts the degree of complexity of work with which the
reliability of performance of the operator diminishes drastically [66].
When organizing experimental ergonomic studies, one must take into con-
sideration that the presence of the experimenter, his set and expecta-
tions influence the results of the sub3ects' performance. It is not by
chance that the problem of "ecological validity" of laboratory research
(possibility of projecting laboratory results to "real life" situations),
which was originally formulated in the field of sociopsychological re-
search, also became the obj ect of the closest scrutiny of ergonomics.
It is impossible to directly extrapolate data obtained in the laboratory
to real situations because, in the farmer case, the subjects are
acting under the influence of specific motivation, which loses its
force the moment the subject leaves the laboratory. For example, an
individual agrees to participate in an experiment in the hope of receiv-
ing a competent evaluation of his skills, but this motivation is not
necessarily important to his professional work. Other motivations that
prompt and control the performance of subjects in a laboratory experi-
ment may be the desire to help science or simply to be rewarded. Of
course, these motivations are not exclusively specific to laboratory
test situations, but one cannot overlook the fact that similar activity
in the laboratory and "real life" situations may be determined by differ-
ent motives. Since the nature of motivation is the decisive factor in
regulating performance, generalization of the results of laboratory
studies without consideration of the specifics of the motivation factor
often leads to misunderstandings. For example, use of feedback for
evaluation purposes w3thou~ reinforcement by~tangible incentives did not
yield the expected effect under actual industrial conditions (on the
Pulsar program implemented at one of the plants in L'vov), although
laboratory tests demonstrated that this factor had a significant influence
on efficiency of work. Evidently, feedback for evaluation purposes had
a regulatory effect on sub~ ect performance ~.n the laboratory when there
was a strong enough motivation background formed. in the experiment. ,
Evaluating feeciback per se, without appropriate motivation, has no
42
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL tJSE ONLY
appreciable effect. Extrapolation of the results of such experiments to
real industrial situations can be made only after proper and comprehensive
interpretation of the data obtained, including consideration of the
specifics of subjects' motivation.
The restrictions that laboratory conditions impose are the reason for the
ever increasing interest of researchers in conducting experiments directly
in industry. However, even these experiments are not without flaws. Some
of them are related to the influence of sociopsychological factors. Since
the logic of experimental analysis requires the comparison of task
performance in the presence and absence of effect of an independent
variable (hypothetical cause of expected effect), an experimental and
control group of sub~ects are genera lly used in experimental studies.
Under industrial conditions, it is difficult to isolate one group of
people from another. As a result, the control group may compete with the
experimental one and, as a consequence, there is masking of the influence
of the tested factor (such phenomena were observed in the well-kno~n.i
Hawthorne experiments). Another situation could ocGUr: the performance -
of the control group may worsen because its members will feel that they
are injured by the absence of the innovations (which are usually att.ractive)
that alter the working conditions in the experimental group. Awareness
of the existence of such factors as competition or demoralization of the
control group helps avoid hasty conclusions based on a superficial com-
parison of the results for control and experimental groups.
All of the foregoing is indicative of the danger of underestimating the
role of interaction between the researcher and object studied, and of tae
need to work out the sociopsychological back-up for studies of work
performance.
To this time, there is no clearcut classification of ergonomic investi-
gative methods. The difficulty of developing such a classification is
related to the fact that it must cover all areas of ergonomic research,
which have not yet been definitively formed and continue to expand quite
rapidly. The problem of classification of inethods in ergonomics is ana-
logous to the one that B. G. Anan'yev encountered when he tried to
create a tentative classification of inethods used in studies pertaining
tu modern human sciences [4J.
? A certain modification of the above classification is also applicable to
ergonomics.
The methods contained in the first group are arbitrarily called organiza-
tional. They include, first of all, the system of inethodological means
that provide for a complex approach to research. The complex approach is
used throughout the entire interdisciplinary study, while its effective-
ness is determined by the end results thereof. The procedural basis of
complex research is only beginning to be developed. The typical feature
43
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
of interdisciplinary research is not the synthesis of results obtained on
the basis of independent studies, but organization of a study in the course
of which there is synthesis of the conceptions of different disciplines
[2J. "The ~rogram of a complex interdisciplinary study is determined by
the common ab~ect under study and separation of functions of different
disciplines, periodic compilation of data and generalization thereof,
mainly those pertaining to relations and correlations between diverse
phenomena...." [4, p 302].
In spite of the fact that the specific routes of solving such problems
have not been worked out, their very formulation has a positive effect
on concrete ergonomic studies, calling the attention of ergonomists to
problems of development of adequate research strategy and means of
- performing it.
The second group of inethods consists of the existing empirical methods of
obtaining scientific data. This group includes observation and self-
observation, experimental techniques (laboratory, iiidustrial, "formative"
experiment), diagnostic methods (diverse tests, questionnaires of the
contemporary types, sociometry, interviews and talks); analysis of
processes and products of work (chronometry, cyclography, professio-
graphic description, work method, evaluation of products, etc.); modeling
(of objects, mathematica], cybernetic, etc.).
The third group consists of data processing procedures. They include
various methods of quantitative and qualitative description of data.
Finally, the fourth group refers to various methods of interpreting the
obtained data within the context of an integral description of the
performance of MMS's.
The second group of inethods is the most extensive and developed; one
can single out a number of concrete procedures within this group,
- depending on the ob~ectives and nature of the studies.
Experimental methods of studying the dynamics of various physiological
functions are used in ergonomics [22]. The typical feature thereof is
the wide use of electrophysiological methods: 1) electroencephalography
(EEG), i.e., recording the electrical activity of the brain, which yields
a number of characteristics of activity of neuronal ensembles of the
a brain under natural conditions; electromyography (EMG), i.e., recording
the action potnential of muscles, which is a sensitive indicator of
involvement in a dynamic relation or s~atic work of specific muscle groups
and plays an important role in evaluating muscle tone (essential in
studies of positions and working motions); recording of galvanic skin
reactions (GSR), the change in cutaneous potential difference that is
a very fine indicator of man's emotional state; electrocardiography (EKG),
- i.e., recording of electrical activity of the heart, which is a reliable
44
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
indicator of the state of the cardiovascular system; electrooculography
(EOG), i.e., recording of the potential that occurs when the eyeball
turns, which is used as an objective indicator of shifting of the
eyes when examining some ob~ect. The recording of bioelectrical processes
permits determination and quantitative evaluation of functional
changes in the human body, that are difficult to detect upon direct
obaervation and that occur under the influence of the most diver.se
changes in the environment.
The method of complex recording of psychophysiological functions, which is
also called the polyeffector method, is used in ergonomics to study
types of human performance that differ in. content and complexity. This
method gained more popularity in the study of functional states of man.
The value of this method lies in the possibility of simultaneous record-
- ing of many psychophysiological parameCers, which yields an integral
idea about the work of the main functional systems of the body.
The use of biomechanical methods is also included in ergonomic research:
high-speed photography, cyclography, cinecyclography, electric tensiometry--
change in electric properties of sensors applied.to parts of equipment
deformed by man, electric recording of inechanical parameters by means
of angular displacement sensors, bearl.ng [reference] dynamographs and
others [9, 28]. With the use thereof, man's motor activity is described
from the standpoint of efficiency of various elements of the skeleto-
muscular system.
Wide use is made in ergonomics of inethods of describing microclimate
conditions--temperature, humidity, etc., methods of ineasuring and
evaluating the intensity of radiation in the radio �requency range,
methods of ineasuring noise level and frequency composition, methods of
measuring and evaluating vibration, methods of assaying dust content of
air, methods of assaying toxic substances in air, methods of ineasuring
light and other methods of industrial hygiene [65] in order to study the
conditions of man's industrial work.
The technique of anthropometric studies is used to solve various ergonomic
problems [30]. Wide applications have been found for somatography--
_ technical and anthropological analysi~ of body position and change in
work posture of man, correlation between the size of man and machine. Ti~e
results of such analysis are usually given in the form of graphs. Somato-
graphy permits calculation of the zone of easy and optimum range,
optimum methods of organizing the work place with consideration of the
proportional correlations between elements of equipment and man.
The essenre af ~ivnamic structural description of work activity, which
is often cailed algorithmic analysis, consists af breaking down the
work into qualitatively different elements, determinat3on of the logical
link between them, order in which they follow one another and calculation
45
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
rOR OT'FICIAL USE ONLY
of a number of parameters that have a certain psychophysiological meaning
[33]. Methods of systems-sCructural analysis of performance are important
- to ergonomicsy in particular, the special methods of functional-structural
and microstructural analysis [38, 39].
The armamentarium of inethods of ergonomics includes many psychophysiological
methods: measurement of reaction time, which has many variants (simple
sensory and motor reactions, reaction of choice, reactions to moving
objects, etc.); psychophysical methods (det.ermination of thresholds and
dynamics of sensitivity in various modalities); psychometric methods of
examining perceptive, mnemic, cognitive processes and personality traits.
The sociometric methods of studying interpersonal r~lations, which are used
in ergonomics, permit solving a number of pressing problems: establishing
the existence of a preference or ~et expressed by an individual with
regard to other members of a group or team ~n specific situations,
describing the individual's place in the group as the subject sees it
and comparing this to the reactions of other members of the group,
expressing the correlations within compared groups by means of forma~
methods [56J. One of the most widespread methods of studying the compa-
tibility of inembers of small groups is the homeostatic method, which has
found applications in planning dynamic [operational] group performance [23].
The performance of ergonomic research requires development of adequate
methods for making a quantitative evaluation of product quality. For
~his purpose, nonmetric and metric scaling is used, i.e., measurement
that is construed as "putting in order a set of properties of real
oY,jects (object-related area) in relation to a set of signs (model
area) by means of the ordering rule (f), which permits isomorphic
representation of elements and relations between them in the object-
related area by elements and relations between them in. the mc~del area"
[61, p 154]. A formal model is obtained as a result of scaling, i.e.,
a scale that can be used as an analytical tool.
A distinction is md::c. between nominal, sequential, interval and propor-
tional scales. The nominal scale is based on ai.tributation of signs
to objects. It is constructed as a classification of objects studied
according to presence or absence of a specific sign [tag] within the
range of two or more observation categories (scale [dimensionality] of
the tag). When plotting a sequential scale, in addition to equality
and inequality, order is considered, i.e., the intensity of manifesta-
tion of the tag to be defined. The scale of intervals, which has all
of the listed properties of the above-described scales, is characterized
by the fact that the distance between two points of the continuum can
be exactly specified and, for this reason, it is possible to evaluate
the size of the intervals (Celsius temperature scale, standardized
- scales for psychological tests, etc.). All of the properties of the
above scales ~re inherent in proportional scales. In addition, they have
46
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
� a natural or absolute zero point, for which reason one can compare the sums,
differences, products and quotients, as well as determine the correlation
betwe2n sc ale evaluations.
~
Rating scales have applications in ergonomics, and they serve as an ancil-
lary means when making judgments about the degree of expression of a tag
of some phenomenon or other. "Rating is a measurement, if the la tter is
construed as a comparison of the quantitative aspect of a tag to a certain
gage. Rating scales should also be considered as such a gage, if differ-
ent judges reliably derive largely coinciding evaluations and opinions of
the same tag" [61, p 211]. The special significance of rating sc ales to
ergonomics is that it is easier to derive a conclusion with their help
about the intensity of complex tags. The method of expert evalua tion is
used in ergonomics to evaluate the arrangement of control consoles,
data displays and other objects [68], when it is difficult to use esti-
mation and experimental methods.
Ergonomics also uses and is instrumental in further development of inethods
of cybernetics, which includes in the optimization concept the requirement
of optimum human control of performance in complex MMS's [12, 87, 88].
There is much in common between ergonomic and bionic methods. Methods of
preparing quantitatively substantiated recommendations on optimum decision
making are alsoincluded in the armamentarium of ergonomics. Use of
methods of automatic monitoriflg theory, information theory, queueing
theory and others in the study and design of MMS's makes it necessary to
devote increasing attention to substantiation of applicability of
approaches, demonstration of their basic capabilities and equivalence to _
the specif ics of the problems to be solved [64].
Use of the methods of systems analysis, which are related to macrodesign
of complex systems, i.e., choice and organization of functions and structure
as a whole, is largely involved in causing the formation of ergonomics as
a planning [design] discipline [1, 87, 88]. General methodological
approaches are being developed for solving the main tasks of ergonomic
planning, which is an element of overall planning [or design] of the MMS,
the objective of which is to substantiate the ergonomic specifications,
implementation thereof in the form of properties of the planned system
and exper t evaluation of the results of designs [14, 24, 29, 35, 41, 42].
Rational economic planning is based on planning the performance of man
(or group of people), development of the methods of which constitutes one
of the mo st diff icult tasks. They can be fulf illed if a system of inethods
is created, since none of the methods is universal and suitable for analysis
of all aspects of the problem of planning activity. The mutual comple-
mentarity and interaction of the methods does not minimize the need to
determine which method is the leading one, depending on the obje ctives
- and tasks of specific studies [17].
As we have shown above, the methods used in ergonomics are diverse, and
it is no t deemed possible to provide a detailed description of each of them,
47
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
even in a special. textbook. Special attenCion will be given to methods of
observation and interrogation, studies of man's executory and cognitive
activity, evaluation of functional states, modeling and use of computers
in ergonomic research.
3. Methods of Observation and Interrogation
Observation refers to the purposeful, organized and systematized considera-
tion of the object under study. It is also very important to clearly
record the results of observations, in order to reproduce them and use
other forms of verif ication. Observation methods have undergone signifi-
cant change in recent times because of the~use of various recording and
other equipment (pho tographic, cinematographic, acoustics, television).
At the same time, greater requirements are being made of sophistication
of observation, primarily from the standpuint of proper formulation of
the objective of observation, accuracy thereof and broadness of considera-
tion of the phenomenon described.
In ergonomics, observation is often an element of an experimental study.
Organization of observation involves solving the following problems:
a) definition of the task and objective of observation; b) choice of
- object, sub3ect and situation; c) choice of inethod of observation that
has the least influence on the ob3ect studied and assures gathering the
required information; d) choice of inethod of recording the abserved
phenomenon; e) processing and interpretstion of the obtained informa-
tion [78]. When organizing observations it is imperative to take into _
consideration the fact that the presence of an observer has an appreci-
able influence on performance. One can describe work performance in
sufficient detaii by means oi the observation method, supplemented with
time and motion studies of all operations in order of their performance.
In prof~:ssiographic studies, a preconceived and prepared observation
scheme is of particular importance. We submit below two observation
sch?~nes for the purpcse of analysis of the work place, working position
and working motions. The first was compiled to study the performance
of dispatchers and the second for that of coil winder operators. The
work of dispatchers was studied for the purpose of preparing the ergonomic
specifications for the desk chair, and that of the winder operators
in order to reorganize the entire work place [74].
Observation Scheme No 1 -
1. Specifics of dispatcher work according to involvement of
sense organs and parts of the body (hands, legs, vision,
hearing) involved in the work.
2. Specifics of body position: fixed pose, mobility in
relation to seat, console.
48
FOR OFFICIAL IISE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
3. Angle of inclination of the body: negative (forward), straight,
~ positive (backward).
4. Height of trunk support point (back) abov~ seat.
5. Height of console and angle of inclination of working panel.
6. Work of the hands (right, left, both).
7. Hand position in range of reach: according to depth (maximum, _
near) and width (on the right and left).
8. Conformity of console dimensions to areas of easy or optimum
reach of motor field of work place. ,
~
9. Hand support points (elbows, forearm).
10. Position of legs (right, left). -
11. Conformity of console size with zones of easy or optimum reach.
12. Determination of suppor t part cf seat: front, middle, rear.
13. Possibility of rest beyond the work zone. .
Observation Scheme No 2 _
1. Specifics of work according to involv~ment of sense organs and .
parts of the body (hands, feet, vision, hearing).
2. Specifics of body position: fixed pose, mobility in relation
to seat and bench.
3. Angle of inclination of the body: negative (forwa�rd), straight,
positive (backward).
4. Angle of inclination of the head: negative, straight, positive.
5. Involvement of hands: constant (right, left, both), periodic
(right, lef t, both).
6. Hand position in the zone within reach: depth (maximum, near), !
and width (on the right and left). -
7. Conformity of console size to areas of easy or optimum reach
- of motor field of work place.
8. Involvement of legs: working (right, left), support (right, left). -
9. Position of working leg during period of no action: on pedal, -
floor, footrest, no support.
10. Position of supporting leg: on the floor, foorrest, pedal, no
support.
11. Conformity of bench size to areas of easy or optimum leg reach,
12. Use of footrest (yes, not; for right, left, both feet). -
, 13. Distance between mean axillary line to mounting (transverse axis
of bench).
14. Determination of supporting part of seat: front, middle, rear.
15. Attitude of worker [female] to improvements made in chair
(positive, negative).
The results of observation can be recorded in the form of a table, to
which instructions are attached for filling it out.
49
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The interrogation method is used extensively to gather information about the
structure of the work process, nature thereof and the individual's attitude ~
toward his work.
The interrogation can be regulated, i.e., there is prior preparation of
questions that are the same for all subjects and listed in a strictly
specified order, or unregulated, in the form of conversation with the
subjects, adhering to the general plan.
The conversation method requires certain skills and even talent. It is
~ recommended for interrogation of a small number of workers. The con-
versati~n method clarifies answers to posed questions, explains questions
that presented diverse difficulties (for example, terminological), and it
also permits recording a subject's comments that are beyon~ the framework
of the questions but of some interest.
Usually, there is a combination of questions in the questionnaires that
are prepared specially for each concrete case, with due consideration of
the distinctions of the occupation under study. The questionnaires must
be prepared in accordance with the tasks and objectives of the ergonomic
study. Before preparing a questionnaire, the researcher must observe
the workers for a certain time or, better yet, learn the main work opera-
tions himself. Then the first variant of the questionnaire is prepared,
and it is tested on a small number of subjects. At this stage, the
clarity and formulation of questions, fullness of the list of questions -
are checked; determination is made of the order of the questions in order
to avoid undesirable influences; additional questions are entered;
overlooked aspects of the problem under study are explained; the format -
of the questionnaire is tested to make sure it would not elicit a
negative attitudE on the part of the subjects [51].
The complex approach implies the use of interrogation methods that have
become widespread in ergonomic research in the ~orm of questionnaires
_ and ~.nterviews. The interrogation methods, like observations, are used
in ergonomics to work out working hypotheses and in order to enlarge upon
data obtained by other methods. In using the interrogation methods, the
nature of the questions, their formulation and direction are very important.
. A distinction is made between open questions (free answer) and closed ones
(multiple choice answers).
The following must be taken into consideration in order to formulate the
questions properly: 1) each question must be logically complete; 2) one
should avoid little-used foreign worc~s, special terms and words with -
- dual meaning; 3) one ~hould not po5e questions that are too long; 4) if the _
question deals with a subject that the worker is not familiar with or to
answer which he does not have the necessary vocabulary of special terms,
the appropriate explanations must be furnished; 5) each question must be
as concrete as possible; 6) one should either indicate all possible
50
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
I
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
~ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
variants of an answer that the sub~ects must bear in mind, or give none at
all; 7) one must provide to the sub~ect only the variants of answers
that could be acceptable to the same degree; 8) the question should be
formulated in such a manner as to avoid stereotype, banal answers; 9) one
should take care not to include in the question words that could ~iicit
a negative atCitude of the subject; 10) the questions should not be
suggestive in, nature [51 ] . '
As an example, we are submitting two types of questionnaires. The first
was uaed to study the work of unified power'system dispatahers and
dispatchers distributing tickets at rail~oafl stations. The objective of
rhis study was to develop an optimum variant of a dispatcher's chair. For
this reason, the quesCions were directed toward determining the subjective
opinion about the existing chair and w~.shes concerning improvement. The
second questionnaire was used to study the work of transformer cGs.l
winders in order to redesign the work place.
Questionnaire No 1
A. Surname, name, patronymic. B. Age. C. Position. D. Tenure.
E. Length of work day.
1. Is it comfortable for you to sit (ye~, no)i
. 2. What causes discomfort?
3. In what part of the body is their pain (back, lumbar region,
arms [or shoulders])?
4. Is it convenient to work with the hands (yes, no)?
5. Should the seat turn (yes, no)?
6. Would it be desirable for the chair to be on casters (yes, no)?
7. IS a back or lumbar region rest needed while working (yes, no)?
8. Is a flexible back needed (yes, no)?
9. Should the seat b~ flat, tilted backward, forward?
10. Are armrests needed (yes, no)?
11. At what level should the back of the chair be (scapula,
shoulders, lumbar region)?
. 12. How should the seat be (soft, semisoft, hard)?
13. How should it be upholstered (fabric, oilcloth, l~atherette)?
Questionnaire No 2
A. Surname, name, patronymic. B. Age. C. Position. D. Tenure
of work as winder [female]. E. Length of work day. F. Bench -
number. G. Bench type. H. Output norm per shift. I. Type of
coil. -
1. Is it comfortable for you to work (yes, no)?
2. What causes discomfort? -
3. Which parts of the body get the mo5c tired in the course of work
(back, lumbar region, shoulders, neck, arms, legs)?
51
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
4. In which part of the body and when is pain felt (back, lumbar
region, shoulders, neck, arms, legs; during work, at the start
of the day or after work)?
5. Do you experience physical tension when turning control levers,
depressing pedals (yea, no)?
6. Is it comfortable ta work with the hands (yes, no)?
_ 7. Is the si~e of the work bench satisfactory: height (yes, no),
depth (yes, no), width (yes, no)?
8. Is the height of the chair satisfactory (yes, no), is its shape
satisfactory (yes, no)?
9. Is a back required for the chair (yes, no)?
10. At what level should the back support be (scapula, shoulders,
lumbar region)?
11. Is a flexible chair back desirable (yes, no)?
12. How should the seat be (soft, semisoft, hard)?
13. How should it be upholstered (fabric, oilcloth, leatherette)?
14. Should the seat turn (yes, no)?
15. Are armrests needed (yes, no); for both arms (yes, no); for the
~ lef t (yes, no) ?
16. Is a footrest needed (yes, no)?
17. Do you have to stand up of ten during work?
As we see from these questionnaires, first general questions are posed:
"Is it comfortable for you to work?" "Is it comfortable for ~ou to sit?"
~ "What causes discomfort?" the answers to which are not veyy informative
but help establish contact with the sub~ects. Then f~'tow quQStions about
the sub~ective attitude toward elements of the work place, fur ex~:~ale:
"Is the size of the console satisfactory (height, depth, width)? Then
there are questions about how the worker feels. Last are the sub3ect's
wishes.
As a rule, the interrogation is conducted right at the work place, during
work. But it can also be done in the laboratory using experimental
samples of products or an experimental stand.
The effectiveness of the interrogatioii method depends largely on the
level of educatinn of the subjects and their occupational experience.
For example, power system dispatchers are experienced and highly skilled
specialists who work under very difficult conditions. Before becoming
d3spatchers they had worked for a certain time at other power facilities.
When they were questioned, exhaustive answers were obtained.
The ticket distributing dispatchers at railroad stations are mostly women
with secondary or incomplete secondary education. Their work experience is
very negligible, so that they answered "I don't know" to many questions.
As for the women working at the plant, they responded differently to
, general and specific questions. For example, they almost alway5~answered
52
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
I
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
in the affirmaCive to the question, "Is it comfortable for you to work?"
There were more variations in answers to specific questions. The women
complained of general fatigue, localized pain, mentioned flaws in the
design of the bench, chair, etc.
The data obtained from the inCerrogation are submitted to statistical
pracessing. The processing results are presented in a descriptive form,
and a distinction is made between observation data and the subjective
comments of a subject. The material described is accompanied by tables _
and charts of relevant data. The tables should indicate the percentile
of a given indicator in relation to data from all surveys.
When observation is combined with interrogation, it is important to find
a rational method of recording answers. Best of a11 is to provide
different variants of answers in the record (or observation scheme).
Simple answers should be given to simple questions: yes, no, don't know.
If possible, one should plan in advance the questions to which more -
expanded answers may be given. _
It is particularly important to have the same (general] observation outline
when several researchers are working concurrently. This permits combining
and comparing the results. However, in such cases, good instructions must '
be prepared on how to conduct observations and r_ecord the results.
Ob~ective (instrumental) research methods imply the use of various
instruments and equipment. The following are objective methods: measure-
ment of various characteristics of the industrial env~ronment (lighting,
noise, vibration, etc.), metric measurements, time studies, measurement
of physiological parameters (pulse, respiration, EKG, etc.), measurement
of psychological characteristics.
The first and last two methods require the use of rather compl2cated
monitoring and measuring equipment. There is no such restriction on the
use of the second and third methods.
When working on problems of improving organization of labor and increasing
its efficiency, data pertaining to the size of the industrial facility,
diffQrent elements thereof, location of windows and doors, as well as
data about the dimensions of equipment, work places, etc., may play a
substantial role. _
Spatial organization of the work place affects the nature and quality of
working motions, working position, etc. For this reason, analysis of
spatial organization of the work place must be made at the first staRes of
- professiographic studies. We submit below an outline [scheme]for anal.ysis
of spatial organization of the work glac~ :ahen it is to be remodeled [74].
53
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Outline for Analysis of Spatial Organization of Work Place
1. Determination of main types of sensory activity (visual, audi-
tory, visual and kinesthetic, etc.).
2. Demonstration of nature of motor activity.
3. Spatial determination of areas of sensory activity.
4. Spatial determination of areas of motor activity. -
S. Making a sketch of the location of the main equipment in rela-
tion to the worker, with indication of main sensory and motor
areas.
6. Making a sketch of location of ancillary equipment in relation
to working individual.
7. General sketch of work place. ~
- 8. Analysis of the obtained data.
The work is done in the following manner.
Determination is made of the main and ancillary types of equipment, zones
of motor and sensory activity on the basis of observation of actions
during work. Then the main working position and pose are determined by
questioning and observing.
The sketches make it possible to detect inconsistencies between the
existing spatial organization of a work place and psychophysiological
and anthropometric characteristics of man 5~absequent ana~ysis thereof,
with due consideration of the main ergonomic requirements, makes it
possible to offer recommendations pertaining to optimum remodeling of
the work place.
Time studies, i.e., keeping a record of changes as a function of time
in some parameters of the work process using a stopwatch or clock, are
one of the methods of ob~ective observation. Time studies permit
determination of various time-related characteristics of the work
process, on the basis of which one can determine the time spent on
performance of various operations and elimination of interference,
actual time spent per unit of production and the norm for the shift,
time lost on actions that have an indirect effect on work activity
- (leaving the work place, lack of materials, etc.), determination of the
dynamics of motor and sensory activity,and other indicators of efficiency
- [fitness]. A time study should not affect the nurmal progress of a work
process. When studying an occupation, time studies are made of both
separate work periods and the work day as a whole, in different shifts,
days of the week, etc.
As is the case with any method of objective staidy, time studies are pre-
ceded by special preparations, consisting of 'determination and study of
the work operations. Singling out the operations is the main prerequi-
site for proper time studies.
54
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
One of the variants of a time study is chronography, which consists of
recording the time characteristics in graph form. Cnronography is used
under industrial conditions to analyze the state and dynamics of a
man's motor and sensory activity during the owrk process.
Working motions (speed, direction, amplitude) and working pose, the number
of visual, auditory and tactile elements addressed to the work object,
information displays, etc., may be the ob,ject of a chronographic study.
4. Methods for the Study of Productive and Cognitive Activity
The content of inethods of studying movements is determined by the aggre-
gate of parameters characterizing the process of making a movement, on
the one hand, and means of recording these parameters, on the other.
Singling out the set of parameters that describe the process of perform-
ing a motion is related primarily to the choice of a certain conceptual
model that describes the work of a motor system (biomechanical model, _
physiological model of the myoneural system, etc.). Awareness of this
circumstance enables us to outline an approach to the classification of
methods of studying motion. Thus, kinematic (characteristics of spatial
motion) and dynamic (force) parameters of movements and means of record-
ing them involve development of a biomechanical model of the motor
system, while electromyographic methods owe their existence to development
of a physiological model of the neuromuscular system.
We should begin the description of inethods of studying motion with the
cyclogram, which is a photograph of a a movement on a stationary plate.
For this purpose, fluorescent marks or electric lamps are secured to the
movable parts of the subject's body. A shutter operating at a specific
frequency, which shuts the lens,~is~placed in front of the camera. The
successive positions of the lamps are recorded on the plate; these lamps
move in the course of a motion, together with the kinematic elements of
the body. This method cannot be used to record complex cyclic motions.
For kymocylography, the film on which information about lamp movement is
recorded moves uniformly and slowly. In this case, the cyclic motions
extend over the recording film. These methods of cyclography and�kymo-
- cyclography are intended for planar recording of movements.
Various modifications of the above methods are used to study spatial
movements: stereoscopic photography, i.e., photography with two lenses
with parallel opticdl axes, photography with lenses having converging
= optical axes, etc. The "mirror method" is also used, which yields
photos of an object from two different points using one camera and one
shutter. Two images of the same object hit the camera lens: one directly
from the object and the other reflected at a certain angle by a mirror.
This method provides for very accurate spatial readings and convenient
analysis of experimental data.
55
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Analysis of cyclograms is a rather labor-consuming process. Photographic
measurements and nomograms are used to analyze the displacements of _
various points of the body in space.
In the former c$se, the cyclogram negatives are printed on photographic
paper using an enlarger. In the same manner, a millimeter or half-milli-
meter grid is superimposed on the print, which alleviates significantly _
the work with the material and increases accuracy of ineasurements. The
nomogram method simplifies determination of all three spatial coordi-
nates of mirror cyclophoto grams.
Cyclography can be used for rather precise analysis of certain motor acts.
A method has been developed for cyclography of arm [or hand] movements
during haptic (blind) passage through a maze, on the basis of which it
has been possible to diff erentiate between orienting-exploring and
productive [executor] hand movements. Motions serving to construct an `
image and for recognition were isolated from tactile m~vements by means
of cyclogrpahic tracings. In auch cases, motion was also recorded in
one plane.
There are more methods that are used to study various motor problems.
They include methods for me asuring the intensity of magnetic and electro-
magnetic fields, tensometry, holography, radar, etc. The intensity of
magnetic and electromagnetic fields is measured to study relatively low-
amplitude and angular motion. The tensometric method, like the gonio-
graphic onQ (the latter will be discussed in greater detail ~elow), is
used for macro- and micro-angular measurements. Tensometry has gained
particularly wide use for measurement of macroscopic changes in an arti-
cular angle when studying tremor. Television, holography and radar have
not yet found the development they deserve in the field of motion
studies. Television is us ed mainly as a display [indicatorJ device. This
is attributable to the fact that there are some difficulties involved
in obtaining spatial parame ters in the form of electrical signals that are
convenient for analysis of movements of an object with the use of a tele-
~ vision system. Extensive introduction of computers in the field of ~
ergonomic research is a means of overcoming these difficulties. Holo-
graphic and radar methods are still used quite seldom, although they
are rather promising. Probably goniography is the most convenient and
widespread method for meas urement of angular displacement. Goniography,
which yields readings abou t changes in spatial position of an articula-
tion of a kinematic chain, is used for artificial feedback. However, it
is a rather complicated technical task to obtain electircal signals
equivalent to the spatial d isplacement of an end point of an open kinematic
chain. For this reason, there are substantial limitations for the use
of this method to study sp atial displacements of an object.
The armamentarium of inethod s for the study of executor activity alsa
includes experimental situa tions that are nrganized in a special manner.
56
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
These situations include diverse variables, which can be viewed as
essential conditions dete~ining performance of motor tasks. Tracking
is one of the widely used experimental situations in studies of executor
activity.
As related to the study of man's executor activity, the tracking situation
can be considered on two planes: as a laboratory model of various types
of practical human activity (work of radar station operator, driving
various forms of transport and others) and as an experimental procedure
for solving certain theoretical problems that arise when analyzing
motor behavio,..
In the tracking situation, the subject is asked to perform a movement,
the parameters of which (speed, direction, amplitude, time) should
conform [satisfy] with the parameters of a moving target, with which the
subject's movement is coordinated. The specifics of the tracking situa-
tion (unlike a"precision task" and that of "preserving stability" of
motion parameters), consist primarily of the fact that, in this case,
the motor behavior of the sub~ect is rigidly determined for virtually
all parameters of motion.
The following terms are generally used to describe tracking: setting
or standard object (or "target")--an ob3ect, the law of motion of which
- is set by means of a certain input function; controlled object (or
"kursor" [tracking device?]), which is the object that the sub~ect
controls by handling a control element. The motor behavior of the subject
in a specified situation is expressed by the motion of the controlled
object (output function).
Thus, a tracking problem consists of having the value of the output func-
tion correspond exactly to the value of the input function at the
appropriate point in time, while the subject must work out a corrective
action to eliminate the discrepancy between values of input and output
functions on the basis of perceived information. ~ao classes of variables
determining a tracking situation are distinguished, depending on how
rigidly the subject's motor behavior is determined and what information
- he receives about tracking.
The first class of variables is related to the type of input function
used, which is determined primarily by the nature of dynamics of the
input function in time. A distinction is made between continuous and
- discrete tracking problems. In the case of continuous tracking, the
parameters of the input function change continuously. But if the
values in input function change in "~umps" at certain points in time,
we are dealing with a discrete tracking problem.
The second class of variahles is related to Che nature of information
abouC how the tracking problem is solved. A distinction i.~ made
~ 57
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
HOR OFrICIAL li5E ONI.Y
between pursuing [fillowing] and compensatory tracking, depending on .
whether the target is moving or not. In the case of pursuing tracking,
the subject receives three types of information: about movement of the
target, actual "pursuit" movement and discrepancy (or error) between
position of the target and tracking~device. In the compensatory tracking
situation, the target does not move and the subject must hold the control-
led object on it, the latter being subject to perturbing factors and
deviating from the required position. In this case, information about
the subject's regulating action on the controlled ob~ect and character-
istics of input function cannot be distinguished. Only information
about the magnitude of deviation of the tracking device from the target
is used to solve the problem.
The main objectives of research were formulated differently, depending
on whether tracking was studied for applied or theoretical purposes,
and specific experimental procedures were designed for the performance
of specific types of tracking. Thus, when using tracking as an applied
method, co~;;?ensatory tracking was and is generally used. This is attri-
butable primarily to the fact that, in this case, one is mainly interested
in analysis of different variables that influence the degree of dis-
crepancy between the position of the setting and controlled objects and
displacement of the control in order to minimize mistakes. For this
reason, it is desirable to simplify as much as possible the experimental
procedure and to rule out of consideration the influence of "super-
fluous" channels of information on the problem-solving process. Con-
versely, when using tracking to analyze theoretical problems (for
example, the role of efferent systems in regulation of movements),
a rich information field in the situation of pursuing tracking offers
wider possibilities.
The use of tracking as a means of analyzing productive activity requires
the choice of different variables determining the process of solving
a motor problem and modeling them under experimental conditions (or
through operator training on simulators). The following are the most
widespread variables of tracking: time lag (i.e., interval between
controlling action and change in parameter regulated at the input),
simultaneous control of several parameters (multifactor control),
including interdependent ones, manipulation of visual feedback (interrup-
tion, inversion) and an additional problem. Introduction of these
_ variables, as well as the use of various forms of tracking combined with
other methods of analysis of motion, makes it possible to solve a wide
range of applied and theoretical problems.
Development of an adequate method of recording and analyzing the time and
space tracing of productive [executor] actions is a mandatory prerequisite
for successful studies of motor acts. An experimental stand for the
study of instrumeneal motor skills meets this requirement.
58
FOR OFFICTAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
~
~OR OFFICIAL U5E ONLY
The functional flowchart of the experimental stand (Figure 1) includes the
following: a system to control the object; color television indicator;
c~ntrol computer which operates both in the calculating mode for multi-
dimensional statistical processing of the results, and in the mode for
control of the experiment.
The object control system includes
~ a multigrade control element, tenso-
screen : Eye metric amplifier and operational
~ amplifier unit.
~ contxo The manipulator type control ele-
ener o ment (sensor of spatial displace-
o: ment of operator's hand) is a
parametric model of the human
hand; it is constructed in the
form of a hinged joint of three
- ampli~ier kinematic elements by means of
simple h3nges, and it has three
ol degrees of mobility. Any spatial
p ygraph
- displacement of the point to
Figure 1. which the operator applies force
Flowchart of experimental stand is transformed into the corres-
ponding change in angles formed
Key: by the kinematic system of the
ABM) analog computer control element. The input para-
M-6000) computer
meters are the current values of
trigonometric functions of angles formed by the sine-cosine senso.rs placed
an the axes of rotation of the element. From them, a spatial mathematical
model of the control in relation to a right-angle Cartesian system of
coordinates is formed in the analog computer unit. The design of the
control element permits retention of the content and natui'al direction
of the operator's manual movements, although the control system has provi-
sions for disrupting the homogeneity and conformity of the ~notor and
- sensory fields by means of inputting coefficients of space compression
or electrical inversion of the direction of similar vectors.
The color television screen ["indicator"] used in the experimental in-
stallation could be described as deceptively [illusory] descriptive,
since an impression of the volume of test and controlled signals is
gained by altering the magnitude of the controlled signal.
The indicator is based on a commercial color television receiver with
a control unit. Light signals of different colors are formed on the
indicator screen in accordance with the analog electrical signals inputted
in the control unit. An impression about the volume is obtained by
controlling the change in area of lit signals. The lights on the screen
59
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
can move horizontally (X), vertically (Y) and by changing the lit area (Z).
Independent control of the light stimuli for parameters X, Y and Z makes
it possible to use them to code the spatial coordinates of displacement of
the control ob~ect and to form a system of reference for tne operator's
sensory field. The controlling coordinate signals are formed in the
object control unit according to equations of relationship between spatial
movement of the operator's hand and the control element.
The control computer can be operated in two modes, active and calculation.
The programs for control of the experiment and processing of obtainec'
results are run by an interpreting system on an M-6000 computer of the~
ASVT [modular system of computer facilities]. The experiment is con-
ducted in the mode of dialog with the computer in accordance with the
principle of priority servicing of the following devices for communica-
tion with the object: modules for input of discrete information of
experimenter's and subject's control signals; modules of group control
of output of discrete information from test signals of the operator's
visual communication channel; contactless commutator; analog-digital
converter that receives analog signals coneerning the spatial position
of the subject's hand. ~
The use of a computer [digital] in the experiment line makes it possible
to display on a screen trajectories of mo tion that change in complexity,
number of elements and components; to introduce disruptions in the custo-
- mary actions manifested by changes in traj ectory of movement; to introduce
inversion, i.e., disrupt the usual correlation between perceptive and
motor fields. Connection to a computer has a'lleviated the labor-
consuming job of manual processing of tens of Chousands of ineasurements
and made it possible to obtain the precision- and sFeed-related character-
istics of motions directly during the exp eriment.
The multipurpose experimental stand we have described permits recording
time and space--speed and precision--parameters of the process under
study. Movements of the lever are record ed on tNe tape of a multichannel
polygraph in the form of three components on the X, Y and Z axes. The
signal from the computer concerning presentation of a new matrix and
subject's signals of matching with each element of this matrix are
recorded on a separate channel.
Movement of the controlled spot was record ed simultaneously on a tape
recorder as well, which permitted zeproduction of the trajectary of
the movement on a plotter, as well as to input the experimental data
in the computer for an estimate.
The use of microstructural analysis, the purpose of which is to single
out rapid components of an integral action, made it possible to distinguish
the following stages for each component X, Y and Z of spatial motion:
60
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OEFICIAL US~ ~NLY
latent, phasic (performing), and the stage of checking and corre~tior..
Figure 2 illustrates an example of a tracing of a shift by one element in
~~ne of the trajectories of motion. It clearly shows that there is a
significant latency pertod for each component preceding motion. After
active muvement of e3ch component, one records a long period of relative
rest which precedes the subject's signal that the controlled spot is
matched with a matrix element. This period can be considered the
period of corrections, which is characterized by fine motions referable to
some component or other, and period of checking the quality of matching.
As can be seen in this figure, the
duration varies for each component
of the stages: there is some lag
a,, CCU in programming for one component,
PU' as compared to another, i.e.,
= { successive pla.nning for each com-
PFU = ponent is possible. A~alogously,
~ j there is also a certain shift of
, - ~ performance and checking.
_
;;i;: ,
These data served as the basis for
" making a distinction between so-
7/ called "net time" of unit-stages:
L. PFU--program forming unit, PU---
performance unit, CCU--checking and
correcting unit, as we11 as two
Figure 2. stages of scatter: L1t1
Diagram of isolation of components includes both planning and per-
of an integral action formance, and ~t2, which combines
TpgU) Tmin. latency . performance and checking. The
TpU) Ttotal '~Tmax.lat. + "net time" of each unit is the
Tcheck.~ time when the coinponents of motion
TCCU~ T~in. checking [or control] function in terms inherent in this
ptl) Tmax.lat. - T min.lat. particular un3t, be it planning,
~t2) Tmax.check. - Tmin.check. performance or checking. The
scatter, which is characterized
by the value of ~tl and ~t2,
provides information about scatter, not only within one stage, but also
between stages of motion, describing the degree of spatiality of the
performed action.
Use of the multipurpose experimental stand offers wide opportunities for
studying processes of control and construction of motion.
The use of modern methods of analysis of cognitive processes is found to
be quite effective in solving a number of applied problems.
The capacity for visual detection and discrimination of critical elements, -
presented against the background of others that differ in some tags and
- 61
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
coincide in others (radar screens, photos of events in Wilson chambers,
x-rays, etc.), is of decisive significance for a number of modern operator
occupations.
Optimization of this type of activity is related primarily to analysis of
the properties of the visual system as a filter of time and space fre-
quencies. Psychophysical studies of man and psychophysiological studies
of animals [21] revealed that there are information processing channels,
in the optic system that a~e specific for certain spatial fr.equencies of
an image. Maximum sensitivity to sinus-modulated distribution of bright-
ness, which has a specific spatial frequency, is inherent in them. Thus,
the visual system is structurally and functionally capable of frequency
analysis of any image, just like a certain function can be analytically
presented in the form of the sum of sinusoidal components when it is
submitted to Fourier expansion.
--i-- The characteristics of these
~ ! I frequency-specific channels deter-
� , mine the function of contrast
~ ~ sensitivity of the sight system
~ ~ (Fgiure 3), which indicates the
' extent to which various spatial
~ j ~ I frequencies of an image are
amplified or, on the contrary,
~ ~~w attenuated as they pass througll
~ Spatial freq. the optic system.* In spite of
(cycl.es/degree) the fact that, by virtue of the
Figure 3. nonlinearity of these trans-
Function of contrast sensitivity formations [85], the functions
of the human visual system (after of contrast sensitivity adequately
Campbell and Robson, 1968) characterize the capabilities of
our vision only for near-threshold
intensities of stimulaCion, it contains appreciably fuller information than
numerous traditional parameters of "acuity of vision." ~~Moreover, in
evaluating any means of visual reflection, the question arises, first of
all, as to whether certain information can be perceived at a11. For this
reason, the problem of supraliminal nonlinearity of the visual system in
this context is not so important.
Let us consider more carefully the function illustrated in Figure 3. The
descent of the right arm of the curve of sensitivity in the area of high
spatial frequencies conforms with the well-known fact that rather fine
details are indiscernible. This flaw of sight is compensated by various
means of enlargement of the angular dimensions of the image. Less known
*Contrast sensitivity functions are plotted by determining the minimum
depth of modulation of sinusoidal distribution of brightness dis-
tinguished from a homogeneous field.
62
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
is ti?e fact that there is diminished visual sensitivity to low spatial
frequencies ref lect~d by the decline of the left part of the curve. It
Ls very important to bear this in mind, for example, in roentgenology,
since soft tissues and tumors are represented on the films by expressly
low-frequency, gradual signs of brightness. Thus, depending on the part
of the spectrum of an image containing critical information, it may be
desirable not only to enlarge, but reduce the image. Since the range -
of possible changes in angular size of details is quite large (about
1:20), i.t is obvious that this cannot be achieved by a simple change in
distance of the photo.
Supplementing analysis of spatial sensitivity with information about the
time-related resolution sensitivity of the eye is an interesting develop-
ment of this approach. In particular, such studies established that
discrimination of features referable to the shape of objects diminishes
- if the time and space conditions of presentation coincide with those
under which seeming (stroboscopic) movement is observed [18]. It is
a known fact to anyone that there is analogous perception of rapidly
moving real objects.
The design and development of multidimensional devices for displaying
information is a similar [close] area of applied research that has ex-
perienced the strong influence of experimental psychology. Here, the
designer~s task is to report to the operator, as simultaneously as
possible and without interference, a muZtitude of diverse information,
which determines separately or in some combination the accuracy of
their solutions. The entire history of work in this field shows that
our ordinary objective perception, which integrates into a single,
integral pattern, not only diverse sensory information but data stored _
in memory, is an ideal example of solving this problem. For this reason, _
all of the more interesting work in this field is based more or less on
the use of ecologically natural mechanisms of perceptual processing,
the details of which are demonstrable by the diverse techniques used to
study perception. Thus, psychophy~ical studies of perception of time
and motion [84] originated an entire family of display devices of the
contact analog ~"conalogs") type that have been well-described in the
special literature. Combined with the possibility of referring to
precise digital information about each of the critical parameters of
a situation, the "conalogs" permit concurrent consideration of multi- ,
dimensional dynamic spatial information about the position of objects
such as an aircraft, rocket, submarine, etc. ~
There is a great potential for use of the reserves of graphic visual
memory for identification purposes. As ~hown by the most recent studies,
while remembering random visual structures suffers from the same restric-
tions as remembering meaningless verbal material [90], the memory of
subject-related slides of landscapes, even when they are rather mono-
t.onous in topic, is much greater in volume and duration than all other
known forms of inemory.
63 ~
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~
The work of Swiss authors [91], who were given the task of developing algo- -
- rithms that would permit visual discrimination between genuine and ~
counterfeit banknotes could serve as sn example, perhaps not the most
important but definitely quite demonstrative, of using the mechanisms of
objective perception as a basis. The difficulty of this task consists of
the fact that there is a co nsiderable number of spatial parameters of
a drawing (distance between elements of the drawing, their size, etc.),
each of whicli is normally characterized by a certain range of variations.
Ir.terestingly enough, an attempt to represent these parametErs in the
form af abstract figures--closed polygons (Figure 4)--failed just as
much as the use of data in d igital form. Conversely, representation of -
these parameters in the form of arbitrary pictures of human faces (Chernov
algorithm), as can be seen in Figure 4, solves this problem rather easily.
Procedures such as recording eye movements, chronometric analysis, -
factor experiments, etc., are also used with succtss for studies of ~ '
information retrieval processes [8, 89]. Development of these directions
of research, which are already quite traditional from the standpoint of
practical use, has led to more comprehensitTe analysis of the possibility
~ of utilizing the spatial characteristics of ey~ movements to optimize
complex sensorimotor coordinations. Experimental analysis of information
retrieval processes, which take place in the endogenous space or, more
precisely, internal, subjective spaces of operator memory, rather than
exogenous, is a new direction of research.
The time study of recognition processes is the prototype of most such
studies: the subject must d etermine as rapidly as possible whether a
presented object belongs to a previously shown "positive" set [92].
The typical results consist of the fact that the time of both positive
_ ("yes") and negative ("no") reactions is a linearly growing function
of the magnitude of the "po sitive" set (Figure 5). In addition, the
slope of bo~h functi.ons is about the same. This shows that the search -
for information among elements of the "positive" set represented ~n
memory i.s a s~.iccessive ;~rocess, in ttte first place, and an exhaustive
one, in the se~ond. In other words, this is a process that continues
until aIl elements of the s et are checked in memory, even if the icentity
of the presented element with one stored in memory had been established
at one of the intermediate stages of retrieval. If the search had
stopped immediately af ter e stablishing that they are identical (self -
ending search), it would be necessary to examine twice as many elements
in negative tests than positive ones. For tliis reason, the slope of
: the function for negative answers must be twice the slope of the function
for positive ones.
_ Interestingly enough, in some studies results were obtained that appeared
to contradict this analysis: the functions for negative reactions were
somewhat steeper than for p~ sitive ones, but not to the extent that one
_ could have expected in the case of self-ending retrieval [83J. However,
64
FOR OPFICIAL USE ONLY
=~;g
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
oo_ooo 00000
0 000 0 0000 0 _
,oooQO o0o a o
_ ~p o 0 0 0 00 o a o
000~~ a~0o~
~DOOC~O C~~OC~ =
~c~ mC~ o . -
~o ~o~~~~~~
m o o ~ o ~ _
m c~ ~ ~ m a~~`
- ~mOm4~~~~~
Qdomm~~~~~ _
Figure 4. Examples of abstract and conventional images: genuine
banknotes on the left and counterfeit ones on the right -
(see text).
a more thorough analysis revealed that these results are artefacts of the
' practice, unfortunately still prevalent in psychology, of averaging
individu~.l data. The results for oue part of the subjects turned out to
correspond exactly to an exhaustive type of szarch, whereas the results
_ for another, smaller group of subjects corresponded rather well to the
self-ending type. Somewhat paradoxical is the fact that the latter sub-
~ects, who seemingly chose the more rational work strategy, actually
performed the task les~ efficiently than the first group of subjects.
65
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
This last example leads us directly -
to a question, which is of utmost
importance to ergonomics, about the
~~yes" description and systematization of
answera individual differences in charac-
ai teristics of work activity. The
, classical methods of the Soviet
q' '~no" � school of differential psychology ~
~ answers [52, 69] lay the foundation for
ergonomic procedures of typology '
~ and concrete individual psycholo-
- a gical analysis. In addition, the
i., development of conceptions of the "
Volume of "positive" microstructure of various forms of
set cognitive and productive activity
Figure 5. also permits offering a psycho-
Typical results of a study of re- logically competent evaluation of
- trieval from memory. A case of differences in the functional _
successively exhaustive search systems that perform these
(after Stexnberg, 1975) elemer_ts in a specific individual.
It becomes possible to overcome
auch deep-rooted empiricism in differential psychology, which deliberately
limits itself only to studies of the correlation type. A more detailed
example of this approach is discussed in the section dealing with
methods of analysis of functional states (see also [40]).
5. Methods of Evaluating Functional States
In the modern literature, a distinction is usually made between three types
of criteria with which one can evaluate the state of a subject: physio-
logical, behavioral and s�~jective indices [40, 79]. However, the
classification of Bartlett [80] is more distinct; he singled out physio-
logical and psychological indices. The last group includes criteria of
efficiency of performance of various psychometric tests and analysis of
sub~ective symptoms of concrete types of functional states.
Pttysiological testin~ methods: The efforts of a large group of researchers
are directed toward the search for parameters of changes in functional
state of the body, be they indirect, but immediately re~orded [20, 57].
Traditional ref erence to this class of phenomena is attributable to a
number of important reasons. The main one is the possibility of an
objective description of observed phenomena. In addition, the use of
physiological parameters expands appreciable the range of manifestations, -
accessible to description,of the studied dynamics of behavioral reactions,
and creates the possibility for at least hypothetical correlation o~
psychological phenomena to their organic basis. The basic possibility of
' making a quantitative evaluation of functional changes in any system is
a rather important argument in favor of using physiological parameters.
66
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
'Che mosC diverse indices of central nervous system function are considered `
as possible indicators of the dynamics of functional states. They include,
first of all, the electrophysiological parameters of the EEG, EMG, galvanic
skin response, evoked potentials, as we11 as heart rate, arterial pressure,
vascular tonus, diameter of the pupil and many others (Figure 6). In
addition, there is intensive development af studies of biochemical changes
in the body associated with various functional states. On the hasis of
special techniques, complex and multieffector recording methods are being
developed.
~1rJ~ EE vo e oten ' ~~1/`r
Superficial~
+~-rM+1~ ~
Spirometer ; ,
~ Tem . .~--.F
~V~Pn~umog ' ~t sys~ollc
Intr~~c ~ \ pressur diastolic
~
~~D~r~amo r. ~ EK
~''..r~
Overall Reflexe
~_acti~
� _ vity
,
~ Ground�- ~I .
~'Figure 6. Multichannel recording of the most frequently studied
types of human bioelectrical activity
T'raditionally, changes in parameters of electrical activity of the brain
are considered as a direct indicator of the dynamics of activation
levels. Typical changes in the EEG are being related to various types
of functional states. Thus, the reaction of desynchronization of a-rhythm
combined with appearance of slow wave (y and A) activity is interpreted
as appearance of developing fatigue. As fatigue increases, these periods
- last longer and there are the signs of EEG "hypersynchronization."
The galvanic skin reaction (GSR) is another conventional method of studying
the dynamics of functional states; it is used as an indicator of
"vegetative tonus." It has been proven experimentally that there is a
direct link between the nature of electrocutaneous responses and change
in state of the re~icular fortnation; consequently, they can be considered
among the most acceptable criteria of level of general activity. Use of
this indicator is related primarily to the task of diagnosing states of
emotional tension [stress].
67
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Various parameters of cardiovasculaf function are among the most sensitive
and informative indicators of the dynamics of functional states: analysis
of main EKG components, heart rate, arterial pressure, circulation ["fill-
ing with blood"J, perivascular and capillary resistance. The development
- of stress and fatigue, which is related to increased expenditure of
energy, leads to a consistent increase in heart rate, respiratory excur-
sions and other parameters, indicatlve of intensification of inetabolic pro-
cesses. The typical changes in main EKG parameters for a specific subject
could serve as a reliable indicator of the degree of adjustment to a
set level of information load.
The dynamics of autonomic somatic parameters--body temperature, digestive
and excretory system functions, etc.--are used with success to describe
involuntary changes in level of activation during, for example, the
circadian cycle.
An extensive area of research deals with the distinctions of hormonal
changes under the influence of various work loads and conditions. In
spite of the purely technical difficulties involved in using these
parameters for diagnostic purposes, the number of inethods being developed
_ and already used in practice is constantly growing. Aside from studies
of quantitative dynamics of secretion of various hormones as indicators
of circadian rhythms, many studies deal with demonstration of distinctions
of secretary activity in various behavioral situations, mainly as re-
lated to nature and level of work load. Increased levels in blood and
urine of a working man of 17-hydroxycorticosteroids or of the "stress
hormones," epir.ephrine and norepinephrine, are generally mentioned as
the typical correlates of stress, incr?ased tension and fatigue.
The dynamics of physiological parameters reflect not only general changes
in level of body activity, bu~ changes in loads on different functional -
systems. According to the existing data, analysis of fluctuations of
cerebral hemodynamics during the performance of rather complex ii~tellec-
tual work makes it possible to distinguish the main stages of decline
in mental fitness and to determine the extent of involvement of various
cerebral structures in the process of solving various problem~. One
observes a typical topography of the points of maximum desynchronization
- of a-rhythm when solving various problems, according to their contenfi.
Fatigue leads to a change in the structural and functional system of
electrical activity of the brain, which is also specific for various
types of activity. Wide use is made in the studie~ of load size of
= its physiological correlates, such as changes in diameter of the pupil
and GSR, which per~uit second-by-second monito:.ing of the exertion
made to perform an assignment (Figure 7).
In view of these data, which a~re indicative of the systemic nature of the
observea changes, it is becoming more important to describe the set of
physiological reactions specific to a given state of the body. It is
68
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
possible to solve this problem adequately on the basis of polyeffector
recording of parameters. However,it is extremely difficult to meet this
requirement due to the diversity of reactions and dissimilarity of
changes observed in the presence of the same state.
ON b~~ tn -
� q ~ ~ O
R~7 N uaI
a u a ~
~ a~, ai o ~
� ~ ~ cn w ~
~ a ar o
~ ~ a a
~
.r.,
'b ~ ~
~
~ ~
a ~ ~ ~~R
a 4'~
~ , ~ ~ ~
- ~ ~ .
34 p
d
.
~ ~ is ~r> m 2~
_ _
Time, s
Problems:.,.-~ adding aloud
,pronouncinc~ aloud
o-----0 mental additipn
o-----0 mental pronunciation
Figure 7. Diameter of pupil as a function of difficulty of task
and time required to perform it (after Kanemann,
Pivler and Onusku, 1968)
There is no question that a mental load and change in functional capa-
bilities of the body are associated with changes in a number of physio-
logical parameters. Unfortunately, there are many other factors that
have an analogous effect on the same parameters. Undesirable properties
have been reported [20] in such a popular parameter as the EEG: varia-
bility of changes in the same individual, variability of these changes
in different individuals, similarity of EEG changes in the presence of
substantially different states. It must be stressed that these features
are also inherent in other physiological parameters.
The use of physiological indices for diagnostic purposes is also being
delayed by substantial difficulties of a metrological nature. In spite
of the basic possibility of direct quantitative measurement of changes
69
FOR OFFICIr1L USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
in physiological functions observed in experiments, the researcher is
faced with a number of problems. They include the development and choice of
methods of analysis (mathematical models and conceptual analytical
schemes) that would be suitable for the material under study. In addi-
tion, there are several metrological problems common to all types of
phyaiological measurements, the main ones being problems of standardized
levels of function and nonlinearity of ineasurement scales [57).
The above facts, aswell as the methodological flaws in procedures for
recording and processing physiological data, usually present real diffi-
culties in using parameters for practical determination of the dynamics of
functional states.
Psychological testing methods: Psychological methods of assessing func-
tional states were developed chiefly within the context of studies of
fatigue and dynamics of fitness for work. Several main stages are dis-
tinguished in the history of development of this problem, and they are
related to basically different approaches to formulation of research
objectives and evaluation of the diagnostic value of the different
parameters [47, 81]. The current stage of research on fatigue began
with the publication of the well-known monograph by Bartly and Chute [79].
These authors, who stressed the complex nature of this phenomenon,
singled out and submitted to comprehensive analysis three main aspects
of the problem. The term "fatigue" was used to refer to a personality-
cognitive syndrome combining diverse disorders of inental functions and
subjective sensations of fatigue, aversion for work, physical discomfort,
etc. Experimental implementation of this approach requires the creation
of subjec.tive and psychometric research methods consistent with the
purpose of the studies.
A. A. Ukhtomskiy had already mentioned the prospects of using sub~ective -
evaluation of fatigue for diagnostic purposes; he wrote that "so-caZled -
subjective evaluations are just as objective as any others, and tney
offer in practice more delicate and precise criteria of fatigue and
fatigability than the existing laboratory methods per se" (quoted in [47]),
and this is attributable to the diversity of manifestations of symptoms
of fatigue in the individual's internal life, ranging from the set of
sensations of fatigue well-known to all to specific changes in self-
afferentation that affect the areas of cognition and motivation. ~
In spite of the widespread opinion that the data referable to subjective
experience are of first and foremost significance to discernment of
fatigue, for a long time this area of research was neglected. It is only
in the last 10-15 years that it began to be studied intensive~y and
fruitfully.
The symptoms of fatigue are quite diverse in the mental life of an indi-
_ vidual. Feelings of fatigue, weakness, lack of strength, rapid fatigability
70
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
- and sleepiness are direct manifestations of fatigue. In cases of severe _
fatigue, ane usually observes negatively colored emotional reactions:
aversion to work, irritability, hostility, distressing tension, etc.
States of physiological discomfort are experienced with varying degrees
of awareness: increased perspiration, faster heart rate, appearance of
dyspnea, tremor, pain in various parts of the body, etc. In addition,
recognized disorders in the area of various mental functions may be in-
cluded with subjective symptoms. We refer to characteristics of attention
(sluggish, static or sporadic, unstable), diverse sensory disorders and
disturbances referable to the motor system (change in speed of movement,
dir~inished precision and coordination, deautomation of skills).
We can distinguish two categories of such symptoms: sub~ective reactions
characterized by the individual's attitude toward his own condition,
- and objectively verified signs of fatigue (physiological discomfort and
impairment of inental activity), which the individual may recognize. The
existence of qualitatively different groups of symptoms paves a foundation
for development of varinus directions of inethods for subjective -
diagnostics, subjective scaling and questionnaires.
- The use of questionnaires is directed toward detection of qualitatively
diverse symptoms of fatigue, which could be recognized more or less
readily by the individual. Quantitative evaluation or determination of
the severity of each symptom is not the main purpose of such studies.
- Man's state is assessed by the total number of above-mentioned symptoms _
and their qualitative uniqueness.
Questionnaires differ appreciably from one another in number of symptoms
listed and method of grouping them. The number ranges from a few to
several dozen or even hundreds. The general trend in developing new -
questionnaires is the desire to limit the list of symptoms, which
conforms with the requirement of brevity of the test description and
simplicity of quantitative processing. At the same time, it implies
the including in the list of the most important "key" signs. _
The choice of the most informative symptoms and groups of symptoms is the
chief ineans of creating more compact and reliable questionnaires. Not
infrequently, this work is done on the basis of use of the methods of
multifactorial statistical analysis.
As an example, let us consider the questionnaire of physical activity
prepared by the Japanese Health Association in 1971. The analytical
factor method was used to design the questionnaire. They proceeded
from the premise that the entire diversity of manifestations of fatigue
could be classi~fied in the following manner: symptoms of low activation,
low motivation and physical disintegration, the first two groups of
symp!:oms being common to virtually all types of labor.
71
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
- Their base materia~ for preparation of the questionnaire consisted of 48
terms describing various manifestations of fatigue. A study was conducted,
in which 65 subjects rated the suitability of each term for testing
fatigue on a seven-point scale. Ttao groups of the most informative symp-
toms, combined under the names of "low activation" and "low motivation,"*
were isolate~l by means of factor analysis, on the basis of the results
of preliminary evaluations. We submit below the questionnaire that was
developed.
Physical Activity Questionnaire
"Low Activation" "Low Motivation"
1. Do not want to walk. 1. Mistakes in work.
2. Breaking vaice. 2. Avoidance of glance.
3. Not ready to work. 3. Difficulties in communication.
4. Hollow cheeks. 4. Slowness.
5. Avoiding conversations. 5. Sleepiness.
6. Gloomy face. 6. Easily distracted.
7. Lifeless eyes. 7. Pale face.
8. Irritability. 8. Expressionless ["wooden") face.
9. Apathetic face. 9. Digital tremor.
10. Listlessness. 10. Difficulty in concentration.
Thus, modern studies in the field of developing subjective questionnaires
are characterized by a thorough development of the symptoms of fatigue,
classification of signs and distinctian of decisive factors, devPlopment
� of inethods of monitoring [checking] performance of tests. However, there
were several serious difficulties encountered in the practical use of
existing questionnaires. In the first place, this is related to the
lack of developed methods for quantitative evaluation of the obtained
results. The total number of symptoms noted is too rough an indicator,
particularly if the relative significance of the presence or absence
of some sign is not assessed. Moreover, the questionnaires do not
usually determine the sever~.ty of a symptdm. The latter flaw can be
overcome by means of inethods for scaling a subjective state.
Methods of subjective scaling are designed for the subject himself to
assess the degree of fatigue. He is asked to relate his condition to a
series of signs, for each of which polar ratings are provided (absence/
presence, bad/good). The distance between the extreme points is re-
_ presented in the form of a multistep scale. The severity of each sign
is determined by the location of the point selected by the sub3ect on this
scale. Thus, this group of inethods is one of the modifications of
*Translator's note: Tbe Russian word for "low" could also mean weak
- or poor.
72
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Osgood's semantic differential method that is widely used in psychological
ytudies.
The dimensionality of the scales and methods of working with them differ
substantially for differenr authors. A~ a rule, scales are used that have
Eive, seven or nine gradations. In recent years, ungraduated scales, i.e.
rhe so-called visual analogs of rating scales, are gaining increasing
popularity. In this case, the subjects are shown segments of lines of
a specified size, on which they have to note the distance that subjectively
corresponds to the degree of the rated experience.
The history of use of the scaling method in the field of diagnosing
fatigue dates back to the work of Muscio and Poffenberger. They proposed
a typical seven-point scale,* which was plotted on the basis of elementary
common sense, and it can also be encountered in many current studies.
Use of subjective methods of rating functional states moves to the fore
the problem of standardization of the meanings of words and expressions
used in plotting a scale or tabulating a list of symptoms. The Thurstone
method is generally used for this purpose. The main element for this
method is the presence of a large enough group of expert subjects who
~ are working on construction of the scale itself. The first phase of the
work consists of selecting a certain number of words and expressions
characterizing critical degrees of fatigue out of an extsnsive list
(up to several hundred) verbal characteristics of this state available
in a given language. Then, the order of the selected signs on the
scale is determined according to the classifications of the same group
of experts.
The Poffenberger method is an example of simple, one-factor scaling.
When designing scales, modern authors proceed from the conception of
existence of a complicated complex of feelings of fatigue. It is assumed
that such a set of symptoms is represented by clearly distinguishable
groups of signs, the severity of manifestation of which changes in
accordance with the degree of fatigue. The test of differentiated self-
evaluation of fatigue (SAN)** is an example of a method of multiple fac-
tor scalinp, In developing this test, it was assumed that it is possible
to describe a functional state by meana of three categories of sign3: state
of health, activity and mood [affect]. The subject has to relate his
state to a number of signs characterizing each of these categories. The
severity of the sign is determined on a seven-point scale.
*I feel: excellent, very good, good, satisfactory, tired, very tired,
extremely tired.
**Acronym from the initial letters [in Russian] of the following words:
state of health, activity, mood.
73
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The SAN test consists of a card on which there are 30 pairs of words with
polar meanings. Each of the three categories is characterized by 10 pairs
of words. In the "state of health" category there are the characteristics
of strength, health and fatigability, for example: state of health--
poor/good, I feel strong/weak, full of energy/without energy, etc. In
the "activity" category there are the characteristics of mobility and
rates of various functions: passive/active, immobile/mobile, slow/fast,
etc. Characteristics of the emotional state are included in the "mood"
category: cheerful/sad, bad/good mood, joyous/gloomy, etc. Presentation
of polar signs referable to the same group 10 times increases the relia-
bility of the data obtained. The location of positive (negative) signs
on both the right and left sides of the card reduces the possibility of
deliberate distortion of the results.
The data for each category of signs are averaged, and three quantitative
parameters are to be used: arithmetic mean, standard deviation [mean-
square deviation] and arithmetic mean error. The arithmetic mean is
the direct characteristic of degree of fatigue, while the scatter of
ratings within a group of signs (standard deviation) serves to ~udge the
degree of reliability of the obtained results and, accordingly, reliability
of testing.
According to the data of the authors of this method, its use permits
a description of a functional state not only according to absolute
ratings of state of health, activity and mood, which diminish as fatigue
- develops, but also according to the indices of correlation between them
(Figure 8). In a rested person, all three categories of signs are
given similar grades. As fatigue
- - ~ ~ ~ increases the divergence grows _
Arbitrary t,~ as a result of decline of indices
'~~0~' ~ for the state of health and _
activity, as compared to subect-
~ ~ Test.grade
~s~do ' ~ 5;~, ~ ~'Tired. ; ive rating of mood.
_p ,~.~,1.\ `
Methods of sub~ective rating of
the func.tional state are develop-
ing along the line of creating
complex and multiplane tests
- Figure t3. based on the use of modern
Mean divergence between SAIQ test cate- mathematics and assimilation of
gories with different degrees of data accumulated in the area of
fati~;ue (after poskin et al., 1973) traditional use of the scaling
) difference between state of inethod in sub~ective psychophysi.cs.
health and activity However, it would be wrong to
difference between state of believe that development of thia
health and mood direction of research is en-
difference between activity countering only metrological
and mood difficulties.
74 -
FOR OFFICIAL US~ ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Already in the first experimental studies of fatigue it was noted that the
fc~eling of fatigue could be the result of low motivation, lack of interest
in pFrforming the work, and that a change in activity restored the initial
fitness level. Subjective ratings of fatigue also depend on factors
that are extraneous to the operational structure of activity, such as level
of pretentions and degree of imposed responsibility. In addition, sub-
jective ratings are overtly or indirectly related to evaluation of the
diEficulty of the activity performed. This circumstance is related to
the problem of ~onsistency between the subjective ratings of difficulty
of activity performed. For this reason, identification of functional
states solely on the basis of subjective experience and self-evaluation
may be far from reflecting the true state of affairs.
In psychological practice, diagnostics of functional states are most of ten
made on the basis of analysis of efficiency [effectiveness] of performing
a certain type of activity. Analy~is is made of the dynamics of indices _
of quantity, quality and speed of performance, as well as changes in the
corresponding psychological functions upon which performance is based.
The objective of analysis could be to describe the indices of performance
of actual work. The main indices of dynamics of fitness in this case are
the characteristics of total amount of production, number of disruptions
and changes in work pace as related to duration of the work day and
influence of diverse deleterious environmental factors, such as poor
organization of the production process, insufficient light at the work
place and ventilation of rooms, delet~rious factors related to the
specifics of production.
However, the dynamics of labor productivity dep end on many diverse causes,
a significant part of which has no direct bearing on changes in the
functional state of a working man. Moreover, for a large number of oc cu-
pations, this parameter cannot be submitted to quantitative consideration
at all, although the task of determining states is also an important
one for them. For this reason, the main psychological means of so doing
is to use short tests which rate the dynamics of various mental processes
while performing a work assignment. In this case, the problem of evaluat-
ing a functional state emerges as a typical psychometric problem: to
describe and quantitatively rate the changes in psychological charac-
teristics under study that occurred under the influence of certain
causes (in our case, work).
We should include amorig the traditionally used procedures the tests for
determination of absolute and differential sensitivity thresholds in
various modalities, indices of visual fitness, critical flicker fusion
frequency and critical phosphene fusion frequency, analysis of dynamics
of successive patterns. However, the changes observed in these physio-
logical parameters are most often erroneously referred to the group of
physiological tests.
75
FOR OFFZCIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAI. U5E ONLY
A change in functional state from the standpoint of the sensorium is
manifested primarily by changes in sensitivity. Already in the early
studies of fatigue, it was found that there was a decline of tactile
and auditory sensitivity with fatigue. A decline of visual sensitivity
is observed under the influence of the most diverse factors--diverse
deleterious exogenous environmental factors (Figure 9), when performing
work for a long time, with loads of varying intensity, etc. The CFFF
(critical flicker fusion frequency) test is considered to be one of the
most popular and reliable methods of detecting this. In the presence of
fatigue and exposure to diverse stressors, c.-~e observes an a~preciable
decline of this parameter, i.e., decline of visual time resolution
capacity. This is indirect evidence of increased inertia of processes
in the visual system under such conditions,
Threshold
Luc
Normal ormal atmosphere
atmosphere o~ cr~ty cr~a~ "{~"'1
I.p fl1t1~Ua -
~
�
~ 3.s
0
x
3,4
''i ~i'~ Blood CO n,:~:, 1.~~,,;,---1.~�-- ~.r�,. u,rc .
"i 3,2
10 :~U 50 7U !)0 ! 10 I;iU I.iO I i U I!111 1111
Time, min ' ~
Figure 9. Influence of altitude, smoking and air oxygen content
on sensitivity of the eyes to light (after MacFarland,
1946)
Analysis of the dynamics of various manifestations of motor activity of
man is referable to another group of psychometric methods, which cannot
always be clearly differentiated from physiological recording methods.
Along with a strong physiological basis for the study of these character-
istics (first of all, this refers to the vast area of myographic studies),
there are diverse psychological methods of analysis. Different variants
of the step test and tappiiig test are traditional means of diagnosing
functional states.
Wide use is made of diverse methods of evaluating different mental func- -
tions: perception, memory, attention, thinking. Most such psychometric -
methods began to be developed at the first stage of research on the
problem of fatigue, at the end of the 19th century. They include the
well-known Bourdon test, the Kraepelin method of continuous counting of
single-dig~,t numbers, the Pierre-Ruzere elementary coding method, the -
method of testing attention using the Schulte tables, etc. [53]. Numerous
modifications of these tests are still used extensively in psychological
practice.
76
FOR OFFICIAL lia~ ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02148: CIA-RDP82-00850R040240090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USF. ONLY
T1te us~ of psychometric methods is one af the most promising means of solv-
ing the problem of determination of functional sta.tes sin~e., on the one
hand, they directly describe the functional capar~~lities of man and,
on the other hand, they are ob~ective in the se:ise that they rule out
the possibility of deliberate overestimation af fitness. However, most
of the existing methods have two serious flaws.
First of all, the assignments used to determine a functional state have
little in common raith activities that man actually performs. Today
~here is validity, as there was decades ago, to the comment of A. P.
Necliayev that these techniques "make it possible to record changes
that take place in the realm of only a specific aspect of inental life,
and the results obtained by one method cannot always be interpreted as
indications of fatigue" [53, p 16]. The lack of consistency between
testing methods and work activity leads, in many cases, to failure in
- testing functional states in real situations. We can site the results
of ane study [see 81] as a vivid example of this inconsistency between
a test and the task of diagnosing fat3gue. After continuous work for
56 hours on a conveyer, the subjects failec~ to demonstrate an appreciable
decrease in efficiency of perform ing a psychometric test. This could
hardly be attributed to motivational effects; in this case, we would have
to speak of the subject's heroic effort. Most likely this is indicative
of the inadequacy of the chosen testing procedure and insensitivity of
the parameters analyzed.
'1'he ~:uitability of a test for a specific diagnostic problem is determined
by �~:ie central concepts of theory of psychological testing: validity and
reliability [25]. Depending on the purpose of a study, the content of
these concepts can be considered on different levels: from the standpoint
of their theoretical significance, set of statistical procedures for
quantitative descriptj~~n, etc. In the most general sense, validity
reflects the conformity of the chosen method with the research prab lem,
_ while the concept of reliability is used to determine the stability or
reproducibility of obtained evaluations. Meeting the requirements of
validity and reliability implies that there is an adequat~_ theoretical
conception in the spirit of which the test is developed and problems
of standardizing the selected procedure are sc~lved. Fulfilment of these
requirements renders the work dealing with development of effective diag-
nostic tests extremely difficult and labor-consuming.
Another basic flaw of the existing psychometric testing methods is that
Chey can merely evaluate activity in the aspect of its results and, as a
rule, say nothing about the causes of the observed changes. Yet it is a
known fact that work loads lead primarily to mobilization of the body~s
resources and change in work methods without changes in its results [S8].
For this reason, it is imperative, for effective testing, to use a system
of functional tests that determine the state of all elements of the
operational structure of the form of inental activity under study.
77
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
An effort was made to find an experimental solution to the problem of the
effect of fatigue on information processing in short-term memory using
a system of func~ional tests that assess the efficiency of performance
of various operations in the microstructure of short-term memory [40J.
- Por this purpose, a computer-automated system of ~es.*.s was developed,
_ including typical procedures for examining the information-processing -
process: time of recognition reaction, complete reproduction, retrieval
- signal in the presence of interference, idPntification of missing number.
The succe~s of using the different methods is determined by the
ef f iciency of performa� .;:e of certain psychological operations or groups of
- operations that are specific for solving concrete problems.
The proposed system of tests as a whole was found to be suitable for use
- for diagnostic purposes. Under the influence of a load there is substan-
v tial d~crease in efficiency (from the standpoint of correctness and
speed of performance~ of performance of most tasks considered. The
typical signs of dynamics of fitness during a lengthy process of activity
can be readily tracked according to the indices of performance by the
r~ethods in question [40J . In the course of the study, the methods
that were most sensitive to the influence of fatigue were selected;
_ they include the method of retrieving a signal in the presence of inter-
_ ference, recognition, complete reproduction and determination of a
missing number, For each of them, the range of conditions was found,
under which there is maximum expression of the effect of a load. -
Ie was established that fatigue selecCively aff ects performance of the
same operations, distinctive "weak pointe" in the system of information
~ processing. These eff ects include increased duration of information _
- storage in sensory memory, impairment of operations of repetition and -
retrieval of infurmation from primary me~mory, impairment of operations
~ of establishing semantic associations in secondary memory. The duration
, of information storage ~n primary memory, as well as operations of
sensory processin~ of a single stimulus, recognition thereof, transfer to
primary memory and response,remain relatively unchanged.
.
The substantial advantage of this system of tests for short-term memory is
thae it is automated on the basis of computers. Use of computers on the
experimentation line broadens significantly the possibiliCy of using
- diagnastic methods. T~e quality of psychological testing is substantially
improved by complete automation of the main stages of an experiment,
- significant expansion of the range of experimental conditions used (quali- `
tative diversity and unlimited amount of stimulus material, rather broad
range of variation of modes of presenting information, etc.), possibility
of using optimum strategies fo~ conducting the study on the basis of
adequate mathematical procedures for planning the experiment and develop-� ~
- ing programs of the adaptive type. In addition, the uGP of computer
technology makes it possible to process data on the real time scale,
whi.:ii provides an immediate evaluation of a man's functional s~ate.
78
FOR QFFICIAL I1SE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
However, it is not always possible to introduce computers into the area of
practical. studies of functional states. In this case, "small-scale au'~-
mation" i~ helpful, i.e,, portable devices specially developed for
testing by means of a narrow class of psychometric problems within a '
preset limited range of experimental conditions,that are small in size,
~ocivenient to use and portable.
Complex methods of evaluating functional states: Our analysis has shown
l-hat substantial flaws are inherent in all existing methodological direc-
ti~ns of assessing functional states. This problem can be solved only
by means of using complex methods combining the advantages of the differ-
ent approaches discussed. This is the logical conclusion derived from
interpretation of a functional state as ar~ integral characteristic of
existing properties and traits of man, which determine the efficiency of
his performance.
I~ is unlikely that we could find in the current literature any experi-
mental work, in which evaluation of the dynamics of a functional state
of man was made with the use of only one methodological procedure. Evea
in cases where the purpose of the study is to analyze the dynamics of some
special sign, investigators always relate the results to efficiency of
performance of a behavioral problem put to an individual, to data per-
raining to his subjective feelings, etc.
Proof of the need for integral description of functional states of man,
as well as the possible routes of implementing this prin~~iple, were
considered more comprehensively within the framework of analysis of the
main methodological approaches to the problem of diagnosing funetional
states. Solving this problem for physiological research is related to
develo~pment of adequate polyeffector recording methods. But development
of psychological testing method~ is proceeding along the lir~es of develop-
~ ing multipiane subjective tests and various psychometric tests. This is
a mandatory but still far from completed preliminary stage of work. The
next step on the road toward solving the problem of diagno~ing functional _
sCates is the conduct correlation studies and, on their basis, to develop ~
complex systems of tests uf a higher order.
The focal problem in this direction of research is to select, out of the
enormous number of existing methods and means, the most reliable and
convenient ones for practical use. The requirement of practical suita-
bility can he met, in principle, by any method by imgroving the testing
procedure, methods of recording and processing data on the basis of using
modern technology (use of computers in the experiment line, development of
~ portable units, use of adequate mathematical models and methods of statis-
tical analysis). The reliability of the selected methods is determined by
the sensitivity of tr~ parar?eters used and their conformity with specific
testing tasks and condition~.
79
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
With reference to the suitability of some type of parameter of dynamics of
functional-states, the problem of sensitivity of the criteria used is ad-
vanced to the fore. It is important to stress that dissimilar dynamics
in time are inher ent in various manifestations of changes that occur in
the functional state of the body. This well-known fact is drawing more
and more attention on the part of rzsearchers. In one of the experiments,
_ a study was made of the change in efficiency of performing arithmetic
problems, symptoms of subjective stress, dynamics of heart rate and
secretion of catecholamines during prolonged exposure to noise. The re-
sults of this experimented demonstrated not only the existence of typical
dynamics of different parameters in the presence of noise stress (on the
order of the adaptation reaction to excessive loads), but the qualitative
uniqueness of manifestations of deferred effects of stress. Thus, while
the sub3ective feeling of discomfort does not last long and has a tendency
to disaFpear soon, endocrine activity is quite lengthy (from several hours
to 2 days) and increases after termination of stimulation. Behavioral and
- p'iysiological changes are observed both during exposure to a stressor and
fcr a certain time after discontinuing the noise.
From this vantage point, the problem of sensitivity of inethods acquires a
different coloration: the screening of diagnostic parameters nust be made
with du~ consideration of the time interval between exposure to a load
and moment of appearance of maximum changes in the area analyzed.
- Another, more important aspect of the problem of choosing the most sensi-
- tive methods is their consistency to specific types of work activity. The
diagnostic problem is always strictly defined. Investigators are com-
pelled to study certain types of functional states that arise when an
individual has to solve specific behavioral problems. Different types of
work activity make strictly specific demands of man, with respect to
their cuntent (occupational characteristic) and specific working condi-
tions. The load on various elements of the system that provide for the
performance of a specific type of activity is far from the same. But
since the eff iciency of the system as a whole is determined by the state
of the elements that experience the greatest load or bear the most
responsibility for success of the work, the corresponding methods of
studying efficiency should address themselves primarily to these elements.
A series of experimental studies showed that tests chosen on the basis
of analysis of the functional structure of activity are diagnostically
more informative than the standard "universal" methods. Thus, comprehensive
psychophysiological analysis of specific types of work activity is a
mandatory prerequisite for developing complex systems of tests suitable
for evaluating the dynamics of functional states under actual production
condition~.
- 6. Modeling in Ergonomics
Modeling of the structure and function~ of Man-machine systems has become
very popular in ergonomics. There are different types of modeling:
80
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
rOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
ob~ective (in the sense of dealing with an object], objective-mathematical,
symbolic and its most important form, mathematical. In addition, wide
use is made of stochastic modeling, which is based on establishing the
probabilistic relations between events.
Ob~ective modeling, in the course of which the study is conducted on a
model that reproduces the niain geometric, physical, dynamic and functional
characteri.sti~s of the "original," is a typical distinction of many
ergonomic studies [11).
Static and functional models are used [50]. The former are usuall}~ three-
dimensional, real-scale models of equipment, different units thereof,
that are tested. A static model can be used for the following puxposes:
choice of optimum means of arranging equipment; ergonomic evaluation of
equipment and answering questions about its operation that cannot be
answered by means of two-dimensional blueprints; solving problems of _
arrangement of the work place; testing arrangement of controls from the
standpoint of convenience of handling; checking accuracy and speed of
instrument readings; determining accessibility of check points, testing
and ad~ustment points in the course of technical maintenance of equipment.
A functional model is a model of equipment on a real scale and, unlike
the static model, it can reproduce the actual operation of equipment i,:
manual and automatic control settings. Simulators intended for training
of specialists and used to study and solve problems of planning [design]
of the corresponding type of activity can be classified as this type of
model. The functional models used in ergonomics are experimental models
of the man-machine system or its subsystem, constructed in accordance with
certain rules, the properties of which determine man's performance in
such a manner that its main characteristics correspond to the parameters
of performance in a real system [76]. There can be significant expansion
of the use of functional models in ergonomics with the use of electronic
and computer technology as programming and analytical devices.
A functional mode~. can be used to study the work activity of a man (or
group of people) under simulated working conditions in order to compare
alternative variants of design (or to check the only chosen one), as well
as to evaluate the different features of equipment. Thus, a 1:1 scale
prototype of a lathe and special stand, which permits ongoing reproduction
of_spatial conditions of lathe operator work, were developed to check
draft proposals and provide ergonomic substantiation for artistic designs
of hydraulic copying lathe with programmed control. A series of three-
dimensional models of the lathe and work zone is successively reproduced
on the stand by means of sliding metal rods and detachable ["suspended"?]
equipment si~nulating the main working elements of the lathe (clamping
chuck, tailstock, etc.). Bioelectrical activity of muscles was recorded
while sub~ects worked with a specific model. The obtained myograms
made it possible to select one out of several tested variants, the ' _
81
FOI' OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USF. nNLY
dimensions and geometric shape of which caused minimal muscular tension
in the lathe operator to maintain the working pose (36].
There is an acute need in ergonomics to use mathematical modeling methods.
More recently, large amounts of models of human factors have appeared in
engineering. However, by far not each of them does indeed simulate the
process under study, and not infrequently modeling is transformed into
a game with mathematical symbols. Nevertheless, this does not warrant
questioning the fact that the desire to provide a mathematical descrip-
tion for human factors as a whole is unquestionably instrumental in
development of ergonomic theory and practice. The main problems that
arise are related to demonstration of the entire set of psychophysiological
properties and traits of man that are essential to his activity in the
system. Expressly these must be reflected in the corresponding mathemati-
cal models that are called upon to provide a quantitative description of
this activity [64].
Methods have been developed, in which such characteristics are submitted
to quantitative modeling as quality of operator performance, competence and
professsional performance of operators, their psychological orientation
("personal," "collectivistic," "business-like"), mental tension (stress),
morale and solidarity of the group and others [35, 37]. Work is being
conducted to systematize models intended for description of human
perfcrmance in concrete modes of operation of the man-nachine system [6].
The use of simulator models in ergonomic and engineering psychological
research of man-machine systems is related primarily to the desire to
cover in one description both man and the technical components of the
system, the need to describe processes of man-machine system function
in a generalized form that would permit distinction and study of
subsystems and rel~tions between them, as well as the desire to be rid
of detai~ed descriptions of intrasystem processes [32]. One of the most
promising directions of development of modeling for the purpose of
planning human activity is the use of the theoretical and mathematical
system of game theory [31]. Ergonomics needs mathematical methods for
planning and processing experimental data. Planning an experiment, which
refers primarily to the system of conceptions of rational strategy of -
a concrete study [44J, is an important condition for effective develop-
ment of ergonomics as an area uf scientific and practical endeavor.
7. Us~; of Computers in Ergonomic Research
Construction of adequate models of man's activity requires consideration
= of an ever increasing number of factors and correlations between them,
which leads to constant complication of models and methods of working
with them. Tt is important that such models consist of either "poor"
equations, which cannot be solved analytically, or systems of a large -
number of equations or, finally, logically complex constructions with a
82
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
large number of connections and conditions. In most case, it is basically
Impc~ssible to work with such models without using computer technology.
'I'he every-day requirements of practice stimulate even more the penetra-
tion of computers into the azea of concrete research. Among sur.h problems,
we can mention the need to obtain a sufficient amount of experimental
results within a relatively short time, development of a system (bank)
of standard reference data in ergonomics, extrapolation of results ob- .
tained from laboratory sCudies to real working conditions, to obtain the
quantitative characteristics of man's capabilities when performing differ-
ent types of cognitive and productive [executor] activity. ~
Such problems can be solved effectively only on the basis of total and
partial automation of diverse ergonomic studies. It is only on this
route that it is possible to change to "industrialization" and standardiza-
tion of investigative methods with wide use of quantitaLive ratings which,
in turn, would permit increasing the reliability and comparability of
results of different studies.
Processing of experimental results (surveys, questionnaires, behavioral
parameters, physiological parameters, etc.) is the most accessible (and
popular) form of using computers. The use of computers is attributable
to its capacity to work with large arrays of data at a speed that is
greater by several orders of magnitude than human capabilities. In addi-
tion, computer processing makes it possible to use more powerful equipment
for analysis of experimental results than any that is available for
"manual" processing. Suffice it to mention asanexample the ma~y types of
multidimensional analysis (special correlation, multiple regression, etc.).
- Until recently, experimental studies were conducted in two stages: first
the experiment proper (information gathering), then analysis and pro-
cessing of the information obtained. Computers were used primarily at
the second stage. There are examples of automation of only the first
stage, direct conduct of an experiment, for example, to present informa-
tion within a specific time following a strict program prepared before -
the experiment.
However, in many cases, such a two-stage procedure is extremely ineffective,
since the lack of coordination in gathering data in the course of the
experiment leads to storage and processing of a large amount of super-
fluous information. Moreover, the surplus of "raw material" makes it
difficult and sometimes impossible to single out the sought patterns.
One way to overcome these difficulties is to conducted automated experi-
r~ents, in which the computer monitors the progress of the experiment,
processing data as they are received (on a real time scale) and selects
the required strategy for running the experiment. Such use of computers
appears to be the most efficient. It is then possible not only to make
an ongoing study of numerous characteristics in the course of one
83
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
l~OR OHFLCIAI. U5E ONLY
examination, but to conduct experiments that are basically unfeasible with
the use of any other technical base, since the need arises in such experi-
ments to make decisions accordii:g to some rather complex algorithms
within periods measured in milliseconds.
Thus, various tasks are put to computer technology in modern experimental
research: data gathering, data processing, control of large complexes of -
devices with adherence to rather strict schedules and, finally, implementa-
tion of adaptive and even self.-optimizing controlled experiments. However,
there are many difficulties in the way of effective use of computer
technology in ergonomics and allied scietitiifc disciplines. We can men-
, tion such circumstances as the need for the researcher to acquire skill in
solving many problems that are not customary for him. In particular,
there are the problems of inputting data in the computer, eliminating
superfluous material, excluding artefacts, convenient method of presenting
the final results of processing, programming, etc., for each computer with
its own technical features.
The diversity of existing computers, which are being manufactured or
designed, also creates the difficult problem of selection of the type of
computer. The exceptionally rapid development of computer technology,
frequent change in types and generations of computers, their software
- and programming languages lead to a situation that, from the standpoint
of the consumer, the computers may become obsolete after barely starting
to be used.
However, the focal problem that will determine the efficiency of using
- computer technology in ergonomics is primarily that of formulating
specific problems to be solved with computers. The widespread opinion
that "machines can do everything" is not always "t,y far associated with
awareness of the fact that it would be futile to use a computer without
a properly formulated problem. After all, computers do not simply
- "calculate rapidly." Virtually any attempt at using them is primarily
limited by the extent of our ignorance. The task for a computer cannot
be simply to study a certain phenomenon. There is the mandatory stage
of preparing the proper algorithm for solving a problem in all its
- details. It happens that the need for the experimene proper no longer
exists as a result of such preparatory work. The success in solving some
problem or other depends on the level of expounded hypotheses and
re.finement of models to a much larger extent than on the use of modern
technology per se. And this work is still the prerogative of man, at
least fore the foreseeable future.
At the present time, more or less traditional mathematical methods borrowed
from engineering sciences are used in ergonomics in the transition to
analysis of data with computers: information, signal processing, operation
testing, pattern recognition theories, etc. But when ~etting up a specific
84
FOR OFFICiAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
ex.~eriment, it is sometimes necessary to either modify these traditional
mf~th~ds as rellted to the problem to be solved, or to develop new methods
and a l ~orithr.:~ .
The hardware used in ergo:~mic researc~ also (with rare exceptions) coneists
� of standard devices and instruments that are not specially designed for use
in this field. For this reason, as a rule it is necessary to make some
effort to adapt this hardware to the conditions of an ergonomic experiment
proper.
It should also be noted that the use of computers makes it necessary to
basically alter the entire structure of an experiment. At the same time,
pl.annin~ the experiment, extent of modification of experimental procedures _
and the equipment involved depend on the means of using a computer. As
an example, we can mention here some of the problems that arise when a
computer is used in a nonindependent mode (on the line of the experiment):
alienation of the experimenter from direct participation in the experiment
requires the use of complex and diverse procedures for regular checking
of all equipment; for the same reason, the instructions to subjects have
to be basically changed; it is impossible with given technical features
~ of a computer to evaluate on a real time scale some of the traditionally
used parameters, which may make it necessary to study other characteris-
tics, etc.
However, it should be borne in mind that the most thorough formulation of
problems and proper use of mathematic~~_1 methods does not guarantee immedi-
ate success and does not prevent the disappointment of those c~ho expect
too much from "computerization" of studies. And this may not be a matter
of particular mistakes and oversight on the part of the investigator or -
flaw in the computer or methods used; it could be the consequence of
wrong choice of approaches to analysis of ergonomic problems which were
generated in studies of physical systems that are immeasurably simpler
than the phenomena mentioned. It may be tha~ in .principle, the existing
- algorithmic methods are not applicable for interpretation of data of
ergonomic and psychophysioZogical studies. An analogy is suggested here
to the problems encountered by researchers involved in computer transla-
tions. Their solution generated a radical change in views about the
structure of language and, furthermore, about formulation of the problem
- itself. Thus, in analysis of human factors, psychological phenomena and
"languages of the brain," it will perhaps be necessary, in time, to
alter substantially the existing approaches [48].
85
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
' BIBLIOGRAPHY
1. Myasnikov, V. A., and Petrov, V. P. (editors) "Digital Systems of
Aviation Monitoring and Control," Leningrad, Mashinostroyeniye, 1976.
2. Akov, R. L. "Systems, Organization and Interdisciplinary Research,"
in "Issledovaniya po obshchey teorii sistem" [Studies of General
System Theory], Moscow, Progress, 1969.
3. Alekseyev, H. G., and Semenov, I. N. "Types of Systemic Conception
of Operator Work," TEKHNICHESKAYA ESTETIKA [Esthetic Design in
- EngineeringJ, No 4-5, 1977.
4. Anan'yev, B. G. "Problems of Modern Human Sciences,"
Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
5. Aseyev, V. G. "The Froblem of Monotony in Studies of Foreign
Authors," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII [Problems of Psychology], No 1, 1975.
6. Akhutin, V. M., and Naftul'yev, A. I. "Mathematical Modeling of
Operator Performance in Developing Ergonomic Systems," in "Chelovek
i obshchestvo. Sotsial'nyye problemy upravlen~.ya" [Man and Society.
Social Problems of Management (or control)], edited by Yu. A.
Dmitriyeva, Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad University, 1973.
7. Bassin, F. V. "Development of Views on the Subject of Psychology,"
VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 4, 1971.
8. Berezkin, B. S., and Zinchenko, V. P. "Studies of Information
Retrieval," in "Problemy inzhenernoy psikhologii" [Problems of
Engineering Psychology], Moscow, Nauka, 1967.
9. Bernshteyn, N. A. "General Biomechanics," Mos~ow, 1926.
10. Idem, "Essays on Physiology of Activity," Moscow, 1959.
11. Biryukov, B. V.; Gastev, Yu. A.; and Geller, Ye. S. "Modeling,"
BSE [Great Soviet Encyclopedia], 3d edition, Moscow, Sovetskaya
- entsiklopediya, V~1 16, 1974.
12. Biryukov, B. V., and Celler, Ye. S. "Cybernetics in the Humanities,"
rioscow, Nauka, 1973.
13. Btok, V. "Wakefulness Levels and Attention," in "Fksperimental~naya
psikhologiya" [Experimental Psychology], edited by P. Fraisse and
J. Piaget, Moscow, Progress, 1970.
86
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAI, USE ONLY
1'v, l:obn~~va, M. I. "En~ineering Psychology and Design," in "Problemy
c~khnicheskoy estetiki" (Problems of Esthetics in Engineering],
~~~scc~w, Iskusstvo, ~lyp 3, 1970.
15. B~~gdanuv, V. A.; Solovova, L. M.; and Sukhodol'skiy, G. V. "Study
~ind E:valuation of Some Information Display Devices for Atomic
- I'ower Plants," in "Psikhologiya--proizvodstvu i vospitaniyu"
[Psychology for Industry and Education], Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad
University, 1977.
lh. I3uslenko, N. P.; Kalashnikov, V. V.; and Kovalenko, I. N. "Lectures
o~i CumplEx System Theory," Moscow, Sovetskoye radio, 1973.
l7. Bushurova, V. Ye., and Naftul'yev, A. I. "The Problem of Planning
Operator Activity in Control Systems and Methods for Sovling It,"
i~t "Chelovek i obshchestvo. Sotsial'nyye problemy upravleniya,"
1,eningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad University, 1973�
18. Velichkovskiy, B. M.; Kapitsa, M. S.; and Zszen, N. V. "Digit
Recognition in Stroboscopic Motion," "NOVOYE V PSIKHOLOGII [News in
Psychology], Vyp 1, 1975.
19. Velichkovskiy, B. M. "Visual Memory and Models of Human Information
Processing," VOPROS~ PSIKHOLOGII, No 6, 1977. '
20. (Jenkin, A. A., and Medvedev, V. I. "Prediction of Psychophysio-
logical States," Leningrad, Nauka, 1973.
'L1. Clezer, V. D., et al. "Visual Recognition and Its Neurophysiological
Meclianisms," Leningrad, 1975.
"?2. Gorshkov, S. I.; Zolina, Z. M.; and Moykin, Yu. V. "Research Methods
in Industrial Physiology," Moscow, Meditsina, 1974.
23. C:orbav, F. D. "Experimental Group Psychology," in "Problemy
inzhenernoy psikhologii," Leningrad, Vyp 4, 1966.
24. Gubinskiy, A. I., and Yevgrafov, V. G. "Ergonomic Planning of
Ship Control Systems," Leningrad, Sudostroyeniye, 1977.
25. Gurevich, K. M. "Profesaional Fitness and Chief Properties of the
Nervous Sys^..em," Moscow, Nauka, 1970.
26. .Jo..as, J. K. "Engineering and Artistic Design. Modern Methods of
Design Analysis," translated from English, Moscow, Mir, 1976.
~
87
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
27. Dobrolenskiy, Yu. P.; Zavalova, N. D.; Ponomarenko, V. A.; and
Tuvayev, V. A. "Methods of Engineering Psychology Studies in
Aviation," Moscow, Mashinostroyeniye, 1975.
28. Donskoy, D. D. "Biomechanics," Moscow, Prosveshcheniye, 1975.
29~ Dubrovskiy, V. Ya, and Shchedrovitskiy, L. P. "Problems of Engineer-
ing Psychology Related to Systems Planning," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow
University, 1971.
30. Yermakov, S. V., and Strokina, A. N. "Program of Anthropometric
Studies as Related to Ergonomic Problems," in "Ergonomika. Printsipy
i rekomendatsii" [Ergonomics. Principles and RecommEndations], Moscow,
zzd. VNIITE, vyP 6, 1974.
31. Zhuravlev, G. Ye. "Problems of Application of Game Theory in
_ Psychology," in "Psikhologiya i matematika" [Psychology and MathematicsJ,
- V. F. Rubakhin editor-in-chief, Moscow, Nauka, 1976.
32. Zhuravlev, G. Ye.; Rubakhin, V. F.; and Subbotin, F. A. "Simulation
Modeling of Group Operator Work," Ibid.
33. Zarakovskiy, G. M. "Psychophysiological Analysis of Work Activity,"
Moscow, Nauka, 1966.
34. Zarakovskiy, G. M.; Medvedev, V. I~; and Munipov, V. M. "Principles
Involved in Ergonomic Descriptions of Operator Activity," in
"Ergonomika. Printsipy i rekomendatsii," Moscow, Izd. VNIITE,
Vyp 2, 1972.
35. Zarakovskiy, G. M.; Korolev, B. A.; Medvedev, V. I.; and Shlayen, P. Ya.
"Introduction to Ergonomics," Moscow, Sov. radio, 1974.
36. Zefel'd, V. V. "Preplanning Ergonomic Modeling," TEKHNICHESKAYA
ESTETIKA, No 2, 1974.
37. Segal, A., and Wolf, J. "Models of Grot.~p Behavior in Man-Machine
System With Consideration of Psychosoca~al and Industrial Factors,"
translated from English, Moscow, Mir, '1973.
38. Zinchenko, V. P. "Microstructural Method of Studying Cognitive
Activity," in "Ergonomika. Trudy VNIITE" [Ergonomics. Works of
the All-Union Scientific Research Institute of Aesthetic Styling
in Engineering], Vyp 3, 1972.
8$
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
39. Ziachenko, V. P., and Gordon, V. M. "Methodological Problems of Psy-
ct~ological Analysis of Activity," in "Sistemnyye issledovaniya.
Yezhtgodnik, 1975" [Resea.ch on Systems. 1975 Annual], Moscow,
Nauka, 1976.
40. "Linchenko, V. P.; Leonova, A. B.; and Strelkov, Yu. K. "~sychom~trics
of Fatigue," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University, 1977.
41. Zinchetiko, V. P.; Munipov, V. M.; and Smolyan, G. L. "Ergonomic
Bases of Organization of Labor," Moscow, Ekonomika, 1974.
42, "Engineering Psy~hology. Theory, Methodology and Practical Applica-
tions," Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
43. Lomov, B. F., and Petrov, V. I. (editors) Engineering Psychology
Applied to Equipment Besign," translated from English, Moscow,
M~.ishinostroyeniye, 1973.
44. "Inf~rmation Materials," KIBERNETIKA [Cybernetics], No 6(100),
rtoscow, Izd. USSR Academy of Sciences, Scientific Council for the
Complex Problem of "Cybernetics," 1977.
45. Koti.k, M. A. "Self-Regulation and Rel;ability of the Human Operator,"
Vil'nyus, 1974. -
46. I.azai~~, R. Stress Theory and Psychophysiological Studies," in
"Emotsional'nyy stress" [Emotional StressJ, edited by L. Levi,
I.eningrad, Meditsina, 1970.
~
- 47. Leonov, A. B. "The Problem of Sub~ective Diagnosis of Fatigue,"
TEKHNICHESKAYA ESTETIKA, No 9, 1977.
48. Lomov, B. F.; Nikolayev, V. I.; and Rubakhin, V. F. "Some Aspects
of Using Mathematics in Psychology," in "Matematika i psikhologiya"
[Mathematics and Psychology], editor-in-chief: V. F. Rubakhin,
Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
49. Medvedev, V. I. Fuactional States of Operators," in "Ergonomika.
Printsipy i rekomendaksii," Moscow, Izd. VNIITE, Vyp 1, 1970.
50. Meister, D., and Rabedesu, R. G. "Engineering Psychological Ra~ting
in Development of Control Systems," translated from Engiish, Moscow,
Sov. radio, 1970. -
51. Kuz'min, Ye. S., and Semenov, V. Ye. (editors) "Methods of Social -
Psychology," Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad University, 1977.
89
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY _
52. Nebylitsin, V. D. "Main Properties of the Human Nervous System,"
Moscow, Pedagogika, 1976.
53. ~iechayev, A. P. (editor) "Mental Fatigue," Moscow--Leningrad, GIZ
[State Publishing HouseJ, 1929.
54. Novikov, M. A. "Principles and Methods of Group Screening," in
"Materialy III Vsesoyuznogo s"yezda obshchestva psikhologov SSSR"
[Proceedings of 3d All-Union Congress of USSR Society of Psycholo-
gists], Moscow, Vol 3, Vyp 1, 1968.
55. Ogurtsov, A. P.; Razumov, A. Ye.; and Yudin, E. G. "The Scientific
and Technological Revolution, and Distinctions of Modern Scientific
+ Knowledge," Moscow, Znaniye, 1977.
56. O1'shanskiy, V. B. "Sotsiometriya," BSE [Great Soviet Encyclopedia],
Moscow, Sovetskaya entsiklopediya, 3d ed., Vol 24 (I), 1976.
57. Paillard, J. "Use of Physiological Parameters in Psychology,"
in "Experimental Psychology," edited by P. Fraisse and J. Piaget,
Moscow, Vyp 3, 1970.
58. Platonov, K.. K. "Problems of Industrial Psychology," Moscow, 2d ed., -
1570.
59. Tochilov, K. S. (editor) "Manual of Industrial P~ysiology,"
Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad University, 1970.
60. "Problems of Engineering Psychology," Leningra.d, Vyp 4, 1956.
61. "The Process of Social Research. Problems of Methodology, Methods
and Organization of Marxist-Leninist Social Studies," translate~i
from German, Moscow, Progress, 1975.
62. Rubakhin, V. F. (editor-in-chie�) "Psychology and Mathematics,"
Moscow, Nauka, 1976.
63. Rozenblat, V. V. "Problems of Fatigu~," Ptoscow, Nauka, 2d ed., 1975.
64. Ronzhin, 0. V. "Information Methods of Studying Ergatic Systems,"
Leningrad, Mashinostroyeniye, 1976.
65. Izrael'son, Z. I., and Tarasenko, N. Yu. (editors) "Manual for
Practical Studies of Industrial Hygi.ene," Moscow, Meditsina, 1973.
66. Safonov, V. K. "Forecasting Operator Reliability in Industrial
Activity," in "Psikhologiya--proizvodstvu i vospitaniyu," Leningr.ad,
Izd-~vo Leningrad University, 1977.
~ 90
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
b7, E~arpova, B. D., and Kovshilo, V. Ye. (editors) "Guide on Industrial
Hy~;iene," Leningrad, Meditsina, 1976.
6~. S;SK[lOdGl'skiy, G. V. "Evaluation of Lay~u*_ of Instrument Panels and
Control Panels," in "Metodologiya issle:iovaniy po inzhenernoy
psikhologii i psikhologii truda" [Methndology of Research in
Engineering and Industrial Psychology], Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad
Universi.ty, Pt 2, 1975.
69. Teplov, B. N1. "Problems of Individual Differences," Moscow, 1961.
70. "Reinforcement of Correlation Between Social, Natural and Engineering
' Sciences," KOMMUNIST, No 1, 1977.
71. Fridman, L. M. "Routes of Development of Mathematical Psychology,"
VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 4, 1970.
72. Idem, "Some Problems of Modeling and Conversion to Mathematics in -
Psychology," Ibid, No 5, 1974.
73. Khomskaya, Ye. D. "The Brain and Activation," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow `
University, 1972.
74. Chernysheva, 0. N.; Ivanova, Ye. M.; Stroicina, A. N.; and Lidova, B. V. -
"Sume Methods of Ergonomic Analysis of Work Under Industrial Condi-
' tion~," in "Ergonomika. Printsipy i rekomendatsii," Moscow, Izd.
VNIITE, Vyp 2, 1971.
75. Shtoff, V. A. "Modeling in Philosophy," Moscow--Leningrad, Nauka,
1966.
75. "Ergonomics," Trudy VNIITE, Moscow, Vyp 10, 1976. _
77. Yudin, E. G. "Methodological Analysis, Its Main Objectives and
~orms," POLITICHESKOYE SAMOOBRAZOVANIYE [Political Self-Education],
- No 8, 1975.
7~, Yadov, V. A. "Sociological Research," Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad
University, 1972.
79. Bartly, S. H., and Chute, E. F. "Fatigue and Impairment in Man,"
New York, 1947.
80, Bartlett, F. C. "Psychological Criteria of Fatigue," in "Symposium
of Fatigue"(ed. by W. F. Floyed and A. T. Welford), Londo~i, 1953.
,
81. Cameron, C. A. "Theory of Fatigue,"in "Man Under Stress" (ed. by
~1. T. Welford), London, 1974.
91
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
~ _ , . , .
.i''r T
~~i~i~ i~ . ~ i i~1~#~#~#'~~t~ ~~~i~ it . ~~~i i ~ ~ _
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
_ 82. Campbell, F. W., and Robson, J. G. "Application of Fourier Analysis
to the Visibility of Gratings," J. PHYSIOL., 1968, 197.
83. Eysenck, M. Wo "Human Memory: Theory, Research and Individual
Differences," Oxford, 1977.
84. Gibson, J. J. "The Perception of Visual World," Boston, 1950.
85. Kaufman, L. "Sight and Mind," New York, 1974.
86. Kelly, D. H. "Frequency Doubling in Visual Responses,~' J. OPT. SOC.
AM., 1966, 56.
87. Meister, D. "Behavioral Foundations of System Development," Wiley,
I3ew York, 1976.
88. Idem, "Human Factors: TY:eory and Practice," New York, 1971.
89. Neisser, U. "Cognitive Psychology," New York, 1967. ~
9U. Phillips, W. A., and Christie, D. F. M. "Components of Visual
Memory," Q. J. EXP. PSYCHOL., 1977, 29.
91. Rieawyl, H., and Schafroth, M. "Graphic Expression of Multidimen-
sional Observations," EDV IN MEDIZIN U. BIOLOGIE, No 7, 1976.
92. Sternberg, S. "Memory Scanning: New Findings and Current Contro-
versies," in "Short-Term Memory;~D. Dentsch and J. A. Deutsch,
New York, 1975.
92
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02108: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
CHAPTER IV. PRINCIPLES IN ERGONOMIC ANALYSIS OF WORK ACTIVITY
The category of activity is the most important in the system of developing
ergonomic science. Labor is performed in various forms of objective -
[object-rela~ed] practical, industrial, cognitive and contro.l activity.
"Activity is a specifically human form of attitude toward the world
around man, the content of which is purposeful change and transformation
_ of this world" [62, p 267-268]. To man, the objects in nature lose inherent _
purpoae and emerge as objects, i.e., primarily as the means of making
- tools. The use of work tools implies that there is a set goal that
man is governed by as the ideal image of the required product. K. Niarx
described this basic distinction of work activity in the following manner:
"At the end of a wark process a result is obtained that was already -
in man's mind at the start of this process, i.e., the ideal. Man not
only alters the form of what was given by nature; at the same time he
achieves his conscious goal through what was given by nature, and like
a law it determines the means and nature of actions, and man must
subordinate his will to this goal" [1, p 189]. This quote clearly
indicates the main structural elements of work activity: goal as the ideal
conceptian of the result, method or means of reaching it and, finally,
will, i.e., specific meaningful personality-related elements.
In ergonomics, activ;ty emerges as the subject of objective scientific
study. It is broken down and reproduced in theoretical schemes and models,
in accordance with the methodological principles developed in science,
and in accordance with specific ergonc,~ic tasks. In ergonomics, activity
also emerges as the object of control., i.e., that which must be or-
ganized into a well-adjusted system of operation and (or) development on
the basis of the aggregate of fixed principles that must be formulated in
ergonomics, social psychology and industrial sociology. In ergonomics,
activity emerges also as the ob~ect of planning (design], i.e., ergonomics
is faced with the task of demonstrating the methods and conditions for
optimum implementation of certain (chiefly new) types of work (and
professional-educational) activity. Finally, in ergonomics activity also
emerges as the object of multilevel evaluation, which must be made in
accordance with various criteria, such as efficiency, reliability, satis-
faction with work, comfort, etc. Thus, activity emerges in ergonomics as
the beginning, content and end of ergonomic analysis, organization, planning
93
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047102/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
and evaluation. Of course, this very general description of runctions of
activity can only play the role of a methodological guideline for ergonomic
" research. In order to solve scientific and practical problems of ergonomics, _
a certain constructive meaning must be imparted to the concept of activity.
This is by no means a simple task, since conceptual schemes of analysis of
activity have not yet been sufficiently developed in ergonomics. For this
reason, wide use is made in ergonamics of the conceptual schemes for
analysis of work activity that exist in allied disciplines, particularly
psychology and sociology. These conceptual schemes are not only assimi-
- lated by ergonomics, but transformed in accordance with the specifics of -
the probl~ms it has to solve. Ergonomics is compelled to develop analy-
tical methods and demonstrate the functional structures of various types
of work activity, ranging fram relatively elementary ones to those of
utmost complexity, which were generated by the sc~entifi_c and technological
revolution. This is the mandatory prerequisite for optimization of work
activity, rational planning of new types and forms thereof. Otherwise,
such problems are solved either on the t~asis of common sense, or empirical
examination of many factors which have some influence or other on effi-
ciency and other aspecCs of work activity, i.e., by the method of
successive approximations.
Before we describe the functional structure af work activity, units of
analysis thereof and types of relations between them, we must describe
the main types of work activity.
1. Classification of Working Occupations
In the historical aspect, a distinction is made between three main stages
of development of technology and labor or the "technology--man"
- system: manual labor, mechanized labor, automate3 labor. All three types
of labor exist in modern industry. Ergonomics, having emerged at the
stage of automated labor, nevertheless is related to all three types
thereof. Ergonomics needs an orderly classification of modern types of
labor. At the present stage, it is deemed expedient to use the classi-
fication developed at the USSR Central Statistical Administration for
grouping workers (occupations) according to tne factor [tag] of inechani-
zation of labor to compile a list of workers according to occupation.
Such separation of workers into groups according to mechanization of
labor was used with success in sociological analysis of labor problems _
[59]. According to this classification, there are five groups of workers
differing in degree of inechanization of labor activity.
The first group refers to workers who use automated machines, apparatus
and installations. This includes workers who monitor the operation of
automatic and semiautomatic units, parts, apparatus, lathes, etc.,
regulate the mode of their operation, adjust and fix them. This group
also includes workers with semiautomatic machines, lathes.and apparatus
94
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
:
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
if their duties are also to monitor and regulate the operation of the semi-
automatic machines, as well as repairmen and adjusters of semiautomatic '
machines whose function is mainly observation.
The second group refers to those who work by means of machines, lathes,
devices, apparatus and mechanized instruments (lathe operators, machine
operators, drivers, tractor operators, instrument control workers, motor
mechanics, pick-hammer operators, ~as and electric welders, etc.).
Direct control of a machine or apparatus is inherent primarily in all
these woi:~ers. Within this group, a separation is sometimes made into
subgroups, depending on the sophistication of work tools used.
The third group refers to those working manually with machines and
devices who supplement with their manual labor the operation of the
machines (shop hands): loaders of containers and transporters; sorters,
packagers, wrappera, washers, dispensers and other workers enaged near
machines and devices. This group of workers can perform utterly ana-
logous work to an equal ex tent involving both nonautomatic machines, ~
- automatic and semiautomatic ones. Unskilled and usually monotonous labor
is inher.ent in this entire group.
The fourth group iefers to workers engaged in manual labor or who use
unmechanized tools, not in contact with machines and devices, i.e., purely
manual labor (unskilled labor of the .~anufacturing type, highly skilled
workers of the craft type, highly skilled labor involving manual
complex assembly and adjustment of intricately assemblgd products.
The fifth group refers to workers who overhaul machines and devices,
locksmiths [fitters, mechanics], electrical fitters, electrical assem~:,lers
and repairmen, including attendants. This group also includes repairmen,
adjusters of lathes and machines, instrument installers whos~ main
function is adjustment [trouble shooting].
At the same time, workers in repair groups, sections, shops, factories
and plants who are not involved in complex machinery overhaul but
specialized operations, can be referred to any of the first four groups.
For example, lathe hands and other lathe operators, gas and electric
welders i_.volved in repair work are classified in the second group as
workers who perform their work with the help of machines and mech~inized ~
tools.
In this classification, there is overlapping of jobs involving manual,
machine and automated production. Critical comments have been voiced
about ~he strictness of classification in the third and fourth groups, `
- which include both unskilled (shop hands, riggers, loaders, etc.) and
highly skilled (fitters, electrical assemblers, fitter-tool makers, etc.)
manual laborers. Nevertheless, a better classification of types of
work activity has not yet been devised.
G~
` FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
For the purposes of ergonomic analysis, the occupations are broken do4m
even more in most cases. Thus, worker~ involved with automated control
systems, or operators (first group) are ::ubdivided into five types, in
accordance with which there are five classes of operator activity.
1. Operator-technologist. The operator is directly involved in the
technological process; he works chiefly in the mode of immediate servicing,
performing primarily actuating ~executory] actions governed by instructions
that strictly regulate the actions, which usually ~~ntain a complete set
of situations and decisions. These are operators oi technological pro-
cesses, automatic lines, operators who perform the functions of formal re-
codin~ and transmission of information.
2. Operator-manipulator [keyer]. In this case the main role is played for .
operator by mechanisms of sensorimotor activity, as well as figurative
and conceptual thinking, though to a lesser extent. Among the funcTObots
of an operator-manipulator are the control of manipulators [keys), ,
machines that amplify muscular energy. The same category includes
operators that service radar stations, which is a classical object of
research in engineering psychology. True, the activity of these operators
could be referred with equal ju~tification to the next type, the activity
of an operator-observer, since an enormous share of the load in perform-
ing tracking functions, monitoring targets against the background of
interference, is also borne by the visual system.
3. Operator-observer, controller [monitor]. This is the classical type
of operator (operator for tracking at a radar station, transFort system
- dispatcher, etc.). This type of activity is characterized by the
large "weight" of information and conceptual models; accordingly control
skills are somewhat reduced (as compared to the first twa types of
operator activity). He can work in both the mode of im~z~ediate and
deferred servicing. This is the predominant type of activity for operators
of engineering [technological] systems operating on a real time scale.
4. Operator-researcher [investigator]. Such an operator makes considerably
more use of conceptual thinking and experience contained in graphic-concep-
tual models. Control elements play an even smaller part for him, while
the "weight" of information models, on the contrary, is substantially
increased. Such operaCors include researchers of any field: computer
system users, object (image) decoders, etc.
5. Operator-administrator. He does not manage the technological compon-
= ents of systems or machines, but other people. This management is done
either directly or indirectly, through technical equipment and communica-
tion channels. Such operators include organizers, administrators on
different levels, individuals who make important decisions,who have the
appropriate knowledge, experience, tact, will power, skill in decision
96
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
making and intuition. Operator-administrators must work, not only ~.ith
an object, they must consider not only the capabilities and limitations
of the machine components of a system, but take full consideration of
the dietinctions of their subordinates, their capabilities and limita-
tions, condition and mood. Dynamic [operational] thinking is the main
mode of activity of the operator-administrator.
Witt~ all i.ts flaws, this classif ication of operator activity clears the
way for coordination of exogenous ways and means of activity and it
permits, at least at the start, better orientation of research and practical
work in ergonomics [39]..
The area of ergonomic research covers grimarily the types of work activity
that are related to the use of equipment [technical means]. Work that
is done by riand is sometimes i.ncluded in the area studied by ergonomics;
there are se~eral ergonomic publications dealing with problems of manual
labor.
Thus far, a universal c.lassification of equipment has not been developed,
which makes it difficult to develop its ergot~omic classification, a need
for which is being felt more and more acutely ~~�cause of the necessity
~ of preparing ergonomic specifications and recounnen.:~tions for specific
types of equipment. The following are objects of ergo.~omics: industrial
equipment (machines, machinery, instruments, apparatus fo: the control of
machines aiid technological processes, transport, communications, etc.);
nonindustrial equipment (equipment for municipal and household services
[util~ties],* as well as military equipment (tackets, rocket installations,
aircraft, sk~ips and submarines, etc.).
- For purposes of preliminary analysis, of interest is the general classi-
fication of tools and means of labor according to degree of their automa-
_ tion [43], which permits schematic presentation of the main ob~ects of
ergonomic analysis:
*It was indicated above that cultural and everyday products, as well as
household products have already become the object of ergonomic research.
In arder to evaluate them properly and design the consumer propertxes
of such products, their use should also be considered as a special
form of activity, as consumer activity. The similarity of dynamic and
technical components of work activity and consumer activity should not
lead into error. Their goals, motivation and results are basically
diffeerent, as are the requirements concerning the conditions for their
use and degree of comfort. Consumer activity has yet to become the
subject of a special investigation.
\
97
FOR OFFICIAI USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
a) manual tool and most elementary device
b) mechanized and electric-powered tool
c) machines without compulsory connection of operating element with
object of labor, which operate when the woricer that services them
is working
d) isolated semiautomatic machines, in which there is compulsory
connection of operating element with the object of labor, but
without au~omatic loading and unloading of materials and products
e) isolated automatic machines, in which there is automation of all
processes in the work cycle, supplying materials and putting out
ready products
f) semiautomatic units (combines, assemblies), in which all processes
are automated, with the exception of loading material and unload-
- ing ready products, usually consisting of a combination of various
mechanical devices (for example, lathe and transmission mechanism)
� g) automated units, in which all processes are automated including
maintaining a set mode and methods of inputing control programs
The ergonomic classi~ication of types of work activity does not coincide
with e;ther the classification of types of work, classification of
occupations or classification of work tools, i.e., external means of work
activity. The classification of ineans (methods} proper of work activity
should serve as its main foundation. Thus far, such a classification has
not been developed, since conceptions of internal means of activity have
not yet been sufficiently analyzPd in either ergonomics or psychology.
For this reason, at the present stage of development of ergonomics we have
' to limit ourselves to a general description of work activity involving
different means of labor, paying attention to the most important psycho-
logical distinctions of these grocesses.
One can distinguish cognitive, executory and motivational, including
goal-oriented,aspects, in any labor, as in any other a~tivity (learning,
- playing). Of course, the content of each of these aspects, as well as
the correlation between them, is specifically historical. They are
determined by the development of goals, refinement of ineans of production,
technological modes and conditions of labor. This is demonstrable with
particular distinction when we compare the psychological features~of
work activity to such production means as a tool, mechanized systems or
machines and automated systems.
The most interaction between a subject and object of labor occurs when
using tools or various types of instruments. Not only the labor of the
~ fitter-tool maker, builder or specialist in repair or overhaul, work
of a physician and designer, but unquestionably also workers in some
types of art, appliad art, sculptors, etc., are examples of these types of
activity. In these cases, the ob~ect stands before the subject in all
the diversity of its properties, while the subject has diverse possibi-
lities of altering and using them to obtain the desired result. In order
98
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
i'OR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
to take advantage of these possibilities, he must perform not only executory,
but various analytical and cognitive actions; in other words, he most solve
the problem by means of most efficient organization of his actions. In
this case, the means of production itself--tool instrument as it is con-
ceived or designed--reflects both the properties of the object (shape,
composition, etc.) and the functional distinctions of man's mode of
action with the object, the effort he must apply, precision required and
speed of action. Many tools and instruments developed long ago still
- amaze us with their "wisdom," convenience and simplicity of use, but
chiefly with the possibility of developing new forms of objects or trans-
forming the same object in an utterly different manner, with qualitatively
rather than only quantitativ ~y different results, by using them. The
' immediacy of interaction with the object by means of object-specific
and function-specific means of work activity creates condi*_ions, not only
for executory but cognitive actions. There may be different correlations
between them in similar work processes, which is determined primarily by
the requirements as to the results of these actions, rather than the
object and means of work actions. The requirements referable to the func-
tional or, for example, ~esthetic qualities of the result determine the
method of work actions and efficiency of their performance. When he
cnakes use of tools, man applies his capabilities, acquires experience
and skills in different areas of work activity. He also satisfies his
need to learn and create. This type of work is characterized by develop-
ment of new, more convenient or purposeful means of production, of obtain-
ing new results.
Activity proceeds differently when using mechanized means of production
in the man-machine system. Here, the object of labor (or base material,
stock, etc.) emerges only with a limited number of properties, since
a machine is incapable of considering all of the properties of material.
The poorer qualitative content of interaction with the object is also
associated with greater requirements concerning the quantitative charac-
teristics of interaction, for example its speed or amount of energv
expended. Accordingly, requirements are made under these conditions for
the work actions of man, from the standpoint of a specific quantitative
effect, i.e., obtaining a specified volume of production within a minimal
time and with the least expenditures.
Under these conditions of work activity, it becomes a constant need to
increase accuracy, organization and stereotypism of executory actions. As
a result, there is virtually no "room" le~t in the work act for cognitive
action. Production itself does not require and even does not permit any
deviations whatsoever in the qualitative characteristics of the result in
relation to what is specified. It requires that man apply only a
limited range of his capabilities, chiefly certain skills and efficient
coordination thereof with the time schedule of machine operation. In
essence, not only the object of production, but the machine itself becomes
the object of work actions for man. It is expressly to its spatial and
time distinctions that he has to adjust his actions.
99
FOR OFFICIAL USE OiVLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Accordingly, man's initiative in op*_iinizing work activity may be mani-
fe~ted chiefly in the area of organizing this activity, development of
a professional style, refinement of technology, i.e., all that pertains _
to the means of action, rather than means of production and properties
of the object. Mainly individuals in other specialties, who do not
_ participate in the work process proper, are involved in the study and
analysis of the efficiency of the latter.
_ Finally, with the use of automated means of production, the functional
direction of man's action is differentiated even more; there are greater
requirements concerning time or speed of performance of actions, even
mora rigid organization thereof as a whole. The rigid, algorithmized
organization of actinns of, for example, an operator-observer or operator
of tracking systems, does not always by far allow the operator to form
the mode of action that is the most convenient for him and ~ioes not directly
create a need to upgrade the quality of the end result. In fact, there is
a change in the very content of the result. This no longer refers to
the result of man's influence on some ob~ect by means of automated devices,
but the result of changes that are caused by man's actions in the auto-
mated device itself. And the gages that determine the efficiency of
the mode of system operation are transferred to man's actions. They in-
clude the gages of precision, speed and reliability.
Thus, the means of production itself becomes the immediate obj ect of
man's activity, while the requirements as to the result of interaction
are limited by the operating mode or state. In practice, these re- -
quirements pertain only to the executory actions of man, and it is only
when the device itself sCops operating in the set mode that man is
provided with the opportunity to perform certain cognitive actions to
determine the causes of the dead hal~. These actions are characterized
more often by the measure of responsibility, rather than measure of
need. As a result, one co~ld have concluded that the meausres [gages]
of executory actions that are established on the basis of efficient
operation of the system should be the main criteria of work actions.
However, under conditions of automated production, new types of occupa-
tions are appearing: operator investigator and administrator, which
require a different approach.
In these types of activity, not only perfect proficiency in use of tools
and means of labor, not only executory and cognitive processes, but pro-
cesses of shaping or setting goals and choice of inethods of reaching
them play an ever increasing role. We refer to establishment of goals of ,
quite concrete, inherent processes of labor and dynamic conditions under
which they occur, rather than any external ones in relation to work
activity. Ergonomic analysis of many modern types of work activity re-
quires the mandatory consideration of human subjectivity, analysis of
~ motivations and goal-setting processes, characteristics of subjective
conception of the goals and change therein during the actual work process.
100
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
�
These requirements of ergonomic analysis are related to the fact that the
goals are interspersed in the work process, than cannot be replaced with
either labor sets or motives.
The subject of ergonomics is any activity insofar as it is included in the
rather broad context of technological equi~ment [means]. Th is, of course,
doea not mean that ergonomics is identical to general theory of activity;
its tasks are much narrower, and they are related primarily to analysis
and purposeful planning of the existing types of work activity. For ex-
pressly this reason, as we have mentioned before, ergonomics makes a
contribution to the devel~pment of general theory of human work activity -
in modern industry.
Serious difficulties are encountered in defining activity as the subject of
ergonomic research. This is also attributable to the fact that there has
been virtually no at~empt made in our literature to differentiate between
the concepts of "activity," "labor," "labor [workJ activity," and the
different usage thereof is irnuitive rather that scientifically substantiated.
In spite of the fact that there is much in common in the concepts of acti-
vity and labor, one cannot automatically make a distinction between their
scope and content [10]. And such differentiation is far from a simple
task. It is not even the problem that the concepts of labor and activity
cross over one another. There is a complex system of correlations (develop-
ment, function, etc.) bet~,reen them. Labor is a condition of development
of activity to the same extent as development of activity is a condition -
of development of labor. For expressly this reason, considerably more
similarity than differences are found between them in general philosophical
or sociological studies [10]. The situation is the same in ergonomics,
which finds itself related to general theory of activity or general
theoretical conceptions of man's activity. Methodologically, this appears
to be quite logical: special scientific studies of activicy should have
as their theoretical and methodological grounds certain general concept-
tions about activity as a whole, about the laws of its organization and
structure. In practice, however, as observed by E. G. Yudin, the matter
is much more complex; modern scientific knowledge does not, in essence,
have a theoretically complete phenomenology of activity as a whole; for
this reason, there remains only one possibility for ~he investigator of
activity if he tries to find and clearly lay the theoretical foundation
to his work: to refer to conceptions of activity that were developed by -
psychology [62, p 338]. This is the route we followed in our analysis r.
of the functional structure of executory and cognitive activity, which
will be submitted in the next sections of this chan.*.er.
2. Functional Structure of Executory (Perceptual and Motor) Acticn
In the Foreword to his "Essay on Working Movements of Man," published
in 1901, I. M. Sechenov wrote that the subject of his essay "consists of
101
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
questions of the complex mu~cul2r motions by means of which man performs so-
called external work, i.e., he exerts the force of his muscles on objects
in the outside world" [54]. Although the nature of "external work" has
" changed appreciably since then, and absolutely new types of work activity _
related to control of complex equipment have appeared, there is still
validity to Sechenov's words, that work always was and always remains
a vital function of the human muscular system, no matter how much modern
technology eliminates from industrial life human muscular labor. It is
imperative to analyze executory activity and demonstrate the general prin-
ciples of development and inception of its functional structure in order
- to solve problems of control and optimization of such activity and to
plan neT~ forms and means thereof. This is needed to organize rational
teaching and training, to develop perfect skills, to organize work and
rest schedules that prevent fatigue.
~ In ergonomics, executory or controlling action refers to the ability
(skill) acquired as a result of learning and repetition to solve a work
problem using work tools (hand instrument, control elements, etc.) with
specified accuracy and speed. As a rule, executory actions are included
as a component in broader structures of work activity, and provide for
efficient performance theieof, along with such components as cognitivc,
including decision making. Dependi~ng on the type of w~rk activity, the
share of executory actions varies quite significantly. These actions
may be performed either sporadically, or throughout the entire work time.
In other words, they may occupy the place of the main goal in the struc-
ture of activity as a whole, or else emerge as a means of reaching it,
for example, transmitting a command, implementing a decision, etc. In
the latter case, executory, motor acts are usually simple, and they do
not require lengthy learnir.g. In those cases where executory actions
~constitute the main content of activ~ty (work with a hand instrument
[tool], lathe operator work, driver occupations, telegraph operator,
computer operator, work in the tracking mode), lengthy formation of the
appropriate abilities and skills is required to assure prompt and ,
precise performance of the work activit~.
For a long time, for ergonomic implementation of these types of executory
actions the traditional conceptions of motor and sensorimotor learning
and conceptions of motor skills as automated and largely stereotype
reactions arising with numerous repetitions of sensorimotor and kines-
' thetic acts were enough. The development of skills was usually described
in terms of stimuli and reactions, reflexes, trial and error. When these
elements w^re repeated, when this repetition succeeded or was rein-
forced, f.'_rst separate reactions were replaced by complexes, isolated
movements combined into integral kinetic structures, a sort of "motor
form" or "kinetic melody."
102
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
For a lon g time, such an "nascent" or later on stimulus-reactive approach
directed toward the result, the effect of a single, relatively simple action,
constituted the scientific basis for the conception of "engineering design"
of work methods that is linked with the names of F. Taylor and F. Gilbreth.
Time-related and motor analysis of elementary actions and operations served
as the methodological basis of such planning. F. Gilbreth expounded the
idea of universal micromovements (therbligs), of combinations of which,
differing in composition and order of therbligs, any operation must consist.
This idea was used at the plants oF H. Ford, where the entire assembly
work process was broken down, by means of thorough planning, into so
many minute operations ttiat a car was assembled while in continuous motion.
Ford strived to have a worker perform a single job with a single motion.
F. Gilbreth studied motion by means of time studies, photography, cinema-
tography and cyclo~raphy. The principles of economy of motion that he
formulated made it possible ro eliminate superfluous ones, and to select
out oF all possible ones those that could be performed the fastest and
required the least effort, as well as to achieve a reduction in intervals
between sticcessive movements. Practical tasks of work planning initiated
studias of kinematic and dynamic characteristics of man's work movements.
The results and methods of these studies, as well as the principle formu-
lated by Gilbreth of ec~nomy of work motions, were used in solving
problems of organization of work places, design of hand tools, arrange-
ment of controls, etc.
Unquestion~bly, the systems of F. Taylor and F. Gilbreth made a substantial
contribution ta the study of elementary actions and operations. However,
use of time and motor analysis of motiun in the form that it was proposed
could not demonstrate the structure and mechanisms of integral executory
activity of man. In 1930, N. A. Bernshteyn wrote: "It must be stressed
that not only the methods, but the very concept of rationalization of
motion, are not as simple as was prPViously thought. The simple
struggle of Taylor and later Giibreth against superfluous movements and
interpretation of a biomechanical operation as the simple sum of
successive movements, which could be passed like grain through a
screen, is beginning to yield its place to interpretation of the mator
complex as an organically inseparable whole that always responds to changes
in any part by readjusting all the others" [5, p 7].
Such an engineering approach to the planning of work (with all its initial
usefulness) was submitted to valid criticism on several grounds. Monotony
and little satisfaction with work are the obvious consequences of utmost
simplification of work, of reducing it to single elementary motor acts. ~
Both have an adverse effect on labor productivity. _
As for the more complex types of work activity, this approach has already
exhausted its "optimizing" capabilities. The complexity of executory
action is growing to s:.ach an extent, that standard motor "forms" or
~ 103 -
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
even kinetic "melodies" cannot assure efficient performance thereof. We
refer to the fact that in modern industry stereotypd.c work movements are
gradually yielding to the performance of purposef ul, wise, voluntary
executory actions. In many types of work activity, protection against
automatism, impulsive, reflex rPactions is required more and more often.
Erroneous actions, which occasionally lead to emergency situations,
often occur because man was too hasty, and not because he did not have
enough ticne. _
This applies equally to a lathe operator and pilot. Modern mechanized
and automated industry requires that man perform not only learned,
assimilated acti~ns, but actiona that are, so to speak, unprecedented, -
which do not neeu to be recalled, but created in a new, unexpectedly
occurring situation. There are increasingly frequent instances when
it is impossible to reproduce all of the important conditions of a
= real labor process during occupational training, and final learning
takes place when performing a work, executory action, rather than an
exercise. Adjustment to real conditions is particularly difficult when
performance of actions requires perfect sensorimotor coordination. The
activity of cosmonauts, who must perform in weightlessness docking,
separating maneuvers, move from one craft to another, engage in extra-
vehicular activity in space, operate hand tools, make manual landings,
i.e., handle controls under variable gravity conditions that transform
customary sensorimotoX coordination, the force pattern of previously
well-learned motions, could be a vivid example of such situations. In
particular, weightlessness affects more than the area of motor activity;
it can elicit diverse unpleasant sensations, transient spatial delusions -
or even signs of depersonalization and derealization of the subject's
perceptions.
The need to perform executory actions in the case of delayed fe2dback
about the results of a performed action imposes a similar mental load.
Among such actions are control of a LEt4 ["lunokhod"--lunar excursion
module], where the delay does not exceed a few seconds, and control of
a supertanker, where the lag in appropriate evolutions of the ship after
performing a control action numbers a few minutes. The appearance of
an entire series of relatively new types of activity, related to control
of spacecraft and space stations, remote studies of planets, handling
of radioactive elements, control of diverse moving objects, including
robots, led to the distinction of operator-manipulator activity as a
special object of ergonomic research. With this form of activity, per- -
ceptual-mot~r coordination and interaction play the chief role, althaugh,
of course, the system of graphic and conceptual thinking also plays a
significant part. The executory actions of an operator-manipulator are
performed by means of so-called "regulated motions," which require high
precision, no~ only spatial but in time. This means that, from the stand-
point of efficiency of their performance, the informative indicator is
not only the end result of action (as in the case of depressing a button,
104
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
key, tumbler), but the ongoing characteristics of motions that determine
the dynamics of the object of control.
Perfect perceptual-motor cooroination is also needed ior the performance
of many technological processes. A vivid example is the activity involved
in manufacturing and opPrating microdevices. The dimensions of micro-
- objects and required density of their assembly also impose high require-
ments of the technology of producing them, so that instrument produc-
tion on their basis has become a jeweler's job. The work ot man
engaged in the area of assembly, for example, of integrated circuits, is
performed under conditions of constant visual monitoring, high tension due
to the need to perform highly accurate and finely coordinated precision _
movements. The influence of these factors is complicated by the fact that
the size of microdevices is on the boundary of visibility to the naked
eye, and visual monitoring of technological operations is possible only
with the use of optical magnifying instruments. It is a known fact that
use thereof results in adopting a forced [contrived] position, hypo-
- kinesia, narrowed field of vision, etc.
Servicing of many lathes requires highly coordinated work ~�ith both -
hands under continuous visual monitoring. The time ~:ithin which the
coordinated movemeats have to be made should not exceed 60-80 ms in some
cases. The need to optimize such forms of activity led to the distinction
of activity of the operator-technologist as a special object of ergonomic -
research.
- These examples indicate that a"nascent," stimulus-reactive approach to
the study and optimization of activity of the operator-manipulator and
operator-technologist cannot satisfy modern ergonomics. The motor acts,
executory actions are interspersed in the fabric of broader structures
of activity, and the success of executory actions should not be evaluated
by itself, but within the context of these structures. It depends on
how well man became oriented in a situation, i.e., on whether man formed
the correct image of this situation and whether he found a way, sometimes
the only possible way to act.
Formation of an image of a situation, creation of programs of rational
action, precise and prompt performance tizereof, monitoring its effective-
ness--these are the problems that confronted modern ergonomics, as well
as a set of allied disciplines, biomechanics, physiology and psychology,
which have long since studied the organization, structure and control
of human movements and actions.
Both the practical tasks that confronted these disciplines and the logic
of their own development require formulation of new approaches to the
study of executory actions. To counterbalance the nascent-reflex
approaches directed toward an assignment, result, effect, etc., researchers
are developing a structured, integral, activity-oriented approach directed
105
. FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
not only toward learning, but constructing motions, actions, motor programs
and schemes. '
A thorough analysis of the pattern of motions, even when repeated many
times iu the same situation, is indicative of their uniqueness and
distinction. A detaled analysis of the moto r act shows that its biodynami~
fabric is as unique as a f ingerprint. This means that not only is the
image of a situation and equivalent motor scheme is constructed, but on
the basis of this scheme each living motor act is constructed (rather
than simply repeated). The results and the progress of this work do not
ensue unequivocally from the structure of exogenous stimulus reinforce-
ment. In this sense, explanation of the motion that is performed with
the "stimulus-reaction" scheme does not conform with the essence of the
question., Researchers have ~till to develop concepts referable to this
type of work, on construction of spatial m~tor action.
Motor action, viewed as a mandatory component ci activity, must necessarily
be correlated with its cognitive and personality components, such as, for -
example, image and goal. And, as we have indicated above, activity itself
as a whole and all its components are o� necessity characterized by ob-
~ective-meaningful features and time and apace certainty. The origin of
this approach goes back to the names of I. M. Sechenov and C. Sherrington.
I. M, Sechenov repeatedly stressed that "feeling always has the signifi-
cance of regulator of motion; in other words, the former causes the
latter and alters its strength and direction" [55, pp 236-237J. It is
also interesting that Sechenov did not limit the task for physiology and
psychology to the sCudy of separate movements, but spoke of the need to
study the area of phenomena in which "feeling is transformed into motive
and goal, while movement is transformed into action." At the present
stage of studying work motions, work operat ions and actions, Che most
complex forms of man's executory activity, it is particularly important
to mention the direction indicated by Sechenov in the search for a solu-
tion to the problem that is a cardinal one to this day for physiology
and psychology: what is the mechanism of regulation of motion by
feelings? The possibility of such regulation is already provided by the -
fact that a muscle, which is a"dual organ, our working organ and, at the
same time, primordial, original sense organ, which educated as part of
its properties all other sense organs, colo rs all our conceptions of the
world around us in images of motion" [53, p 936]. Moreover, Sechenov wrote
that the muscle gave us our conceptions of space, time, date ~ counting,
etc. All this is possible only provided that motions and actions them-
selves are not merely elementary and utilitarian acts of execution, but
that they also p erform co gnitive, learning and expressive functions. The
latter is clearly implemented, not only through movements, but postural _
and t~nic components of action, which carry its persona?ity-meaningful
content.
106
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Many work motions and actions are so perfect, coordinated, expressive and
, beautiful that they are often included in dramatized performances. The
idea of creating a special choreography of work processes, which has been
voiced from time to time, is not without meaning.
The functional duality of a muscle, functional heterogeneity of motions and
- actions provide not only for potential, but actual integrity or activity, _
the possibility for it to develop and improve. In this sense, C.
Sherrington made a remarkable assumption that the performance of
actions directed toward a dcrinitive, final act in the process of selec-
tion opens up the possibility for elements of inemory (though rudimentary)
and elements of forewarning (though insignificant)to develop into the
mental capacity of 'running in reverse' from the present to the past,
as well as ahead, into the future, which is a mandatory sign of higher
mental development in higher animals" [60, p 314]. It is expressly this
"mental capacity" that is the regulator of executory acts. I. M. Sechenov
understood this very deeply, stating that feelings relayed to conscious-
ness by sense organs do not serve directly as sources of u~otion, but -
- via the psyche, since a certain meaning is related to a signal.
The difference between the nascent-reflex and integral approach is also
fixed in the language used to describe motor behavior. For the former,
mainly such terms as reactology, reflexology were used, and for the latter--
psychomotor system, psychonervous activity, mental activity, etc. Of
course, the use of the terms "reflex" or "reaction" per se does not yet
signify that a given author is a proponent of the "nascent" approach. It
is expressly in these terms that the original foundation was laid for the
structured approach to the study of motion and action. Thus, C.
Sherrington, in analyzing forewarning and concluding reactions, wrote:
"It is not difficult to see what broad opportunities are provided for
adaptive reactions by a device consisting of an entire chain of successive
acts, each of which alters the influence of the preceding act" [60, p 312].
This quotation clearly discloses the idea of integrity [wholenessJ of
adaptive activity. Analogously, I. P. Pavlov, who analyzed chains of motor
reflexes, arrived at the idea of dynamic stereotype as an integral element.
Since the times when I. M. Sechenov and C. Sherrington gave a psycholo-~
gical interpretation to motor behavior, numerous data have been accumu].ated
on the decisive role of sensory processes in the control of human motion.
A. A. Ukhtomskiy, who analyzed the structure of the anatomical system
that implements the movements of higher animals and man, o~served its
originality, as compared to artificial mechanical devices characterized
- by a significantly larger number of degrees of freedom. Neither the
bone and muscle system as a whole, nor any part of it forms a ready
mechanism for the performance of any speci~ic purposeful act, but repre-
sents only a set of certain anatomical components that are needed to
- form it. The structural distinctions of the skeletomuscular system
impart flexibility to the behavior of higher animals and man, and at the
107
~ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
same time render the task of controlling this behavior extremely complex
and difficult. Since control implies a limitation of degrees of freedom,
such restrictions are virtually lacking in the actual arrangement of
performing mechanisms in living organisms, and the functions o� r�egulation
of performed actions l~ave to be assumed by central mechanisms. Let us _
briefly examine the evolution of conceptions and current views of the
mechanisms of control of movements.
Originally, it was believed that central mechanisms can perform this
function using rigid standards that predetermine the nature and sequence
of required movements. R. Woodworth [80] introduced the term "central" -
or "motor" programming for this means of construction of motion. He
proved the existence of motor programs in studies of rapid voluntary
movements of man.
Analysis of the kinematic characteristics of precise hand movements led
- him to the conclusion that there is a phase of motion that is independent
of visual feedback, a phase that is determined by the original program.
In addition to this phase, there is a second one that is performed with
consideration of visual feedback and which implements that precision
features of motion. Thus, Woodworth described the means of controlling
motion that were later named controlthrough open and closed loops of
regulation. At the present time, each of these means has been largely
absolutized and has its proponents. A c~nsiderable amount of experimental
data has been accumulated in favor of each of them; debates are taking
place between representatives of theory of open and closed l00~5.
K. Leshly was apparently one of the first to distinctly formulate the
conception of central motor programs and experimentally prove that
development of a~kill is a centrally organiaed process, in the imple-
mentation of which proprioceptive mechanisms may not necessary play a -
significant role. The conclusions of Leshly, referable to the fact that
a learned skill could be implemented by various motor structures do
indeed confirm the idea of motor programming, but at the present time
thay are virtually not used as evidence of the minor role of kinesthetic
control. The search for proof of an open loop proceeded along the
road of studying rapid ballistic movements and blocking feedback channels
that function in performing motor acts. The proponents of the conception
of motor programming and open loops relegate to afferentation only
triggering functions and modulating influences. However, no conclusive
evidence has yet been obtained to the effect that man's voluntary move-
ment can take place only as the result of centrally organized nervous
commands, that are structured before the start of a movement and permit
performance thereof in the absence of peripheral feedback.
The main flaw of open loop systems is that they do not have feedback
mechanisms to correct mistakes that occur as a result of both the
108
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFrICIAL USE ONLY
properties of tk~eir inputs and transformation of signals within the system.
This type of system has poor compensatory capabilities.
Conceptions of motor programs were develoned comprehensively within the
framework of the open loap concept. Motor programming means that the
sets of motor commands, both inborn and learned, are stored in the central
nervous system,and can be evoked and synthesized into the desired motion.
A motor program is a meticulously coordinated order of synergies (they are
sometimes callec~ subroutines, or submodes) that together involve the
required motion an4 are independent of feedback.
Regardless of the attitude of proponents of the open loop concept toward
- involvement of feedback in regulation of motion, they have developed some
interesting ideas about the hierarchy of motor programs, existence of
generalized programs, program-schemes, the lower elements of which relieve
the main program f.rom laborious calculations. Of importance also are the
hypotheses of a link between programs, motives and goals, which are
transformed into a certain internal conception of the subje~~t about the
desired, required motion or action. In other words, motor programs are
more closely related to the image of a situation, image of action,
rather than only to a set of commands stored in the nervous system. The
- open loop conception of regulation has been used, with minimal stipula-
tions and limitations, to inc.trpret the mechanisms of human eye movements.
Numerous studies established virtually the same correlation between speed
of the jerk at the initial start of movement and ultimate amplitude
of the jerk. This means that tiie velocity of the saccade was programmed
even before the start of movement. On the basis of electrophysiological
studies, it was concluded that control of saccadic movements in one
f ixed direction amounts to determination of the time segment within which
a constant force is applied that contracts the rectus muscles of the eye.
We find the seed of the opposite ideas of a circular or closed loop
- of motion regulation in the works of W. James [70], C. Sherringtcn [60]
and others.
James :~ssumed that .peripheral feedback from one part of a movement brings
the nFxt one into action, and he expounded the hypothesis of "chain
reflexes," against which Leshly spoke later on. According to closed
loop theory, it is assumed that a response is not merely triggered
by the. receptor system, but controlled by it.
Controt of movement via a"closed" loop implies transmission through
feedbac~; of information about conformity of the movement with the required
goal and elaboration on this basis of new control commands. Feedback
serves two functions: with it determination is made of the spatial charac-
teristics or" the goal that are required to form the program of ballistic
movement, a.s we].1 as correlation of the results of implementing such
programs w:ith the actual position of the goal, which serves to adjust
109
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
the prog*.�ams for subsequent movements. N. A. Bernshteyn provided the
most complete argumentation for the fact that rigid programming cannot
assure the purposeful effect of movement.
The theory of N. A. Bernshteyn deals with a wide class of functionally
different movements and is a general theory of level-by-level control
and formation of human movements. This theory is comprised of three basic
principles: central progra~nming, sensory corrections and level-related
organization of movements. It describes the principle of coordination
of movements in a faultless form from the standpoint of modern theory
of automatic regulation: as soon as an organ that is under the
influence of exogenous and reactive forces plus some added endogenous,
muscular Force deviate in their resultant motion from what is intended
by the central nervous system, the latter receives an exhaustive signal -
about this deviation, which is sufficient to make adequate adjustments in
the effector process. For this reason, this entire principle of coordina-
tion deserves to be called the principle of sensory corrections" [6, p 28).
For a long time, N. A. Bernshteyn emphatically rejected any possibility
of control of movement via an open loop, However, later on, he
retreated from such an extreme point of view and conceded that there
was a possibility that, in some elementary processes, the arc is not
closed into a reflex ring, either because of the brevity of the act
or its extreme simplicity.
Sensory corrections are made, in the general case, by all of the receptor
systems availa.ble to the body. In special cases, some of the feedbacks
do not necessarily participate in the control of movement. The primary
receptor signals are first submitted to complex processing and "recoding,"
which is needed for example, so that they can be compared to the plan of
movement made in the language of spatial and kinematic conceptions. The
"syntheses" obtained as a result of processing, which consists of signals
of all types of feedback involved in controlling a given movement, serve
for sensory corrections.
- In the model of Bernshteyn, the concept of sensory synthesis plays a basic -
role. The composition of afferentations that f~rm it, i.e., feedback,
and the principle of their combination serve as the main criterion that
distinguishes one level of forming movement from another.
Each motor tasks finds its own main level, depending on its content and
meaning-related structure. Levels differ from one another, not only in
type of sensory synthesis, but anatomical substrate, i.e., the aggregate
of nervous system organs without which implemention of function of
this level is impossible.
One of the levels takes the lead, coordinating the action of underlying
background levels, depending on the purpose and meaning of the motor act.
110
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY _
Only the leading level is perceived in any movement. Development of a motor
skill is a process of formation of the level composition of movement in
the course of learning and conditioning, singling out the leading level
and coordination of all levels involved in control.
Development of an adequate method is a mandatory prerequisite for success-
ful studies of motor acts; it would permit recording and analyzing the
time and space evolution of movement, the entire course of the motor act ~
"over the entire motor system of the body." In studies of executoxy
activity directed toward demonstration of objective indicators of the
process of formation of a sensorimotor image of space and structure of
action, the microstructural analytical method was used, which consists _
of isolating rapid components of integral mental acts and analyzing
their correlations. Use of this method in studies of voluntary spatial
actions permitted disclosure of the strucf_ure of spatial action, tracking
the dynamics of its inception and developn!ent under different conditions
of action, isolating several component stages: formation of the program,
execution, control and correction, component structures of action,
dynamics of their development, correlation between them at different
stages of learning the action, as well as changes occurring within the -
isolated components of an integral action (see Chapter III for a des-
cription of the method).
The experimental situation involved t:he study of formation of instrumental
spatial action under different conditions. Under stable conditions,
the tra3ectories of the required moventent were of the same size and
complexity. Under dynamic conditions;, the trajectories differed in number
of support [reference] elements and n~,imber of spatial components of move-
ment. In the case of inversion, there was a mismatch (complete or partial)
between the perceptive and motor fields. Inversion was introduced after
= development of the skill under normal conditions. ~
As a result of this study, it was found that there are complex dynamics
of correlations between different stages of an integral action in the
course of development of a skill (stable conditions, norm). In the first
place, in the course of learning [mastering] a spatial action there is ~
a reduction in time of each isolated stage; in the second place, the
time reduction in each stage occurs unevenly; in the third place, as
_ conditioning progresses there is redistribution of time among the isolated
stages. The nonuniformity of rate of reduction of time in the isolated
stages indicates that all components of integral action are disssimilarl.y
refined. The studies demonstrated the sequence of formation of components
of a spatial action. The stage of formation of motor programs is the
fastest, followed by the stage of control [ehecking] and correction;
both are formed against the background of gradual decrease in time
taken up by the stage of implementation of motor programs. Only after both
cognitive components are formed is it possible, apparently, for the last
reduction in time of performance of the action as a whole. This reduction
111
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
is referable to the executory part thereof. The redistribution of time
among the stages within an ir.~egral action at different stages of forma-
tion indicates that each new exercise is a new problem solving process, a
process of change and refinement of the ways and means of solving it.
When using inversion as a mear.s of disrupting a formed spatial action, it
' was demonstrated that subjectively the process of formation of a skill
in the presence of inversion is experienced ~ considerahly more difficult
than the norm. Formation of a skill with any form of inversion (total or
partial) alleviates adoption of any other type of inversion. The change
from the norm to any type of inversion occurs with great difficulty and
requires more time than the change back. A comparison of the course of
formation of compatible and inverted instrumental spatial action shows
that when changing to work with inversion there are effects of transfer
and interference (Figure 10).
-
1 s _ -
Stages:
~ ~ control & correction
phasic
- ~ lat~nt
~
II~~i111N111~M1111M1111~1111N1111N11111~IIN1111N1111M11 IMIIIIN1111MIIIIMIIIIMIIIIMIIIIMI IIMIIIIMI II IIIIMIIIINIIUMIIIIMI IMII ~NIIIINII .4 of test
Iu ~a III lu In 111 III III I~ Itl III I11 lo la III III III 10 III III III III III III I~I III Id Irl
~ y;{ ~.i Ii 7 N!~ 10 11 1'1 13 I~ I.i Ili 17 IM 1!I 'lll 'll 'l'1 Y~1 'l3 Y:~ Yb 1; �~N 1~~ of matr2x
~ norm X, Y, Z norm X X,~ver�i6n norm
Figur~ 10. Dynamics of process of formation of instrumental spatial
movement (3-month interval between 6th and 7th, 9th and
lOth, 25th and 26th matrices)
One observes different dynamics of behavior of functional components in
the course of change in a skill, analysis of which leads to the conclusion
that faster than normal formation of an inverted skill is possible by
means of transfer of phasic, speed-related features of spatial action.
The implementation stage retained virtually all of its characteristics.
Inversion of perceptive and motor fields had a negligible effect on
speed characteristics of the phasic elements of action. In the case of
cognitive components, we are not dealing with transfer, but interference
of the spatial image formed under normal conditions and the imsge that is
just being formed under inverted conditions. This affected the nature of
the cognitive elements. Moreover, expressly this affected the charac-
teristics of the implementation stages at the early phases of formation of
a new action under new conditions. The phasic part of the action again
assumed cognitive functions. By means ef hand movements, the subjects
palpate new space and find the features of this space. When a new
112
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
sensorimotor image of space is formed, the stage of implemention is re-
lieved of cognitive functi~ns and begins to operate as in a compatib 1 e
spatial action, but now it implements other programs. The cognitive
components continue to be refined but no longer with visible particip a-
tion of the implementation stage. Thus, the phenomena of transfer and
interference are different in nature. Transfer occurs at the expens e of
the performing [executory] part of action and interf erence,of the cogni-
tive components; however, these phenomena are not mutually exclusive ,
_ they interact in each spatial action.
The dynamic conditions of presenting information affected chiefly the
characteristics of cognitive components of the process under study,
analogously to the changes recorded with introduction of inversion. The
particularly drastically changing conditions of nresentation of info rma-
tion affected the characteristics of the control and correction stage,
the time of function of which is 2-3 times longer than the time requ3red
for control under stable conditions. This is related to the fact that
in the presence of uncertainty a dual load is imposed on the control and
correction stage: not only to check each discrete action, but also, what
is particularly important, relate the conditions of presentation of
information to the action performed. In other words, the control func-
tion includes not only checking the result of action, but control of
consistency of the chosen program with the action to be performed. The
results of this study provided new material for investigating the process
of formation of a sensorimotor image of a work space, based on active
actions, the cognitive component of which is the most important at the
first stage of formation of the new action. It is obvious, from the data
on the indicator of cognitivity, which characterizes the dynamics of
time relations between cognitive and executory components and is man 3-
fested by the ratio of sum of time of cognitive components to the
executory one, that as a skill is acquired the share of cognitive com-
ponents in the integral action diminishes. When an image of sensorirnotor
space is formed, the function of cognitive components is reduced to
programming the performed action which, of course, affects the reduc tion
in the indicator of cognitivity. As compared to dynamic conditions. of
presentation of information, under stable ones the reduction in value
of the cognitition indicator is expressed because the control function
is more reduced under static conditions.
At the first stages of formation of a new action, whatever the condi tions,
there are vague boundaries between stages. The scatter between elements
X, Y and Z within each stage is so wide (in some cases up to 1 s) that
the impression is created of one stage entering into another. This is
quite consistent with the thesis formulated within the context of th e
structural systems research, according to which a less developed struc-
ture is characterized by less differentiation of its components. Th 3s
leads us to two hypotheses: first, at the early stages of learning there
may be para11e1 execution and implementation of the program, as well as
113
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
implementation and control; the second, which ensues from the first, is
that, at the early stages of formation of a skill, execution of the pro-
gram, its implementation and control proceed separately in movement compo-
nents. In other words, there is successive planning of movement in each
coordinate. Analogously, there is successive implementation and control.
The learned action is characterized by a significant decrease in scatter,
and since it is scatter that characterizes the quality of action (its
spatiality), at the final stages of learning the formed action acquires
features of a more distinct functional structure. And while the functional
structure of action is comparable, according to the index of spatiality,
for different conditions of action at the early stages of learning, at
the end of learning the actions formed under dynamic and inversion condi-
tions are comparable, being 2-3 times greater in scatter than this para-
meter under normal conditions. Consequently, introduction of inversion or
uncertainty consistently worsens the quality of action, as manifested by _
an increase in values of the scatter index. In other words, the quality
of acCion is extremely sensitive to various changes made in the conditions
under which it is performed.
Knowledge of the functional structure of action, studies of the dynamics
of its formation and inception, demonstration of interrelations and cor-
relations between components of the object studied offer the possibility
of controlling the process of formation and optimization of movements and
actions.
The change in share of components in the structure of action, both during
its formation and under the influence of some changes or other made in
the conditions under which it occurs, indicates that prevalence of some
type of regulation of motor acts is related essentially to the conditions
under which the action takes place and the degree of assimilation, learning.
Figure v. illustrates the shares of components of an integral action taking
place under different conditions and at different stages of its formation. _
a b c
~ ~ ~ i Figure 11.
~ Share of components of formed
spatial action under stable (a),
- dynamic (b) and inversion (c)
, conditions:
1) sCart of learning
2 2) end of learning
r
Stages:~.
program formation -
prograia implementation
~ control and correction
114
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
- rori ~FFLC1'AL USE ON1.r
The correlation between components of the functional structure of an in-
tegral action is similar at the start of its formation, regardless of the
conditions under which the action occurs. At the end of such formation,
a similar correlation between components of action structure is observed -
in actions formed under dynamic and inverted conditions; an action formed
under normal conditions has a very different structure. The situation of
inversion and dynamics and the normal situation can be compared in terms
of open and closed control loops. Under normal conditions, the sub~ects
formed a simultaneous image of the situation after lengthy conditioning,
and the grogram that organizes the motor response, i.e., a signif icant
part of the action, appeared to be implemented over an open loop, as
confirmed by the significant share occupied by the stage of forma tion of
programs and relatively small share of the control and correction stage.
In the inversion situation and under dynamic conditions of presentation
of information, during the experimental series there was retentio n of
regulation by the closed loop principle, as indicated by the share of
the control and correction stage, which constituted about 50% of the
integral action.
Many diverse variants of closed loop of regulation have been proposed to
date, and they describe more or less complex acts of human behavior and
activity. These theories refer to such processes as discrete and con-
- tinuous motor processes, perceptual-motor skills, verbal behavior, etc.
The general features of these theories are that the closed loop.implies
Chat the subject is aware of the course of performance of movemen t.
Such knowledge is gained by means of feedback from movement and is
directed toward control of this movement. The closed loop is bas ed on
checking information from elements of the system, "calculation" and
consideration of errors indicating the direction or degree of deviation
of the system beyond the set range, and correction of such errors. The
~ main function of closed loop systems is to minimize these errors.
J. Adams [63J proposed an interesting variant of closed loop cont rol of
movement in development of motor skills. In developing his theory,
Adams made wide use of the conceptions of P. K. Anokhin pertaining to
action acceptor, N. A. Bernshte;~n concerning the setting element and
comparison system and Ye. N. Sokolov concerning the nerve model of a
stimulus.
The theory was expounded to interpret the process of learning simple,
_ discrete movements performed in a moderate, unimposed pace, i.e., it is
the theory of formation of a motor skill. It applies, first of all, to
linear movements of the hand over a specified distance without the
subject seeing the mark showing the required end position of the hand,
while the distance is given to him in either verbal form, or else he
learns it in the course of training by shifting his hand up to the stop -
in a limiting device.
115
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFF7CIAL tJSE ONLY
According to Adams, in a closed loon mechanisms by ~ueans of which information
received over feedback channels is compared to the standard for detection
of errors occupy the central place, i.e., it is assumed that a standard
mechanism exists in the system, in which the specified action is fixed,
as well as feedback channels,system for making comparisons, isolating and
correcting errors. Knowledge about the results of each performed movement
is of first and foremost significance to formation of skills. Man uses
this knowledge to alter movement and eliminate or attenuate errors in each
successive trial. Such successive corrections ultimately lead to develop-
ment of the correct movement. The standard mechanism is called the
perceptual trace, which consists of information of previously performed
movements stored in memory.
The concept of a p~rceptual trace is the equivalent of the concept
of nervous model of a stimulus [56]. The perceptual trace is a mechanism -
that determines the amplitude of movement and, perhaps, time organization
of movement. In the general case, all forms of feedback are sources of
' formation of a perceptual trac~: visual, auditory, proprioceptive, as
well as tactile and pressure receptors. The stability of a perceptual
trace increases with increase in number of trials. Information about
early, inaccurate trials is forgotten, and there is an increase in "weight"
of the most recent tests performed with great precision.
However, learning a movement is not reduced to such a. simple scheme,
according to which it would be sufficient for a perceptual trace to be ~
formed and for the stimuli of current feedback to conform with it. At _
the early stage of learning, conscious and verbalized knowledge of the
results is of decisive significance. This stage is called verbal-motor.
It ends when a satisfactory result has been obtained in a series of
performances, and there are few errors. After reaching a certain level
of perfection, the percep*_ual trace is fixed. Further lea-rning can then
proceed without knowing the results. Instead, there is comparison of
feedback information to a highly accurate and stable perceptual trace.
This final stage is callsd the motor stage.
Adams offers logical evidence of the existence of a spQCial mechanism, the
function of which consists of initiation and choice of movement, which
is called a trace in memory. A memory trace functions in an open system,
controlling by program, without correction, the feedback of movement
at the initial stage. The action of inemory trace and perceptual trace
does not coincide in time. At first, the memory trace is involved in
control and somewhat later, when feedback signals begin to be received,
control is transmitted to the perceptual trace. In other words, a memory
trace is a motor program which merely actualizes the mechanisms needed to
implement the reaction and triggers them, but does not control the
execution of a longer sequence, as is generally implied in the open loop -
conception. Some movements are executed only on the basis of the memory
trace, if the motor reaction can be classified as being ballistic. Such
116
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
a reaction is initiated by the memory trace and ends before the subject
is able to adjust it in the performance process, comparing the received -
feedback to the perceptual trace.
It must be stated that the explanation of ballistic movements performed
within 100-200 ms presents the greatest difficulties for ~the closed loop
conception, since correction in such cases must be made before the
movement is completed. The hypothesis that motor control is planned
prior to the start of movement is expounded to explain such cases. The
- fact that man can perform a movement lasting no more than 100 ms was
used as the strongest (true, still indirect) argument in favor of the -
open loop conception. However, current studies in the field of physiology
of proprioception yielded numerous facts indicating that proprioceptive
feedback can occur within substantially less than 100 ms. Cortical po-
tentials from nerves located in the tongue and extremities are recorded
within 3-5 ms. The full cycle ~rom the muscle receptors of the eyes _
through the brain and back again occurs in 10 ms. A cortical response to
a hand movement is recorded after 10 ms, while the total interval between
delivery of motor stimulus (through the cortex) and EMG response constitutes
only 30-40 ms. Thus, the motor system has tl~e required "neuronal speed" �
to regulate movements in a closed loop, and feedback was used not only at
all learning stages, but in the performance of each individual motor act
[75].
Bearing these facts in mind, we cannot fail to give attention to the -
rather important circumstance that the "neuronal speed" and speed of
human actions do not coincide. For this reason, the speed of conduction
of neural impulses cannot be interpreted as indirect proof of the po-
tential possibility of passage of information through the feedbac:k
channels. Direct proof of this must be obtained in a psychological,
behavioral experiment.
The conception of J. Adams is an appreciable contribution to the solution
of the problem of constructing and controlling movements. At the same time,
we cannot fail to note that Adams' persistent rejection of the possibility
of constructing programs and their involvement in regulation of movement,
even ir. the variant of generalized schemes, is a step back from the
theory of construction of movements proposed by N. A, Bernshteyn.
- In recent years, an increasing number of works has been published, in
which the alternative between conceptions of open and closed loops is
overcome, and an effort is made to combine the strong points of bott~ con-
- ceptions: construction of the program and correction of movements as they
are performed by means of feedback channels.
We mentioned above that there is a fortunate combination of open and closed
loop conceptions in the theory of N. A. Bernshteyn, i.e., he introduced
117
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
both a program and feedback in his model of construction of movements. R.
Smidt made an analogous attempt at combining the two conceptions, but with
consideration of the latest advances in theory and practice of studies of
movement; he analyzed both theories and arrived at the concluaion that he
was faced with several difficult problems [75]. The first problem is re-
lated to storage and call for motor programs, the number of which is in-
conceivable, if we accept the thesis of "one motor program--one movement."
Closed loop theory also does not eliminate the storage problem; moreover,
_ in this case, not only programs, but stand ards of accuracy to which each
movement must be compared have to be stored. The second pro hlem is re-
- l.ated to the appearance or formation of new movements. Theoretically, the
problem is formulated as follows: where are the programs or accuracy
standards taken from if the performer can produce movements that had
never been performed before�in the very s ame way: Finally, the Chird
~,roblem is the question of how the individual arrives at detecting his own
motor errors and increasing accuracy of subsequent actions. Also unclear
are the mechanisms of deteetion o~ two types��of�errors, which have differ-
ent sources: "noise" in the sensory or motor systems, or the environment.
These difficulties prompted R. Smidt to propose a compromise variant, schema
theory which, as he conceived it, elimina tes them to a significant extent.
He proceeds from the fact that both mechanism.s of regulation are used
widely in the system of movement control, and for this reason there is
no sense to classifying systems as only op en or closed. However, the
relative role of each of them differs substantially, depending on the type
and complexity of movements, time of perf ormance thereof and system level
studied. For example, a computer can, on the one hand, be viewed as an
open loop system, since it can operate without taking into consideration -
any mistakes that may be present in the program; but, on the other hand,
it would be a closed loop system, since the programmer can detect the
error after running the program and make changes in the next series.
Similarly, an open loop system could have a feedback loop that prevents
the program from, for example, dividing by zero, and if such an attempt
is made the internal feedback loop can detect this and make changes in
- running the open loop program.
Analysis of numerous data leads to the conclusion that there are no
motor programs of human behavior tha~ produce movement without feedback.
The moror program gives motor systems all details of the work required
for the limb to travel the distance to a certain target, and feedback is
needed to reach this target. But if it becomes necessary to alter the
target [goal] of movement because of a change in the environment, the
program must be run as before for a certain time (about 150 ms), until
the movement readjusts to reaching the new one. In this case, feedback
- mechanisms are ac tively involved in reaching the "wrong" target under
different conditions. Smidt defines the motor program as a set of pre-
construcCed motor commands, which are expressed, after activation, in
movement directed toward reaching the set goal, and these m~vements are
not affected by peripheral feedback that reports about the need to change
the goal.
118
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
As he develops schema theory, which is called upon to combined the open and
close loop conceptions, Smidt postulates th.at there are two states of
motor memory: one for calling and the other for recognition. Recall
is the structure that is responsible for generation of impulses to muscles
that perform the movement (or make a correction), whereas recognizing -
[or identifying] memory is a structure responsible for evaluating the
feedback produced by movement, which permits pr~duction of information
about an error in movement.
In schema theory it is also assumed that there are "generalized" motor
programs created within the central nervous system and containing muscle
commands with all the necessary details to perform the movement. The
role of the program varies, depending on the duration of movement.
In the case of rapid movement (i.e., one lasting less than 200 ms), the
motor act is under the complete control of recall [memory], in which the
program determines all details of movement in advance.
In the case of slower movements, a motion is made with the use of both
recall and recognition together. The role of recall in this case consists
of prc,duction of small corrective motions, while the main factor that
determines the accuracy of performing the assignment is a comparison of
expected and actual feedback. Consequently, slow movements depend on
recognizing memory, although the subject could make corrective movements
with the use of recall.
Open and closed loop theories, as well as different variants of combina-
tions thereof, constitute a substantial contribution to understanding of
the mechanisms of formation and control of human movements and actions.
An arsenal of functional elements that are important to comprehension
of regulation of movement has been accumulated in studies, upon which
these theories are based. A more eomplex research problem must be solved
next: determination of different types of relations between these elements.
Without solving this problem open and closed loop theories cannot presume
to be the needed scientific basis for practical rationalization, organiza-
tion and planning of new types of work activity. However, with all the
originality and substantiation of several important theses, the are
still general., competing theories of construction of movements, and they
require not only coordination, but development, more details and
experimental verification, as well as, perhaps, correction of some the~es.
Practical experience in ergonomic work indicates that it is far from a
simple matter to turn from general theory developed in physiology, bio-
mechanics or psychology to the solution of practical problens of optimiza-
tion or planning of activity and its means.
To ergonomics, it is not enough to maintain that the theoretical extremes
converge, and that in real activity there is cluse interaction between
programmed and circular [ring] control of man's movements and actions.
119
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Crgonomics is concerned with the specific range of independence or con-
vergence, interaction between the programmed and circular means of control
as applied to various types of movement and specific conditions, including
timittg.
The tenacity of opposition between open and closed loop theories is attri-
butable to the following circumsntances. Movements that were toc differ-
ent in their biomechanical pattern and ohjectives were taken as objects
of research. Natural and instrumental, isolated and chain (series), fast
and slow, inborn and learned,and evoked(reactive) movements were studied.
Methods were used to study them that had different resolution capacities,
ranging From simple observation to rather sophisticated methods of record-
ing the timing and spatial pattern of movements. Organization of move-
ments was studied on different levels, and there were not uncommon cases
' of generalizing results obtained on the psychophysiological, neuropsycho-
logical, biomechanical and psychological levels. Finally, in many studies,
motion was either considered as a whole without sufficient separation into
its structural component, or else individual elements, isolated from the
structure of movement as a whole, were the subject of investigation. All
this caused and now causes great difficulties with regard to comparing
results of different studies. For this reason, as before, it is still a
' pressing scientific and practical task to overcome the opposition between _
open and closed loop theories of regulation.
In these theories, as well as in the experimental studies on which they
were based, not enough attention was devoted to analysis of the object-
oriented content of activity. And even the motor acts studied were usually _
extremely elementary and seldom exceeded in their complexity the standard
variants of stimulus-reactive schemes for studying motion. The means of
- recording motor acts were intended primarily for physiological processes
occurring during performance of movements.
We were also impr essed by the interpretation of the obtained data, which
is made primarily in terms of automatic regulation theory, or cybernetics.
The very names, open loop theory, closed loop theory, are indicative of
the influence of the ideas and methods of cybernetics. Of course,
there is nothing pre3udicial in this influence, and some useful analogies
to technological systems and control of man's execuzory actions did indeed
help clarify many questions and 1ed to formulation of new problems. N. Ye.
Vvedenskiy once wrcte: "Unfortunately, the constructions in the living
world are so complex and original that their meaning is usually learned
only after physicists and engineers arrive at the same results by other
means [15, p 574]. But he also warned that, when observing the activity
of some tissue or organ "one should not overlook the fact that one is
always dealing with liv~~:g entities whose activity is put under the
same conditions as all living organisms" [Ibid, p 566]. There is a great
temptation to consider, by analogy to technological devices, an organ
or function as a mechanism intended only for a certain job, i.e., out
120
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLX
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
of context with the conditions under which its vital functions take place.
However, any analogy has its limits and boundaries. Analogies between
the eye and camera obscura or photographic camera became outdated long ago.
_ We are not dealing with the fact that open or clos-' loop theories have
already experienced the same fate, but that an even broader view must be
developed about human motion and action, including them in the context
of vital functions. At the present time, both theoretical and methodolo-
gical conditions have emerged to overcome the opposition between open and
closed loop theories. The theoretical prerequisites consist o� the fact
that, in many areas of research on mental activity, the technological,
engineering approach, including its current infoxmation-cybernetic variant,
ie being successfully surmounted. The methodological conditions refer to
the fact that, thanks to the use of computers on the experiment line,
basically new opportunities have appeared for recording and analyzing
movements.
As an example, let us mention a study [52], the sub~ect o~ which was to
analyze the correlations between cognitive and executory components of
an instrumental action. The experimental situation provided for rapid and
accurate horizontal movement toward a target, which was a light square
equal in size to the controlled [guided] square and appearing fr~m right
and left of the starting position on the horizontal axis of a television
screen following a computer program. Time and velocity characteristics
of movement were recorded.
Figure 12 illustrates a sample of
tracing of the movement toward a
~ti target, including the tracing
I of the parametric curve of tra-
- ! I jectory as a function of time, data
~ pertaining to velocity and accelera-
~ ~ tion of movement. The appearance
~ i of curves S(t), V(t) and A(t) des-
~ �ij" cribes movements directed toward
~!i) rapid and accurate superposition
( of the controlled spot and target.
~ The rate of movement increases
~ ~�i~~ up to the middle of the trajectory,
~ then begins to drop monotonously
~ until corrective movements begin
I which lead the controlled spot to _
~ the target. In turn, the change
~ in velocity is due to the fact that
the force applied to move the hand
Figure 12, in space and, accordingly, the tool
Sample o� tracing of movement toward that it controls change in time. _
target with characteristics of move- The nature of change in this force
ment time S(t), scatter ~(t), velocity is described by a change in
V(t) and acceleration A(t) acceleration of movement in time
- 121
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
A(t) where we can distinguish a faster segment corresponding to the
start of movement when velocity grows from 0 to a maximum, and the part
of movement when acceleration is negative. At the same time, for each
group of performances (depending on amplitude of displacement), calcula-
tion was made of the atandard [mean square] scatter (6), i.e., determina-
tion was made of the segments of maximum and minimum deviation from the
ideal curve. As shown by analysis, maximum deviation of curve (6) is
observed half way to the target, where the velocity curve shows that it
has reached its maximum. In other words, the scatter is at a minimum at
- the start and end of the trajectory. Hence, it may be assumed that the
movements at the very start of the trajectory, which correspond in time to
the phase of progressive acceleration and are characterized by minimum
scatter (Q), are performed in accordance with a well-prepared program
for this group of movements.
These data are ~~onsistent with the data of proponents of programmed or
open type of control of movements, who postulated that there is a set of
motor programs, which can be synthesized into the desired movement,
cover it entirely and which are independent of feedback. The results of
this study indicate that a programmed type of control is present only for
the first part of a movement, lasting 125-150 ms for the above experi-
mental situation and gorup of movements. As was demonstrated, the
standard scatter increased, reaching a ma~:imum on the section of the
tra~ectory corresponding to maximum velocity covering an interval of
225-275 ms on curve S(t). Because of the many degrees of freedom of
kinematic chains of the human body, effect of reactive and exogenous
forces and other causes., no system of triggering afferent impulses, even -
the most accurately measured, can unequivocally determine the required
- movement. But the movement is made anyway, and rather accurately at that;
and it is made by means of corrections in the course of performing it,
on the basis of efferent signals received during the motor act, by means
_ of "sensory correction." However, the impulses delivered to the nervous
system during performance of the movement are not enough to control the "
action, they must be compared to their set, programmed values, which
makes it possible to make corrections during performance of the action;
correction of the motor act is made on the basis of such comparison. In
other words, there are grounds to combine two types of control in one
motor act, programmed and the one based on feedback, i.e., closed type
of control.
_ The very conceptions of motor program and feedback, which are central to
these theories, also require explanation, particularly since they are
considered in these theories primarily from the standpoint of their
physiological mechanisms. Yet, contemporary research is disclosing
such complications, variations and directions in human action as are
unknown to biomechanics and physiology, at least not in their present -
state. The main difficult is, apparently, that both the program and
~ontrol are derived from the image, just like the image is derived from
122
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~ ~ � ~ -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONL~'
action with an object. This is not a logical circle, and for this reason
it does not have to be broken; but it is necessary to understand the cor-
relations between action and image, otherwise it is impossihle to solve
the problem of construction of movements. It is not by chance that we
have qunted above the statement of I. M. Sechenov to the effect that
feelings serve as sources of movements through the psyche, rather than
directly, i.e., through the image that itself is just as dynamic as the
movement it regulates.
Comprehension of this circumstances distinguishes radically the theory of
N. A. Bernshteyn from open and closed loop theories. With reference to
the function of the "setting" element, he very validly raises the question
of the origin of macroprograms of goal-oriented action and its relation to
a motor problem. The latter is directly or indirectly determined by the
situation that has developed at a given time. In Bernshteyn~s theory, the
image or representation of the result (final or stage-by-stage) of action
is the decisive factor in the appearance and formation of macroprograms ~
of a motor act. "The fact that I refer to the concept of image or repre-
sentation of result of action, which belongs to the realm of psychology,
to characterize the main element of the motor act, with emphasis on the
fact that we are still unable to name the physiological mechanism upon
which it is based, by no means signifies that I do not recognize the
existence of the latter or that it is outside the field of our attention.
At the present time, we are able to find and name with a specific term
the psychological aspect of the prime factor being sought in the inseparable
psychophysiological unity of planning and coordination processes, whereas
physiology, perhaps by virtue of the fact that it is behind in the research
of movements, has sti11 not been able to disclose its physiological
aspect. However, 'ignoramus' does not mean 'ignorabimus"' [7, p 241]. In
spite of this distinct formulation of the problem of regulatory functions
of an image, we cannot fail to note that N. A. Bernshteyn discusses these
functions in their most general form. Obviously, it is expressly in this
point that he refers to psychological research, which cannot bypas:, the
problem o~ for~nati,;:. uf a~~ image chat emerges as a regulator of a voluntary
motor act.
Consideration of orienting-exploring, cognitive components was an important
stage in the study of voluntary movements and skills. A. V. Zaporozhets
demonstrated that an image of the situation and actions that must be per-
foruieJ is formed in the course of orienting-exploratory activity. The
contribution of orientation is particularly significant at the first stages
of formation of voluntary movements [28]. Logic led A. V. Zaporozhets and
his colleagues to differentiation between orienting-exploratory, testing
and actually executory actions. New arguments appeared in favor of the
multifunctionality of movements that can perform both executory and
cognitive functions, which led to creation of theory of perceptual actions
[29-32], development of inethods of microanalysis of cognitive processes,
123
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
including perceptual ones. And executory actions proper were analyzed only
in the most general form: only the time of their performance and accuracy
in reaching the goal were assessed.
Development of theory and methodologi~l armamentarium for the study of
perceptual actions enables us to formulate the task of combining a number
of approaches for the study of voluntary movements and skills: theory c~f
construction and development of movements of N. A. Bernshteyn and A. V.
Zaporozhets, open and closed loop theories (along with the different
variants of combination thereof), and theory of perceptual actions.
The first attempt at such a combin.ation was made on the basis of micro-
str uctural analysis of executory and cognitive activity.
The Eollowing was advanced as the essent�ia1 theoretical basis of necessity
and usefulness of combining these conceptions. In the construction of
movements the superfluous degrees of freedom of kinematic chains of the
human body are overcome. The hypothesis that there is something in common
between the task of constructing movements and the task of constructing a
visual image is not without grounds. In image construction, superfluous
and inadequate variants of reflection of the same object are also over-
come. From the standpoint of regulation and control of voluntary _
movements, apparently it could not be otherwise, since the visual system
represents a substantial part of the regulatory element of a motor act.
For this reason, there must be as many degrees of freedom in the regula-
tory element (which, incidentally, is not necessarily related only to
the visual system) as in the executory one. Otherwise, several of the
- degrees of freedom of the performing [executory] element would inevitably
slip away from the regulatory one [36].
For expressly this reason, proceeding from the principle of innervation
of separate muscles, one cannot explain the integral act of movement, one
cannot discuss similar relations between innervation impulses and the
movements they evoke. Ideas that are similar in meaning have laeen voiced
by M. Turvey [78], who believes that pruposeful movements are not regulated
by a rigid (prepared in advance) pattern, but by an image of ac~~tion thar
is itself a constantly forming structure. It is unlikely that there is
a ready regulatory pattern (standard) for each mode of performing a move-
ment, particularly since the use of many modes of movement and action is
possible without prior learning. Movement is performed by means c~f
matching with one another the structures to be coordinated, which~. are
relatively independent from the standpoint of organization of moveinent.
Formation of movement proper could be viewed as a heterarchy, in th.e
higher regions of which there is a small number of large and comple:x
coordinated structures, and in the lower regions a large amount of :amall
and simple structures. Turvey also believes that the image of future
action or conception of it occupies the central place in organization of .
movement. According to this interpretation of the process of control. of
124
FOR OFFICIA]L USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
_
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
movement, the initial conception of action must be uncertain, as compared
to the final conception in the executory commands for muscles. To put it
in a simpler form, the "image of an action" cannot and should not be
constructive in relation to the concrete details of the motor act. The
action image includes a general evaluation of pose or body schema and
isalated perceptive properties that may be needed to control the movement,
which are also represented in a general form. In a developing movement,
the "action image" is gradually defined at the subsequent levels of
movement control by means of addition of detailed object-related content.
The combination of coordinated motor structures occurs on each level by
means of the correspflnding, visually isolated properties of the environment.
It is imperative to determine how and on what basis an activity is �ormed
that is new to the individual, what its functional structure is and what
are its components.
To answer these questions, inversion was used in an experimental situation
as a means of disrupting a formed skill, with which the perceptual and
motor fields, each individ.uall}, did not actually undergo any changes.
There was merely impairm~~nt of consistency between movement of the key
[manipulatorJ and displo~ement of the~spot on the screen; in other words,
inversion impaired the customary correlation between the,perceptual and
motor fields which, of course, disrupted the sensorimotor image of space
that was formed under compatible conditions, i.e., the means became
inconsistent with the goal. Use of inversion made it possible to track
more thoroughly the stages of development of a new sensorimotor image
of a work space [18, 19].
Let us discuss in more detail the structure of the phasic stage of spatial
action, which changed with inversion from spatial, unique and purposeful
into a set of many movements in different directions, either alternating
with full stops or significant delays. Each such stop indicates that,
having made a small movement, the subject checks himslef and outlines
(programs) his sut~sequent route (Figure 13).
~
x
v
z z
~
_fl ~ fl ~
norm X, Y, Z inveraions
Figure 13. Sample of tr_a~?::b of the start of formation of a skill
with X, Y and Z inversion
125
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY .
In essence, one can count 3-8 complete cycles in the structure of the phase
with a move over one matrix element, and each of these cycles consists of
its own stages of programming, implementation and checking [control]. In
other words, the phasic stage of integral action separat~d into a numher
of mavements in different directions; and if we consider that such hi~h-
amplitude movements in different directions seem to permeate the operational
space were recorded for each element X, Y, Z of spatial action, it becomes
clear how chaotic and disorganized this action appears, and it cannot
actually be called an action, since it is not purposeful and sep�rated. It
can be conceived as artificially connected chains of operations, each of
which has a specific direction, velocity and point of application. Hence,
it is quite obvious that the primordial function of movement--executory--
is transformed at this stage of mastering action into a cognitive, ex-
ploratory, orienting function.
Thus, a new sensorimotor image of space begins to develop on the basis of
active actions that probe the work :~pace in all directions, the function
of which is not executory but exploratory. At the first stage of
development of the sensorimotor image there is formation of a rather
general image of the situation as a whole (Figure 14, Curve 1), which
could be called the stage of construction of an image of a concrete
situation.
The next stage is characterized by long duration, taking up about several
dozen implementations. This stage is chsiacterized by probing motions in
the direction of the target (Figure 14, curve 2). Here there are no
more wide-amplitude movements in different directions. The movement from
one matrix element to another appears to be divided into several successive
operations, in each of which one can clearly distinguish programming,
implementing and checking stages. The subject appears to quantize the
imagined trajectory into small segments,where a build-up in velocity of
performing the action is followed by complete stops. And the less the
image of the space is assimilated, the more quanta there are. It must be
noted that the increase and decrease in velocity occurs separately for each
component of movement, X. Y, Z. This indicates that at this stage of
assimilat~on of the image, the action is not planned simultaneously
(spatially), but successively, separately for each coordinate. Moreover,
it is not planned completely even for one coordinate, but is separated into
quanta where the end of the preceding one serves as the beginning of the
next.
The single action at this stage is transformed into a chain of successive,
probing operations in the direction of the set goal and ultimately reaching
it. Such actions are necessary to match the image formed in general features
with specific motor objectives. In addition, they are apparently directed
toward finding the scalar conformity of hand movement with the location
of the matrix element on the screen.
126
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Thus, the second stage of assimilat-
ing the sensorimotor space could be
called the stage of construction of
the image of real executory actions.
The next stage of the image of sen-
sorimotor space could be referred
to the figurative, orienting part
of action only at the very first
stages of its formation (Figure 14,
~ curve 3). It is characterized by
;i purposeful integral actions, the
1 function of which is essentially
directed toward merging of the
N already formed image of the situa-
tion with the image of real execu-
~p tory actions. This function is
rather complex; it does not re-
n quire a mechanical connection, but
' good penetration of one into the
Figure 14. other and, on the basis, construc-
Diagram of stages of formation of a tion of a simultaneous sensori-
sensorimotor image of space (the motor image, the only one for the
segments of drop in velocity to 0 Prevailing conditions, of the work
have been isolated in the phasic space. The executory part of
stage); JI--latency time; ~--phasic action proper will then be refined
structure on its basis. The presence of such
a single orienting image makes it
- possible for the action program to be formed and perfected at this stage,
the first attempts at construction of which had already been evident at the
stage of construction of the image of executory actions.
How is a connection of regulatory and executory components, each of
which has many degrees of freedom, possible: What is the process of
limitation of number of degrees of freedom in both elements of the motor
act? These questions arise when analyzing a formed motor act, but they
become even more acute with regard to the process of its formation, with
regard to the process of man's mastering both traditional and new tools
of work activity.
Studies of characteristics of cognitive components, as well as of the pro-
cess of their formation are of extreme importance, since expressly they
relate the orienting and executory components of activity.
Comparative qualitative and quantitative analysis of the characteristics
of hand and eye movements made at different stages of mastering motor
- skills demonstrated general patterns of change in the parameters under
- 127
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFYCIAL USE ONLY
study [21]. As the motor skill is learned, there is a reduction both in
total time of performance of the action and in duration of each isolated
stage of integral action, as well as in duration of the period of oculo-
motor acti.vity. The duration of the programming stage of action is
~ proportiot~,ate to the length and difficulty of the trajectory of movement.
When traveling over any trajectory, the latency time of hand movement
- when changing from the start position to the first reference point on
the route is several times longer than the latency time of movement
between any other points on this route, and the more complex the trajec-
tory, the greater this difference. The general sequence of phases of
hand and eye movements is always the same: after delivery of the signal,
a latency period for hand and eye movement is recorded, followed by a
period of oculomotor activity; and the more complex the trajectory, the
longer the last mentioned period; then hand movement begins.
The eye movements observed in the study were divided into two functiona_l:y
different classes. The first class refers to orienting-exploratory eye
movements that are demonstrable only at the latency stage of hand movement.
As the motor skil'1 is formed there is gradual reduction thereof. The
function of orienting-exploratory eye movements is to �orm a perceptual-
motor image of space and plan tnovement over the entire route. The
second class refers to afferenting eye movements that are divided into
two types: saccades tracking hand movements, and jerks toward the
target ahead of the hand movements. As the skill develops, the tracking
jerks are transformed into beforehand ones. The function of afferenting
movements is to compare, correct and determine the scale conformity of
the set program with the real problem.
- At the early stages of learning, subjects who do not know how to use the
manipulator presant many eye movements that intersect the test matrix
during the latency period of hand movement. These eye movements are mainly
of the back-and-forth type. At the performance stage, these subjects
present afferent tracking movements of the eyes, associated with executory
hand action (Figure 15).
As skill develops, there is gradual decrease in back-and-forth eye move-
ments. They persist only during the latency stage of. the first transi-
tion, i.e., before the start of hand movement. This also corresponds to -
a reduction in latency periods of hand movements over each transition to
matrix elements; the first latency period decreases the least. The back-
and-forth saccades are transformed into forward-going ones, directly
preceding executory action. In turn, with a well-developed skill, the
afferent tracking eye movements are transformed into advance ones of
executory action. After the advance saccade, the eye fixes on the target
until the executory hand action is terminated, i.e., until the controlled
spot coincides with the appropriate matrix element (Figure 16).
128
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
a ZI
s
1 .
~ ~
5 F.~~
` ~
~ ~ lu 111
~
:i ~ y~
~ i~,; i
- i
Start ~ _ i.~
lo __~_-~i ~ ^
X ~ 1
/
i /
i i
/ i~
Hand L'~ b N IB~IY
~
Eye x ~ ~ _ i i ' ~
� ~ ~
V- ~ 'l 3 4
_ Figure 15. Sample of tracing of eye movements at the first stage
of learning action -
a) orienting, back jerks in coordinates X, Y of plotter
b) time scan on polygraph; dash line--orienting jerks
I, II, III, IV) matrix elements
1-20) eye fixations
L) latency period
In the course of learning, a new spatial image is formed, and sensorimotor
coordinations corresponding to the experimental situation are altered or
formed anew; after the sensorimotor image is formed, the program of the
action under study begins to form actively. One of the indications of _
a formed spatial image and spatial action is the type of eye movement,
number of eye movements, velocity of hand movements and nature of
sensorimotor interaction.
According to the foregoing, in order to understand the process of trans-
formation of the human hand into the "tool of tools" there must be proper
theoretical and methodological orientation of studies of executory activity.
The movements of a living organ not only must be understood, but disclosed
as a sort of morphological object, functional organ. "Any temporary
combination of forces capable of making a specific achievement" [58, p 71]
is a functional organ. The analogy between movements of a living organ
and anatomical organs or tissues was conclusively substantiated by its two
~ most important properties: in the first place, living motion reacts;
in the second place, it consistently undergoes evolution and involution
[7, p 178]. Such'interpretation of living movement, distinction of its
129
. FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
I
4
fi
S
10 3
2
Start~~~~
t
11 II
~ s 2
9
~ IlI
Y 8.
~ IV
X b ~
hand x L , ~i ~i
eye x ' 'i
. 1 2 3 a 5 6
Figure 16. Sample of tracing of eye movements at the last stage
of Iearning an action (designations are the same as
in Figure 15)
"biodynamic tissue" as the object of investigation provides a new strategy
f or scientific study and practical organization thereof. In particular,
this also means that movement, the motor s~chema, skill cannot be assimi-
laCed; they have to be constructed by the subject. "Exercise is repetion
without repetitions" (7].
It is known that as man learns a specific system of movements it becomes
s tereotyped. But then, this system, which was previously something
exogenous, an object to be assimilated, gradually changes into a
d istinctive organ of individuality, a means of expression and realiza-
t ion of man's attitude toward reality" [28, p 394]. Modern ergonomics
is increasingly concerned with the structure of this "organ of individu-
ality," understanding and foreseeing what could be done with its help.
3. Functional Structure of Cognitive Actions
With each year, there is more and more new experimental confirmation of
interpretation of inental processes as special cognitive actions formed
during ontogenetic and functional development, and practical applications
are being found in ergonomics and engineering psychology. Specialization
and differentiation of work activity have led to frequent limitation of
the functions of a worker mainly to the area of perception, as a result
130
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFCCIAL USE ONLY
of which processes of detection, identification, recognition, information
retrieval, short-term storage and transmission of information, and
decision making emerge as independent purposeful actions in the work pro-
cess. Of course, each such action ends with a specific executory act,
i.e., it is part of a broader structur e of activity; but since these
executory acts are often quite element ary, professional competence can be
determined by perceptual or intellectual components. For this reason, ergo- _
nomics is ref erring more and more of ten to general, experimental and even
genetic psychology; it actively formulates and solves new problems that are
within the competence of these branches of psychology.
New technological means of activity require the formation of special per-
ceptual abil.ities, actions and skills. The diverse forms of activity of
operator-obser~~ers, which appeared in the last decades, illustrate the
best the known thesis that human sense orgacis are the product of all
prior worldwide history. The study of these types of activity under
real and laboratory conditions has led to accumulation of enormous factual
material, which has been generalized in a number of theories and models,
the substance of which is that ~hey overcame naturalistic conceptions of
human capabilities in general and cognitive capabilities in particular. ~
There is an extensive psychological literature dealing with functional,
structural and genetic aspects of perception, memory and thinking pro-
cesses. In this section, we shall limit ourselves to a general descrip-
tion of the most important cognitive processes, which play a leading role
in work activity, and we shall submit material that may be useful in
solving ergonomic planning problems. Special attention will be given to
the enormous reserves available in human perception, memory; reserves
that may alleviate substantially the solution of complex technological
problems if they are used rationally.
In the preceding section, we demonstrated the significance of image of a
~ situation and image of actions, which must be performed in this situation
for development of skills. Studies of images and distinctions of their
formation are becoming central to cognitive psychology as wel 1, which
is successfully overcoming, through the work of its most farsighted
representatives [68, 71], the stimulus-reactive and behavioristic schemes
~ that had been used for a long time to analyze behavior and activity.
The concept of image is being to play an increasingly noticeable role in
engineering psychology and ergonomic research. The information model of
a real situation in man-machine systems must first be analyzed by the
operator; he must form his own graphic-conceptual model of the situa-
tion, make a decision and only then p erform the executory action. This
example clearly demonstrates the inad equacy of explanatory stimulus-
reactive schemes. There is dual likening to reality, or two images, two
models of reality between action and reaction in the activity of an opera-
tor. Each requires specific cognitive actions by the operator, which
are performed both externally and int ernally.
131
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Purposeful action can no longer be implemented on the order of immediate
servicing; it is performed accordin$ to schemes of deferred servicing or, -
more precisely, active action; there is repetition and transformation of
phenomena into an information model in the interval between action and
reaction; this model is obtained by technical means; repetition and
transformation of phenomena in the graphic-conceptual model are ob-
tained by psychological means.
Information and graphic-conceptual models emerge as synthetic elemer~ts
that disclose to man space that is accessible to comprehension and � �
action of the world. Of course, graphic-conceptual and information models
are not identical, but the description thereof in similar terms alle-
viates substantially the task of synthesis of man-machine systems.
It is useful to discuss the most general properties of visual images in
order to comprehend processes of formation of graphic-conceptual models,
as well as transformation proceases that are performed for information
processing and decision making.
Images are subjective phenomena that arise as a result of object-related
practical, sensory-perceptual and thinking activity in both the presence
- and absence of adequate sensory stimulation. An image is the integral,
whole reflection of reality, in which there is simultaneous representa-
tion of the main perceptual categories (space, motion, color, shape,
composition, etc.), and, as is well-known from psychology of perception,
the effects of these catego:ies on the observer are not independent. The
most important function of an image is to regulate executory acCs. It is
logical to conceive that a regulator is just as real as an actuating
mechanism and that it has the same properties as the regulated ob3ect. In
the preceding section we submitted arguments in favor of consideration of
living motion as a special functional organ having, by analogy with mor-
phological organs, the properties of react~vity and sensitivity, which
are governed by the laws of evolution and involution.
It is not difficult to detect analogous properties in cognitive processes
as well.
Perception, memory and thinkingare also actions (ar systems of actions),
each of which undergoes reactive evolution and involution [28, 30, 41~.
- The results of these actions are first fixed in images (motor, perceptual,
mnemic, mental) which, in turn, perform regulatory functions with regard
to subsequent occurrence of cognitive and executory acts. We always
localize images of real objects in external space, where the objects of
perception or action are.
The same applies to visualized images, representations, which the observer _
sees in the absence of the object of observation. An image does not
exist as a certain sub~ective datum outside the process of objectivization,
132
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
' FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
exteriorization. Thanks to the localization of an image in external, three-
dimensional space (including its analogs transformed by the sub~ect [73,
74], it is possible to regulate executory actions performed externally.
In other words, regulation of executory acts is possible only through the
subject-related [subjective?] environment, reflected in the objectivized
ima ge .
A distinction is made between two types of structures in visual perception:
spatial, which is related to localization of the outside world in the
coordinates of three-dimension space, and structure of proximal stimula-
tion, which ie related to the anatomical coordinates oF the retina. It
is possible to demonatrate the relative independence of these structures
from one another in special studies, although they are interrelated in
a real act of perception. Both structures are also characterized by
certain iconic (pictorial) properties [79]. The iconic properties of
these structures constitute Che sensitive fabric of the image (and con-
sciousness) which is usually fused with the tangible content of perceived
reality [48], i.e., it is localized in external three-dimensional space.
It is expedient to continue the discussion of image properties in terms _
of biodynamic and sensory tissue, although this separation cannot be _
absolute, since there are iconic, sensory properties in biodynamic
tissue of movement (see Section 2). The spatial structure of an image
is formed as a result of tangible [object-related] actions of the subject,
by virtue of transformation of biodynamic tissue of movement into the
sensory t~ssue of the spatial image. This applies not only to the pro- _
cess of formation of an image, but to the formed image; for a halt can
be interpreted as accumulated movement, a simultaneous cast thereof.
A form of biodynamic tissue of movement is present in both the generated
and embodied image. _
As a spatial image is formed, it is filled with tangible properties,
imbued with sensory tissue and, with it, is localized in external space.
This applies to both sensory tissue that is related in origin to bio-
dynamic tissue, and sensory tissue related to the iconic properties of
proximal stimulation. The latter is also exteriorized and fused with the
spatial structure of the image. After such fusion, the image emerges as
an integral, indivisible whole. _
Consequently, both sides of the same whole appear to be represented by
biodynamic and sensory tissue in the formed image. Moreover, they become
reversible. Biodynamic tissue of movement, action, plays the leading
role in formation of a spatial image. In the formed image, the leadirig
place is occupied by sensory tissue, including that originating from
proximal stimulation. The reverse occurs in construction of movement,
i.e., the sensory tissue of the image is transformed into the biodynamic
tissue of movement. Ultimately, movement is, so to speak, the substance,
the she11 of the image. But if the thesis is valid that activity dies in -
the product, it should be just as valid that the image dies, is embodied
in activity in order to be reborn as a result of its completion. For
133
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY _
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
expreasly this reason images have the property of openness. The sensory
tissue of a spatial image, which is related in its origin to the sub~ect's
active movements in the outside world, can emerge as a regulator of
executory actions. Performance of the latter again leads to transforma- ~
tion of biodynamic tissue into sensory, to expansion and fixation in
the image of more and more new properties of tangible reality. However,
very often the formed detailed image of the surroundings is too super- _
fluous to solve utilitarian problems of regulating executory acts,
although it is, of course, necessary to decision making as to the pur-
posefulness of some action or other. Transformation of the spatial image,
of its biodynamic tissue, into a more or less automated scheme is a means
of overcoming superfluousness in stereotyping and standardizing the
conditions f or performing action. In the schemes that are formed as a
result of such transformation, then in the sqmbols, there is amplification
of elements of abstraction and, consequenlty, a decrease in share of bio-
- dynamic and, particularly, sensory tissue.
The foregoing leads us to the conclusion that images, ~ust like movements,
should be viewed as functional organs for regulation of behavior. This
interpretation of an image as an organ of individuality ensues from the
views of A. A. Ukhtomskiy, who considered the dominant as a special func- _
tional organ. He wrote about its ~xternal and internal expression [58J.
Stationary p~rformance of work or working pose of the body is ref erable
- to the external expression of the dominant~ The internal expression refers
to experiencing the dominant in the form of an abbreviated symbol
- ("psychological recollection"). B. G. Anan'yev, who also stressed that
the whole, or integral image can be viewed as a distinctive organ of
betiavior, called attention Co this aspect in the works of A. A. Ukhtomskiy.
Such a single interpretation of movements, images and sets as functional
organs of individuality makes it easier to demonstrate the correlations
- existing between them.
Each man has numerous images of the most diverse spaces: rooms, streets, _
~ cities, favorite painting, etc. Some of us can find our b~arings well
in microscopic space and even in outer space, and unquestionably there
is a capacity to readily move from working in one space to working in
another space. As a rule, images of the surroundings also include
_ the "body schema." Body schema is man's general conception of his body,
its outline and dimensions, boundaries, orientation and movement in
surrounding space. F. D. Gorbov [17] observed that as man continuously
changes the position of his body he concurrently creates and tests the
postural model that forms the body schema. The perceived boundaries of
the body schema are extremely mobile. The body schema incA vividlexamnle
and diverse work tools (pen, shovel, car, tanker, etc.) . P
of the spatial properties of images are the phenomena experienced by
amputees of movement of phantom limbs, when the stump does not really
move.
134
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
These examples are indicative of the gradual shifting of the individual's
sensitivity into external space, of the individual's development of
increasingly adequate and complex spatial images and models of reality. _
Of course, d epending on the ob~ ective of activity, reflection of physical
space in subjective images may be transformed. It may be perceived in
direct and reverse perspective, deliberately reduced or extended,
schematized, etc. The description of sub3ective images, conceptions and
actions in terms of time and space properties is no more arbitrary than
the description of DNA in the form of a double helix. It is not by chance
that specialists in the field of industrial psychology and planning
ergonomics have long since operated with such terms and concepts as
motor field space, time and space properties of motion and perception,
graphic-figurative.schemes, which guide man's activity in the work space.
They also uae such terms as operational unit of perception, image-
manipulator, which bears a re~lection of reality, as well as its inter-
pretation and plan of action.
Not only space, but time are reflected ir~ visual images. There are elements
of the present, past and future in simultaneous pictures ("arres~ed moments").
Reflection of time in images is based both on mechanisms of perception and
extrapolation of movements and mechanisms that are similar to the semi-
transparent f ile of traces fixed at different points in time. On the one
hand, this permits perception of the world as stable and, on the other,
consideration of past, current and future changes in it. Consequently,
visual images permit potential and actual reflection of reality with
all the wealth of both vi~ible relations between ob~ects and latent ones
at a given time.
Reflection of time in images is Che basis of phenomena that are described
in terms of "foreseeable future" (N. A. Bernshteyn) and "acceptor of
results of action" (P. K. Anokhin) .
It is an extremely�difficult matter to create an adequate conceptual system
to describe the structural and functional features of time and space `
schemes and constructions present in images, since they are usually con-
cealed, not only from external observ ation but introspectian. In ontogenesis,
the basic perceptual categories that form the foundation for subjective
[ob~ect-related] meanings are virtually learned prior to development of
processes of verbal communication, within the framework of which there is
initial formation of symbolic knowledge of the world. As the powerful
system of voluntary regulation of activity, which is speech in adult man,
develops the impression is gained that processes of graphic-figurative
reflection begin to play a subordinate r~le. It must be stressed that,
at the stage of fully developed verbal communicati~n, perception (and
content of the image) is not identical to the process of reference to
some arbitrary categories or other.
135
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
With respect to information, images are an exceptioanlly capacious form of
representation of reality. There is room in them for information about
time and space, dynamic, color and figurative features of objects. They
are multidimensional, multicategorial, as well as multimodal. The images
reflect noC only basic perceptual categories, but correlations between
Chem, both within one category and in intermodal correlations. Hypotheses
have been expounded to the effect that graphic images are readily trans-
formed into amodal images, perceptual or tangible concepts--complexes
("diffuse concepts"), etc. In other words, images are multilayered, hoth
genetically and functionally, which enables man so to speak move into
the realm of symbolic meanings and concepts, to reflect on the upper
layers of the image of the world he has constructed, to deliberately
operate with signs, symbols and words. As for basic perceptual categories,
although they do serve as guidelines for man's practical activity, they
seldom become the subject of reflection. Of course, man continues to make
effective use of graphic-sensory, figurative reflections of tangible
reality, but mainly in a discrete, latent form.
Just liice the different aspects of complex motor acts are implemented by
the coordinate function of different levels of construction of movements, -
' the perceived spatial localization of ob3ects and description of their
shape are, according to the results of special studies [11], products
of information processing on different levels of construction of the
image. In perception, just like in regulation of movements, primarily
the object-related content is recognized, which corresponds to the
meaning aspect of the task confronting the subject. Background coordina-
~ tions, implemented at lower levels, are not represented in the focal
area of consciousness, even when dealing with such processes as reflec-
tion of brightness features or movements of an object. This latency of
perception, which is beneficial to the sub,ject, does not relieve psychology
of its quite conscious consideration, the task of reconstructing this
amazing world of inental reality, of the search and dedelopment of ob-
jective and, at the same time, psychological methods of studying it.
Reconstruction of background coordinations on the lower levels of the
process of formation of a tangible image is particularly important, since
ob~ects, situations and events are represented in coded form in informa-
tion models. There are not uncommon cases when the most informative
signs of reflected objects are encoded by distribution of brightness,
movement, while the spatial features of objects are coded as alphanumeric
information or points and lines on the plane of the means of display.
In sitch cases, operators have to reconstruct the situation on the basis
of input information that is known to be sparse and often distorted. In
other words, the background, unconscious levels of operator activity
become the object of special perceptual actions, and it is only on their
basis that the tangible image of the reflected situation can be formed.
136
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Studies of operator work show that reference of information the operator
receives to real ob~ects is often done as a quite conscious action, which
causes certain difficulties and is not easily exercised or automated. M.
L. Gallay wrote about these difficulties: "I can imagine how the pilot's
glance darted from instrument to instrument during this turn: bank,
acceleration, velocity, climb, bearing, bank again, velocity again....
Inertia causes the body to adhere tightly to the seat.... He is trembling -
from the strain of the aircraft.... Beyond the steamed windows of the .
cabin is nothing but milky darkness, but the pilot sees, with the
_ inner vision developed over years of flying, what a clever curve, on the _
very border of what is feasible, his craft is describing." In this
description, we are impressed, in the first place, by the fact that the
pilot does not see the instruments as he does the tra~ectory of his
craft in space and, in the second place, that this vision is the result
of the function of internal vision developed over years of flying. This
example i.s not an exception. There are many occupations, the main content
of which is the perception and recognition of visual images, their inter- _
. pretation and processing. Decoding aerial photogr_~phs, photos in tracking
chambers and x-rays may serve as examples. Specific problems arise when
organizing man's activity under such conditions, which alter appreciably
the characteristics of sensory and perceptual processes, for example,
visual perception in space without landmarks, perception in weightless-
_ ness or in the presence of distorting media. Although this may sound
paradoxical, perception, which seems so natural and spontaneous, is
a serious and sometimes very difficult job. The comp~exi*yo uf many _
_ professions related to infarmation receiving and processing is referable
to the fact that one has to detect clear and distiuct signs of specific
physical events in a confused and vague picture, i.e., construct an
image of these events with tangible significance, which cou_ld then be
converted to symbolic, verbal form.
Visual images have an enourmous capacity for information. As compared to
auditory and motor images, they are characterized by subjective simul-
_ taneousr.ess, which permits instantaneous "grasp" of the relations be-
tween elements of a real or imagined situation. Simultaneousness charac-
terizes not only perception of real objects, but reflected ones, including
those that are coded. For this reason, the use of multidimensional codes
(combinations of color, shape, configuration, etc.) does not increase per-
ception time, as compared to one-dimensional codes [42].
Images have a greater associative force than words. For this reason, it _
is possible to store images well in memory. After single presentation of
several thousand pictures, observers are capable of correctly recognizing
about 90% [12].
137
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY _
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
In addition to reflecting reality, visual images have intentional and
affective components; for this reason, regulation of behavior and acti-
vity by means of an image is remarkable in that it allows for a certain
' degree of activity that is independent of the immediate external situation.
In other words, the images are subjective and biased. There are also
operational components in images, since their origin is related to action.
The presence of operational components enables images to become transformed
_ into perceptual-motor schemes and perform the function of regulating be-
havior, with due consideration of external circumstances, as well as
motivational and goal-related aspects of activity.
The next group of properties is related to their lability and flexibility.
These properties are manifested primarily by the fact that rapid transi-
tions are possible from a general evaluation of a situation to detailed
analysis of its elements on the figurative plane. They implement diverse
spatial discplacements of objects reflected in the images, changes therein,
turns, as well as enlargement, reduction, perspective distortion and
normalization. This distinctive manipulative capacity of the visual sys-
tem [36] makes it possible to represent a situation in both direct and
reverse perspective. Image manipulation serves as the means of solving
recognition problems, it makes a certain contribution to mechanisms of
_ consistency of perception, and ~.t is also a most important means of
productive perception and visual thinking [40, 64, 73]. Collision or
combination of different images may serve a meaning-forming function. As
we well know, the degree of control of image manipulations may vary
significantly.
Productive manipulations of images are the most effective when they occur
either in the absence of an ob~ect of observation or when there is detach-
ment from the external situation. Visualization and manipulation of images
on the level of graphic representation interfere with perceptual work
directed toward external reality and, to a lesser extent, they interfere
with processes of pronunciation, inner speech. This creates a possibility
for parallel recording of results obtained from work with images in
verbal meanings. Incomplete, partial images, in which there is an
element of something "unfinished," impairment of equilibrium, tension,
etc., are induce3 more by the manipulative capacity of the visual system
than completed images.
Studies of manipulative capacity of the visual system lead to the conclu-
sion that a formed image is a multifunctional organ of behavior. In it
is fixed the multilevel reflection of reality; it is the regulator of
executory acts and, at the same time, emerges as the "sub~ect" of repro-
ductive or productive activity and, finally, as its product. Of course,
images that are formed as a result of tangible, practical action differ from
those formed as a result of perceptual actions. The same applies to images
formed as a result of mnemic or mental activity. There are also differ-
ences between images formed in the process of learning Lexamining] and those
138
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
that regulate executory action. Although they have a very close rela-
tionship to one another, their content, completeness, level of generaliza-
tion and other features are different. These properties of images depend
on the problem being so lved by the subject and the methods he uses, i.e.,
the nature of perceptual actions used by the sub3ect.
Development of perception leads to the acquisition of new quality to
both the image af one's own body and images of ob~ects in the outside
world and they can become part of the linguistic, semantic space.
Images and the perception process as a whole become accessible to
reflective analysis. Along with perception of the ob3ect there is
awareness of its funct3 one, thanke to which perception becomes ~eneralized
and categorized. Verbal generalization permits drawing upon complex
semantic associations established in language for analysis and distinction
of the aspects of the p erceived ob~ect that would have otherwise remained
inac'iequately perceived. Ob~ectivization of images permits "running
through" variants of behavior and activity on another substrate, the
sul~strate of reflection, model, image, before performing executory ac-
tions on the real substraCe.
This, of necessity brief , description of visual images confirms the pre-
viously expounded thesis Chat the study of processes of reeeiving and
processing information without consideration of the enormous informational,
cognitive and creative potential contained in the tangible-practical
and sensory-tangibl~.* forms of reflection of reality could lead to a
drastic underestimation of man~s actual capabilities for perception and
information processing. Man has realy inexhaustible reserves for in-
creasing the "carrying capacity" of perception. Tt is only a matter
of properly utilizing these reserves, i.e., creating external means of
activity designed for the strong points of cognitive processes, and not
the weak ones.
Processes of image formation, recognition and operation take place by means
of special perreptual a ctions.
Perceptual actions: Ac cording to current conceptions, perception is an
aggregate of processes that provide for subjective, biased and, at the
same time, adequate ref lection of reality. The adequacy of the image
is not a given from th e start, it is achieved because as the image of
perception is formed there is comparison of perceiving systems to the '
properties of the stimulus. With regard to prace in the structure of
activity, perceptions are usually actions, with the exception of cases
when the creation of an adequate or new image constitutes an independent
motive. The requirements made of perception by practical activity are
called perceptual prob lems. To perceive means to solve some perceptual
problem by creating an adequate reflection of the situation; for this
*Translator's note: Tangible, i.e., related to an object.
139
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
reason, perception is a system of pex'ceptual actions. Perceptual action
consists of various operations and functional units. Perceptual a ction
is an active, dynamic process regulated by the objectives of activity,
which has feedback and anticipation mechanisms and is governed by the
distinctions of the ob,ject examined. The activity of perception consists
primarily of participation of effector components that emerge in the
form of movements of receptor systems and displacement of the body or
parts thereof in space. These movements are divi.ded into two maj o r
classes. The first ref ers to searching and setting movements, by means
of which a search is made of the specified object, the eyes are se t.in
the most convenient position for perception and change in this pos ition.
The same class includes head movements in response to an abrupt sound,
tracking movements of the eyes, etc. Such movements not only create
optimum conditions to perceive an object, they also participate in
determining its spatial position.
- The second class consists of actua].ly gnostic movements. They are
directly involved in estimating dimensions, recognition of familiar ob-
jects and, finally, the actual process of construction of the image.
There is continuous comparison of the image to the original in the
~ hand movements probing an object.and eye movements tracing the vis ible
outline. If these do not correspond to one another, there is correction
of the image. Consequently, the role of the motor system in perception
is not limited to creating optimum conditions for the function of
aff erent systems, and it consists of the fact that movements themselves
are involved in formation of a sub~ective image of the ob~ective world.
In order to determine in greater detail the role of perceptual actions
in formation of an image, it is expedient to use the line of reasoning
that is analogous, to some extent, to the one used by N. A. Bernshteyn to
def ine the role of sensory corrections in regulation of human movements.
Because of the many degrees of freedom of ob,jects around a subjec t, as
compared to What is perceived by him and the infinite diversity of condi-
tions under which they appear, they constantly alter their appearance
and face us with their different sides. In other words, no senso ry
impul~e ar stimulus alone can unequivocally determine the appearance
of an adequate image of perception. Here, a correction is needed to
rectify inevitable errors and it causes the image to conform with the
ob j ect.
However, if such an image is materialized only in internal processes of
the body (states of the receptor and cortical end of analyzer), i t would
be impossi,ble to compare it to the original and, consequentl.y, the
necessary correction could not be made. Consequently, there must be
exteriorization of the reflective process, and this is what occur s in the
form of perceptual actions. Just like motor behavior of an individual can
comply with the problem conditions only by virtue of sensory correction,
so the adequacy of perception is ultimately assured by effector correction,
140
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
In general, the physiological scheme of activity (no matter whether we
- are dealing with the scheme of the reflex arc or reflex ring) cannot
"contain the object in itself" w~th its specific tangible properties
is the broader aspect of this problem. Within the framework of this
scheme, the object can only appear external in relation to a given
process, as a stimulus that has to be recoded into a series of nervous
impulses. For the object to be included in the system of human acti-
vity, we must go beyond the boundaries af its physiological description
and conaider it psychologically as external, purposeful activity of
the individual. The latter contains the object with a11 its specific
distinctions as its own organic component. This applies in full and
primarily to work tools, which are contained in ma~~'s "body schema" to
such an extent that sensitivity is transferred to t.heir boundaries.
Special training and rather long practice are required to master the
system of perceptual actions. It is important that .both perceptual
actions themselves and the criteria of image adec!uac~r do not remain un-
� changzd, but undergo a considerable process of dc:velc~pment, along with
development of activity itself.
The process of image formation consists of a seri~~s of perceptual actions,
such as detection, isolation of informative signs cons.tstent with the
tasks of activity, examination of isolated signs and a~:tual construction
of the image. Perceptual actions are seen in their ex~?anded external
form only at the early stages of antogenesis or functi~~nal genesis, when
the observer encounters perceptual content that is ne~i to him. In such
cases, there is the most distinct demonstration of tt;eir structure and
role in formation of images of perception.
Thereafter, they undergo a number of successive changes and reductions,
until they take on the form of an instantaneous act of "perceiving" the
object, which has been described by the representatives of gestalt
psychology and mistaken bq them for initial, genetically primary form
of perception.
The most important property of perception is the ability to alter perceptual
images and models of the outside world and possibility of changing the
means of constructing and recognizing them. The same object can serve as
a prototype for many perceptual models. They are refined in the course
of their formation, invariant properties and signs are extracted from
the object and this ultimately leads to perception of the world as it
really is. The diversity of possible perceptual images of the same situ-
ation or object is attributable to the fact that external perceptual
actions, like executory actions, contain the reflection of the motor
problem. Involvement of differently organized movements and actions in
perception processes is also the basis for explanation of subjectivity
and bias of perception. In the course of development of perceptual
actions, there is also formation and development of their cognitive products,
141
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
which include sensory and perceptual standards, operational perception
units, schemes, images, etc.
Format ion of sensorimotor standards play a most important role in percep-
tion; they do not correspond to isolated properties of reality, but to
systems of socially developed sensory qualities [29]. They include the
general ly recognized scale of musical sounds, "phoneme grid" of the na-
tive 1 anguage, system of geometric forms, etc. While sensory standards
are th e result of sociohistorical endeavors of mankind to isolate and
- create systems of sensory qualities needed to get one's bearings in the
world, the result of man's individual activity to learn the sensory
standa rds is called operative unit of perception. The operative units
of per ception are compact, semantic, integral elements formed as a result
of per ceptual (including occupational) training, and they make it .
possib le for virtually instantaneous (simultaneous, one-act) integral
percep tion of objects and situations, regardless of the number of tags
[signs] they contain. Concretely, operative [operational] units of per-
ception emerge as the ~~ontent extracted by the individual when performing
some p erceptual task or other. Development of perception is related to
change in operative units. As shown by studies, this change is manifested
by th e transformation of groups of random, special tags into structured,
integr al ones [32, 61]. Concurrently, there is also a change and
refinement of perceptual actions themselves.
Whenever an individual encounters a reality that is new to him, or
when a previously formed image turns out to be inadequate, the perception ~
proce s s again changes from simultaneous to successive, and it occurs by
means of expanded perceptual actions.
There are special recognizing actions in developed processes of percep-
tion. They are used to single out the informative content, from which
the o b server can compare the presented object to already formed
opera tive units of perception, identify it and, finally, refer it to a
class, i.e., categorize it. Recognition takes considerably less time
than image formation. It is enough, for comparison and recognition, to
isola te in the ob~ect only some typical, informative signs. This is
feasib le because prior experience in active orgariization of perceptual
action, i.e., well-learned "schemes" for examining an object, has been
accumulated in operative units of perception. These schemes emerge as
an aggregate of rules or genera~l motor programs intended for singling out
the important aspects of what is "typical" in a given class of objects.
These properties of operative units of perception are the basis not only
of processes of examination and identification, but of generation or
visualization of the image, which occur in the absence of a physical
stimulus. Such interpretation of operative units of perception is
similar to the schema theory of F. Bartlett [65] and the concept of
"general efferent readiness" of the individual, which
142
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
is central to the motor theory of visual perceptio n expounded by L.
Festinger et al. [69]. According to this theory, conscious visual per-
_ ception of the outline of an ob~ect is determined by the ~'efferent
readiness" of the individual to perform specific movements of the
eyes,hands, head and trunk in response to incoming visual information.
Efferent readiness refers to the agreggate of preprogrammed efferent
instructions (motor programs) that are activated by visual information
and are in a state of readiness for instant use.
Efferent readiness, actualized by a stimulus, may be related either to
deployment of perceptual and identifying actions, or implementation of
adaptive, executory actions. In the latter case, efferent readiness
accelerates performance o� executory acts and could serve as the source
of erroneous action.
- To return to the description of operative units of perception, it must
be stated that they reflect not only the subjective aspect of perception,
but objective characteristics of problem conditions, possible strategies
and means of solving it. They have a reflective component (sensory tissue,
perceptual meaning, etc.) and a dynamic, operative component (efferent
readiness for further deployment of perceptual actions directed toward
more complete formation of image of the situation, r~adiness for visuali-
zation and even performance of executory actions in familiar, simple
situations). This means that there may be fusion of perceptual meanings
�and efferent readiness to actualize generalized motor programs in the
operative units of perception.
_ The sensory standards, like the operative units of perception, should be
viewed as certain tools, instruments for perceptual and identifying
actions. The standards mediate these actions just like practical (labor)
activity is mediated by a tool and thinking,by wo rds.
Development of perception leads to the creation of a rather vast alphabet
of operative units of perception, i.e., a certain set of schemes, perceptual
models of the environment. While at the phase of image construction and
transformation there is comparison of perceptual systems to the proper-
ties of the stimulus in the operative units of perception, at the phase
of recognition or executory actic,.. there is substantial change in
" characteristics and direction of the process on the basis of the formed
operative units of perception. These changes consist of the fact that
the individual no longer only recreates the image of an object by
means of perceptual actions, but records, translates the information '
received into the language of operative units of perception or perceptual `
models that are already known. In other words, concurrently with com-
garison [likening) of the perceptual systems to the object, there is
;'_ik~ning of the object to the individual, and only this two-way trans-
formation leads to formation of a complete, adequate and, at the same
time, subjective image of objective reality.
143
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The forego ing indicates that perception processes are active, historical and
object-related. The last quality of perception emerges in the form of
integrity, constancy and meaningfulness of the perceptual image. Per-
ception is integral, since it reflects not the isolated qualities of
stimuli b ut the relations between them. The integrity of perception is
closely re lated to its constancy, which refers ~o the relative independence
of perceived characteristics of the obj ect from projected characterisCics
- of their reflections on the receptor surfaces of sense organs. Active
perceptua 1 actions serve as the source of constancy. The relatively
invariant structure of object properties is singled out of the variable
flow of s timulation by means of percep tual operations. The operative
units, which are formed under the most diverse conditions, permit active
considera tion of changes in projection properties of the object and
compensate for them. As this is done, the reflection of the object _
remains unchanged both in relation to movements of the ob3ect and move-
ments of the observer. Consequently, changes in projection properties
of the obj ect may even be necessary to retain constancy.
As we hav e mentioned, the visual system has a very distinct manipulative
capacity which, like external perceptual actions, is derived from prac-
tical, tangible actions. One of the most important problems that are
- solved wi th these perceptual mechanisms is counterchange in operative
units of perception to comgensate for changes in stimulation from an
objectively stable object. The ability to manipulate an image enables us
to perceive as stable and constant obj ects that are seen at different
angles, f rom different distances, as well a~ when there is a relative
shift in the field of vision due to eye movements.
Manipulation of image and operative units of perception occur by means
of a special class of perceptual actions that were named vicarious.
Various sections of a successive image are analyzed by means of vicarious
eye move~ents. Typically enough, vicarious eye movements are observed
after tachistoscopic display of images, which is too brief for any
searching eye movemenrs. They are also observed when an image is
stabilized in relation to the retina, in dreams, when imagining an
ob~ect in its absence, when working with visualized images, etc. In
the last cases, they serve to analyze and transform visual images.
Vicarious perceptual actions replace action with real ob~ects, antici-
pate and plan them. Evidently, the mechanism of vicarious perceptual
actions consists of selective change in sensitivity of different parts
of the retina, which is controlled by low-amplitude eye movements. These
movements are made in the 2-5� zone, and they are in the form of either
drift or rapid saccades. This mechanism has been named the functional
fovea mechanism [36~.
Depending on the complexity of the task, prior experience of the indi-
vidual, including the operative units of perception consistent with the
task, performance thereof may require various perceptual actions:
144
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR O1~FICIAL USE ONLY
_ detection, identification, recognition, information retrievat, etc. In
turn, each of these actions may be performed as a more or less complete
part of perceptual operations. For example, viaual estimation of distance
is possible through consideration of many different signs of distance
to the ob~ect (disparity, monocular parallax of movement, differences in
- angular dimensions of close and distant ob~ects, height, position of
the object in the field of vision, etc.). Different signs are used,
depending on observation conditions; and althougr the concrete percep-
tual operations are different in each case, the result--formation of an
idea about distance of the ob~ect--is about the same. The same applies
to perception of form, which is possible by means of both touch and
vision. Recognition processes may occur as single, simultaneous actions
or acquire an expanded form of comparison of different signs of the
object to those of the standard.
Even the detection process, which, it would seem, occupies the initial
position in the system of perceptual actions, may consist of expanded
processes of information retrieval, identification, comparison and recog-
nition. In other words, in each individual case, there is actualization
or formation of a functional structure of perceptual actions and operations
consistent with conditions of activity, depending on the task, the
tangible content of activity and experience of the observer.
. In solving many scientific and, particularly, applied problems, one often
encounters situations that are beyond the "resolution capacity" of
analysis of macrostructure of cognitive processes. This applies
entirely as well to perceptj.on, which emerges most often as an opera-
tion in everyday life and occupational activity. Of course, it does not -
cease to be a complex mental process. The terms simultaneousness or
"in one act" are no more than epithetics that conceal [mask] the
true complexity of formed perceptual actions. For this reason, in order
- to comprehend and optimize perceptual processes, we need the means for
microszructural analysis, self-styled probes, with which it would
be possible to examine highly productive, though brief, mental processes.
In other words, for many practical tasks, we need to use and develop
principles of analysis of microstructure of activity, which would
yield a detailed description of perceptual actions and operations, and,
what is equally important, would define the nature of coordinations
formed between them. This applies both to perception of the tangible
surroundings (including analysis of phases of perception on a real time
scale--microgenetic aspect of studying perceptiun) and to the study of
processes of reception, storage, use and reproduction of graphic, sym-
bolic and other types of information.
Let us first consider the situation of microgenesis, ?.e., actual incep-
tion of the visual image of an object. Three or four phases in this
process were demonstrated in numerous traditional studies. In the first
phase, the answers of the subjects were characterized as: no perception,
145
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
,
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
diffuse background, vague feeling of presence of a shape, etc. At the
secand phase; amorphous shape, presence of lines, some details (without
general, adequate recognition), simplified shape (as compared to the one _
presented), false hypotheses, general form (without details), addition
to what was perceived, etc. At the third phase: recognition, certain
perception of shape, clear gestalt, optimum perception of shape, identi-
fication, interpretation, etc.
In all these instances, the investigators delibetately made recognition
difficult by reducing contrast, increasing distance and eccentricity
of the position of the object in the field of vision, etc. Zn one of
the studies of microgenesis of perception conducted in the context of
microstructural analysis, not only qualitative, but quantitative charac-
teristics o� the process were obtained. B. M. Velichkovskiy succeeded
in reconstructing the timing of microgenesis of object perception in
' sequential and even metric form. He considered three classes of percep-
tual problems. The f irst included processes of localization of an object
in three-dimensional space, as well as estimation of its dimensions.
About 50 ms are spent on solving such problems. Solving problems of
the second class 3.s related to the possibility of estimating the
sequence of events in time, which requires about 100 ms with intramodal
and intermodal combinations of stimuli. This class includes processes of
perception of brightness and parametexs of ob~ect movement. These types
of perception are invariant taith respect to spatial position, and
visible brightness is so also with respect to time of presentation.
Finally, the third class of perceptual problems includes processes of
perception of the shape of ob~ects. For 1Q0-150 ms from the time the
sitmulus is delivered, the ob~ect is shapeless and a rather labile
structt~re in percention; 200-300 ms are needed farthe shape to be per-
ceived as an unchanging whole, that retains the mutual location of its -
parts during diverse movements, inclinations, turns of objects in
space. Time of perception of a rigid form depends on velocity of movement
and copnplexity of shape, which is approximately proportionate to the
number of elements in shape and randomness of their arrangement [11, 14].
' In the most recent works of this authory it was demonstrated that two
independent stages can be clearly distinguished within processes of per-
ception of figurative features of objects: at the first, more rapid stage,
there is evaluation of general outlines, izr particular, orientation of
the object in space; at the second one, evaluation of specification of
internal details of the ob~ect. Involvement of focal attention is
required for campletion of the second stage. Thus, visual perception -
advances from localization of quasi-ob~ect areas in space and time to
subsequent description of the general features of these areas and,
finally, to distinct perception of the object with all the diversity of
its details.
1.46
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
All this enables us to refer to different levels of construction of the
image of an object. The process of microgenesis is the successive
climb from level to level regulated by a perceptual or any other problem,
as we11 as the time and energy conditions of stimulation [14]. As this
ascent progresses, more and more new systems of functional units,
operations and perceptual actions are involved in perception. These
data justify our returning to the problem of perceptual and verbal
meanings and categories discussed above. With visual perception, the
minimal lag in verbal categorization is 250-300 ms. In this time,
perceptual categorization of data on localization in three-dimensional
space, parameters of movement and shape of the object is concluded. It
is easy to see that, with integral perception of objects on this time
scale, verbalization of all the extracted perceptual information is
~ impossible. It must also be borne in mind that each of the perceptual
categories has its own metrics. Apparently, our short-term memory sets
the limit of verbal categorization. If it does occur, it is only with
- regard to the most recently (on the microgenetic scale) isolated per- _
ceptual category. Of course, with the appropriate set, the observer can _
render any of the above categories the object of purposeful perceptual
action. Its result will be verbal categorization. The presence of the -
others can also be fixed in verbal form, but the accuracy of their abso-
lute evaluation will be substantially lower than the category that was -
the object of special, expanded perceptual action.
There are data indicative of the fact that the sequence of phas~s imple-
menting the microgenesis of perception may be quite labile. Depending on
the individual's problems and sets, microgenesis may not undergo all of
the stages and end at any of them. Depending on the same circumstances
and properties of stimulation, some of the stages may not participate
in the perception process. For example, on the basis of studies of
microgenesis, the hypothesis was expounded that aconstant perception is
normal perceptions in the microstructure of which certain narrow-level
operations of evaluation of ob~ect position in three-dimensional space
have been "wrapped up" [ar reducedJ. The type of perception that is
generally called impressionistic is also attributable to incomplete
microgenesis. This mode of vision holds a much larger place in both
everyday life and occupational activity than attentive, thorough examina-
tion. We often look on a broad field of vision, "without allowing"
microgenesis to terminate with distinct perception of an individual object.
The adaptational meaning of this mode of perception is that the perceptual
systems are open for reception of expected or urgent information. Studies
of microgenesis of perception are presently being conducted on a rather
wide scale. On their basis, it is possible to optimize processes of
control of various transport systems, when man deals not only with
displayed information, but must be guided in real space among really
moving ubjects.
Researchers arrive at the conclusion that the fullness of microgenesis is
determined by a perceptual or practical, etc., problem. For example, in
147
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USIs OIVL~
ttie course of formation of ~he image of an object in order to then recog-
nize or remember it there will be distinction of various features. But
if It is necessary to make a dec:ision concerning the pur~~osefulness of
some action or other, the isolated signs may be quite different from
Chose used for mnemic problems. It is expressly for decision making that
it is necessary to form an integral, ob~ecitve, constant and categorial
image of the ob~ect or situation. Eut this image, which is necessary for
decision making, has considerable limit.ations with respect to regulation
of forthcoming action. It has to be transformed and altered in the
interE~sts of the action. This change proceeds in the dirPction of its
decomposition and disintegration, distinction of different perceptual
categories iz it, such as space, motion, real (and not constant) size,
shape, etc. tind each of these categories must have an adequate reflection
in motor programs. It is quite likely that the microgenesis of perceptual
categories observed in the course of image formation, its composition,
differs from the order of isolation of perceptual,categories involved
in construction of action. Nor is reverse microgenesis, or reverse
evolution of the integrity, in the course of decomposition of the image -
and tor,nation of motor programs.
Consideration of this real difficulty requires rejection of a simple
linear chain: perception, decision, action, verification [control] . In
broader structures of acttvity that include these components, it is
difficult to unequivocally pinpoint a given component. New experimental ~
and conceptual means of analysis are needed to describe them.
Microstructural analysis of cognitive processes: In order to render more
graphic the problem � of studying congitive activity by means of micro--
structural analysis, let us start with a description of a real case,
which one of us witnessed. A grand master involved in psychological
tests was shown a complex chess position to remember for 0.5 s. The
- chess player refused to reproduce the position, stating that he could not
remember anything, but he added that the position of the white men was
weaker. In this example, we are amazed by the fact that the subject =
extracted the meaning of the situati~n and made an integral (most often
flawless) evaluation of this situation without comprehensive, detailed -
perception, let alone remembering the elements af the complex situation.
Such brief, productive mental processes, which create the impression _
in self-observation of absolute ingenuousness, have long since attracted
the attention of scientists. They were named "unconscious conclusions," -
"contemplation of essentials," "pure datum" ["dannost etc. At the
present time, interest in such phenomena is significantly stimulated
by the engineering psychology aspect of studies of processes of receiving
and processing information, and particularly studies of preparation
of information and decision making. Demonstration of the structure of
short-term processes will help in better planning of external means of
operator activity, in particular, information models, as well as in more
purposeful formation of internal means of activity.
148
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Microstructural analysis of cognitive and executory activity consists of
the study of short-term perceptual mnemic and thinking processes. With
the method of microstructural analysis, the latter can viewed as mor-
phological objects with a fully developed f~inctional ,c,.ucture, specific
objective [ob3ect-related] content and semantic load.
- Since microstructural analysis is intended for the desc.ription of the
structure of cognitive and executory actions, its most important tasks
are to isolate components (units of analysis) that have retained the
properties of a whole, and to determine the types of correlations or
coordinations that develop between them. The set (alphabet) of such -
components raust be broad enough to cover the process as a whole; in addi-
tion, each of these components must have not only qualitative, . but
quantit3tive certainty. Microstructural analysis works with the con-
- cepts of operations and functional unit. The latter are rather elementary
units of conversion of input information. Each _`unctional unit diff ers
from another in a numbeY of parameters, the most important of which.
are: place in the structure of the operation or action, information
capacity, information storage (conversion) time, form of representation
in it of some objective content, tqpe of transformation of information
and possible connections with other functional units.
The method of studying microstructures is based on isolation, analysis and
quantitative evaluation of factors that affect action performance time
under various experim~ntal conditions. These factors have the charac-
teristics of external and internal means of activity, which are related
to the distinctions and objective content of test material, prior experi-
ence in cognitive or practical action. The following is the most popular
- procedure of microstructural analysis. The time from the start of presenta-
tion of test material is divided into a series of intervals, and it is
assumed that some conversions or other of input inforntation, made by
means of certain functional units or a series of units, occur within each
such interval. This preliminary mode' is submitted to experimental
analysis, even in the cas~ of using the same test material (there are _
provisions for varying the condition~ of its presentation, types of instruc-
tions and subjects' responses). Then, on the basis of analysis of the
results, a more refined model is constructed, which consists of func-
- tional units, each of which performs one (sometimes more) function of
storage, retrieval, conversion of presented information. In turn, this
hypothetical model is then submitted to comprehensive experimental verifi-
cation, etc. Of course, in such a study, individual functional units
cannot be the immediate object of investigation. The object is the _
integral action of the individual. However, variation of the problems,
test material, amount thereof, speed of presentation, type of response
actions, etc., which are based on modern methods of planning experiments,
permit isolation in this action rf individual operations and functional
units. Microstructural analysis is a variant of analysis of levels.
Accordi~gly, its most important task is to determine the structure of
149
FOR OI~'FICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
converted forms of external objective activity, which occur interna~~y
and in the new elements appearing in internal activity. Numerous
studies conducted along the lines of microstructural analysis can be
conceived of as a certain prototype, true not yet refined enough, of
the planning of different functions of operator activity.
AC present, there are many models of processes of receiving and process-
ing information, which are not infrequently called models of short-term
visual and auditory memroy. This is related to a persistent misunderstandir.g,
which consists of the fact that the methods of microstructural analysis
presumably are applicable only to the study of short-term memory. In
fact, however, although they did originally appear in studies of short-term
memory, they then were used for the study of virtually all cognitive
processes, and recently also began to be used to study executory ones.
Figure 17 illustrates the block diagram of potentially possible types of
conversion of input information in the segment between the input of
the visual system to the verbal response. Drapending on the objectives
of observation and action, existence of sensory standards, operative
units of perception, hypotheses, sets and a number of other factors, the
perceived information can be submitted to v~arious transformation. In
other words, input information processing can be interrupted in any
unit, and the units themselves may be involved in processing, in a different
assortment an,d coordination. All these may serve as one of the grounds
for interpreting the diverse individual c.istinctions that characterize
human perception, memory and thinking.
Sensory memory: This unit is also called '~~,~1~o~Ly '"=gis*~r," "very short
visual memory, etc. The function of this unit consists of reflecting
and engraving 1[in memory] of the ob~ect with all of its features that.
are accessible to the perceiving system, i.e., within the range of its
resolution capacity. Information is stored in sensory memory t.,r a
short time, since it has to be free for reception of a new batch of
information when the visual system is operating in a dynamic mode
(constant change in fixation points), and the time thereof is of the
order of 100 ms.
- The spatial localizati~n of objects is recorded in sensory memory. If it -
changes, information for analysis is delivered to higher levels of process-
ing. Data about the volume and storage time for information in sensory
memory are based on experiments, in which subjects solved the problem of
identifying two successively presented arrays, consisting of randomly
arranged black and white cells. An array [matrix] containing 64 cells
was presented for 1 s, and after a variable interval it was followed by
a second one exposed until the sub~ect responded. The second array was
either identical to the first or different in that it contained one
black cell more or less. The answera were rapid and accurate when the
interval between arrays did not exceed 100 ms. There was substantial
decrease in accuracy of responses when the interval was longer [72].
150
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~ i I i~ I r' p , i~~~~~,;, r~
~~i ~ jill I ~ I ~ il ~ ~ II i I'~ ~ ~I j~li ~ ~ ~~I~lI~1~~U;~ q~jji~ll';~'~~
ji~ll ~ ,,II ~ ~ ~(,.I, ~
~ ~ I~ I~'ii ' ' ' ~ ~ ~ ' ,;;I i . , ~ , .
, ~ ~li j ~~.~~'Ij ~ I;, i~
~ ~i i I t~ ~ I' ~ II
sory I I em~ Scan- Recog tion~ ula ~ e
j an Rtepe ~~II
re ning r~iion i
f p tor tion
ess- I;~~
i!j,~; r+ I ~ ~ ~ ~~I!~ i ~ ~i i,,,,ii ~'~I
l~I~, i, i! ` ~ ~ i I ~ ''i 11 i; ~ i ; i I~1~,~ ~I~i~.~,
; ir':!,' II II ~ iI i!~i II' ~ ~ I~ ~ II ~ I(~ i I~ I, I' ~~Illljl I(!~i ~i.,~ll!~ii~ (~i I ~
~
~~~i;~ ( ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ I I j I~!ili''
~ I i,I!i~~~ p~
~~IIIIIij ,~II I 1 i I~'' ~ III~~ I~I~ ~I
~ I~~;~ i.
~~II I~I;III~ .','ij~l I ~ t 4 [ I jll i Y I. III ,
I I'~~I~~~ 1i I I~ ~ 1 ~~'1
+ I I I I' I'';~.~~~ ~ ~ 1 S ly l; I' i I~ I j I ~
~ II ~ ~ I ~ I~ I' ~ I I I i ' I I I ~ I I ~ ~ ~
yI~ II I' I I 1 I ' ll I~ I ~ I~ I I I I I~I~I I II I 1 I I II I I~I I I I I I I
~I~ 'II~~.II I'I~ 'll ~ ~ ~I I' ~II~1~~~~ I ~ , I~III ~I~I'I I
~ ~ ~ I I , ~ ~ , i I I I I~; i
i I
,,,i,,~, ~ i ~I ~ ~ ? II ,
- I I G r a
p
h i c- I(~ ~ ~ i I, I I I~ I ~ I
~ I ~ ~ ~~I ~
! I con c e p t u a l l i ~ I',' I I
I! ~ i~ ~ i ~ ~ I I memor model ~ ( ' I ~i . ~ ~
~ I
i I I ~ I~ ~ i
, I
I~ I I;!'~~ I ' I' ~I ~ ~ ' ~
Ii4~ ~ ~ '
~ ~I
Figure 17. Functional block diagram of conversion of input informa-
tion in short-term memory. Information is inputted in
the graphic-conceptual model from different functional
units of short-term memory (shown by Arabic numerals)
and long-term memory
a) conversions that are possible during one visual fixation ["a" and "b"
not shown in figure]
b) formation of immediate [operational] graphic-conceptual model of
situation by means of information retrieval steps (I-IV)
PMI) program of motor instructions
We must call attention to the fact that the identification procedure,
which takes place on the level of the sensory register, appears to occur
on its own and does not require deliberate remembering of the control
image, or detailed comparison thereof to the test image. The use of the
mechanism at the basis of the sensory register permits substantial in-
crease in labor productivity of specialists engaged in identification of
different patterns (x-rays, aerial photographs, microcircuits, etc.).
Because of its enormous capacity, sensory memory serves the function of
preafferentation and monitoring of changes occurring in the environment.
Changes recorded in sensory memory are the cause for including other
levels of information F~cessing, which are responsible for detection,
retrieval, recognition, as well as other forms of processing arrays of
"raw" sensory information.
151
_ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Iconic memory: While sensory memory stores all presented information,
regardless of whether it is organized or r.ot, in iconic memory there is
conversion and storage of objective information in the form of sensory
and perceptual standards, which can subsequently be categorized percep-
~ tually or verbally. The volume of information stored in iconic memory
is very large, and overtly larger than the volume that can be reproduced
or used to reguiate behavior and activity. This surplus implies the
selectivity of subsequent stages of perception and memory. According to
existing estimates, up to 12 symbols are stored in iconic memory in 800-
1000 ms [76]. Relatively longer storage of information in iconic memory
is functionally important. Its first function is to preserve the visual
"original," by means of which it is possible to monitor adequacy of
transformations made in other functional units. The second function
is thar lengthy storage provides for an association between previously
fixed traces and s~bsequ,~nt ones. Special studies [16, 33] demonstrated
that 2-3 fixed traces (within 1 s) are acc~ssible to analysis. Thus,
there are both dynamic (conversions) and conservative (retention)
components in iconic memory.
Scanning: Information stored in iconic memory is submitted to further
processing. Here, a scanning mechanism plays an important role. Scan-
ning the content of iconic memory takes place at a constant rate of 10 ms
per symbol. According to experimental data, an observer can searc h for
specified symbol in a changing information field at the rate of 120 symbols
per second [27, 77]. It should be noted, however, that this mode of per-
ception is a distinctive variant of blindness to the world, when man
perceives only what he expects. The scanning mechanism is an effective
means of overcoming superfluous and excessive information fixed in
iconic memory. It experiences the influence of higher levels of informa-
tion processing, which give it the retrieval standards and dire~.t~ia~n~
of scanning. There is discussion in the literat~e of a hypothesis that
replaces the scanning mechanism with a filtering mechanism. In this
case, the retrieval standards must shift to the level of sensory memory.
Buffer memory of recognition: The name of this unit indi.cates that it
is the place of encounter of information from the outside world and
from long-term memory. The recognition unit is a certain part of the
contents of long-term memory brought to the input in the form of percep-
tual hypotheses, standards, operative units of perception and memory. The
number of such hypotheses may vary. If it is small, the operative units
of perception can shift even to the levels of iconic and sensory memory,
being submitted to reverse transformation into the language of these units.
It is quite difficult to estimate the number of hypotheses stored irr the
recognition unit. The number of surnames that are being sought in a
text by professionals, according to the address classification of informa-
tion, may exceed 100. For alphabetic information it does not exceed
10-12. If there are more letters to be searched, the reaction time
begins to increase. For pictorial information, the number of perceptual
152
. FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
hypotheses is apparently enormous, but it has not been precisely deter-
~ mined whether they are stored in the recognition buffer or long-term
memory. It is important that the pictorial perceptual standards are
very accessible. In the recognition unit there is distinction of informa-
tive features as related to expounded perceptual hyptoheses and comparison
of incoming information to the actualized standards, images.
Formation of programs of motor instructions: Information that has been
evaluated as useful must be changed in the recognition unit to a form that
is suitable for its use. As we have already mentioned, it may be assimi-
lated by the system of sensory or perceptual standards contained in the
recognition [identification?] unit. Then the incoming information must
be translated or related to certain motor programs. Thi~ is necessary
for it to be possible to exteriorize the infornaation, either in the
form of verbal reports, or in the form of some other response actions.
In this case, we should not be dealing with traces, standards or even
images, but with efferent readiness, operative units of perception,
sensorimotor schemes, efferent copies, programs of examination or
execution.
It must be stated that studies of short-term memory have not yet offered
strong arguments for separating the recognition unit and unit of
formation of motor instruction programs. Some authors related transforma-
tion of information delivered by the scanning mechanism to the motor
instruction program to functions of buffer recognition memory. The
function of the repetition unit actually consists of running one of the
possible programs that are formed in the recognition unit. The
rate of the scanning block and recognition block, including formation of
programs of motor instructions, is estimated at the same figure, 10-15 ms
- per symbol, but it is not indicated whether the operating time of the
- recognition unit is additional or whether it coincides with the operation
of the scanning unit. In any case, it is important to note that the
recognition unit operating time is longer by more than a factor of 10
than the operating time of the repetition unit (15 ms to form a program
of motor instructions in the recognition unit and 300-500 ms to run this
program). The maximum speed of repetition unit operation is estimated
at 6 letters/second, although in memory experiments a rate of about
3 letters/second is more common. Evidently, the estimates of time of
formation of programs of motor instructions are exceedingly exaggerated.
We could agree with such estimates if we concede the possibility of
existe~e of two types of motor instruction programs: potential and real.
The former programs could be created at a rate that is clo~e to the
one assumed by G. Sperling, i.e., 10-15 ms/symbol. Real programs must
have considerably greater details and, accordingly, the rate of their
formation must be substantially lower. If we were to digress from real
programs of motor instructions and accept the estimates of time of
creation of potential programs of motor instructions, the question
arises as to why such a reserve of stability is needed in the function
153
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
of the first units, as compared to the repetition unit. It may be assumed
that there are situations that justify the enormous speed of operation of
units that are close to the input of the visual system.
Evidently, such situations are closer to real conditions of man's activity,
when it is not so much complete reproduction of presented material that
is required of them as recognition of the material, evaluation of degree
of usefulness and selection of a small part of information that is rele-
vant to the objectives of activity. It is logical to believe that, in
such situations, it is not just any recognition that leads to formation
of real programs of motor instructions for repetition (or execution) units.
- This is particularly apparent when we analyze information retrieval, in
which there is something like "negative recognition," when the observer
evaluates the information as useless and for this reason does not form a
real program. As shown by numerous studies, the number of stored programs
may be quite large, although their storage time is limited. As a rule,
in situations of real activity, only part of the formed programs of
motor instructions is realized. At the same time, it would hardly be
correct to conclude that info~?ation that did not reach the repetition
unit is lost and not used at all in behavior. The question arises: what
positive function could these potential, superfluous and unrealized
programs of motor instructions in the repetition unit perform? That
these programs can indeed serve for certain positive functions can be
seen from the so-called "rapid reading," in which a large part of the
text bypasses the repetition unit.
Consequently, there may also be other units with two functions in the
hierarchic system of conversion of input information, between the scanning
and recognition units, on the one hand, and the repetition unit, on the
other. In the first place, the speed of their operation should be
commensurate to that of the recognition unit. In the second place,
potential, still unverbalized programs of motor instructions must be the
_ object of transformation. This brings us right up to productive functions
of the described system of information processing.
Manipulator unit: We described above the characteristics of manipulative
capacity of the visual system. In recent years, several studies were made
of this capacity within the context of microstructural analysis of cogni-
tive processes [8, 9, 16, 74]. Most demonstrative are experiments that
were conducted by the method of determination of a.missing element. In
essence, this technique consists of the following. Before presenting a
sequence of numbers in the same place in the field of vision, the sub-
ject is informed by means of digital instructions of the size of the
alphabet (i.e., the size of the segment of the natural series of
numbers, out of which a sequence will be chosen). After thi.s, the subject
ia shown a series of digits, the length of which is shorter by one unit
than the alphabet. The subject has to find the missing digit. The
numbers were displayed for 50 ms with 50-ms intervals between stimuli. The
154
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
results obtained indicate that the subjects succee~ in solving the problem,
even in the case of short intervals and series 9 digits in length. With
such exposure time and interval, there is obviously not enough time to
pronounce the presented numbers. Consequently, the subjects worked
with nonverbalized potential programs of motor instructions. Formation
of such programs in the described experimental situation was superfluous,
since the subjects new in advance the alphabet of digits that would be
presented to them. The task for the subjects was to "strike out"
potential and superfluous programs. However, since the numbers were
given in random order, this could not be done automatically as they were
presented. These programs have to be stored and submitted to certain
manipulations directed toward putting the random series in order. An
important distinction of the manipulator unit is that ~nformation can
be delivered to it successively and r_onsidered after the start of
conversions that have already been t~:ade with the ir~formation contained
in it. This provides for continuitr of consideration of successively
perceived information.
There are also data indicative of transformation of images of geometric
- forms, which occurs in the manipulator unit by means of operations
(mental) of shift, turn and turning of images. The function of the mani-
pulator unit is important to reinterpretation of visual stimulation, to
anticipate the new position of an object in space and possible change in
its form. Transformation of sensorimotor schemes, graphic images and
_ more complex forms of cognitive representation, including symbolic, is
possible in the manipulator unit. In other words, it makes a contribu-
tion to restructuring of the image of the situation, to putting it in a
form that is suitable for decision making (33].
Unit for semantic processing of information: When discussing the possible
transformations of information that take place on the route from forming
a trace in iconoic memory to its reproduction, the question arises: is
it possible for some operative units to be transformed into others: Can
such transformations (like manipalations with motor instruction programs)
take place before information reaches the repetition unit? To answer
this question, a comparative experiment was conducted with two groups of
subjects: an experimental group, consisting of experienced operator-
programmers proficient in binary and octal calculation systems, and a
control group consisting of sub,jects who were not familiar with these
systems. The sub~ects were shown 19 binary digits for a short time (80 to
1000 ms). The display time was such that the obtained information could
not be processed in the repetition unit. Nevertheless, the subjects
" skilled in recoding correctly reproduced all of the presented material
in most cases. The same results were obtained with artist subjects, on
whom a different method of perceptual grouping of information was used.
They perceived zeros as the background, ones as figures, and this
reduced significantly the number of objects to remember. These results
serve as grounds to add another functional unit, namFly the unit of
semantic processing of unverbalized information.
155
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The submi tted results warrant the conclusion that, with a high enough degree
of traini ng, base information can go directly to the unit of inter-
pretive p rocessing, bypassing auditory memory. Only rather important
information is transferred to the unit of repetition and, accordingly,
auditory memory, rather than the initial sensory data. Overt or
discre[e pronunciation is the chief ineans of storing information in
short-term memory and transferring it into long-term memory. Information
can be s t ored for an unlimited time in long-term memory, apparently in the
form of an abstract chart of logical statements, a sort of conceptual
reservoir.
Such organization of correlations between visual and auditory short-term
memory is a11 the more rational, since the visual system is indeed
unique from the standpoint of ~:nstant grasp of a complex situation and
capabili t ies of analog transformation of the primary reflection of reality.
The descr ibed system of information processing performs not only repro-
ductive, but productive functions, including those that form meaning. The
fact of the matter is that short-term memory does not function only
~s a dev i ce for receiving information, but it is the place of encounter
of flows of information from the outside world and long-term memory. The
individua 1 always has his own system of previously formed operative
units, wh ich is involved in receipt of information and implements the
second as pect of the comparison process, namely comparison of the object -
to the subject [individual].
The presence of productive units in the information processing system is
indicative of the existence of one more form of comparison, i.e., compari-
_ son of information to the goals of solving practical and thinking problems.
To concl ude the description of microstructures of initial levels of cogni-
tive act ivity, we should briefly discuss the general distinctions of
the descr ibed system of information processing. As indicated above,
each of the units in these scheme first consisted of a certain theoretical
construc tion, or model. Then the experimental conditions were created,
under which a given unit could be demonstrated in the purest form, i.e.,
isolated from the influence of other units. Of course, this was not
always successful. It can only be stated with confidence that, in
experimental situations, the unit under study performed a dominant function.
On the basis of the presently available results, the list of cognitive
operation s and units could be significantly expanded. There are also
other variants of representation of the system of functional units,
which are related to the theoretical ~.nd practical problems that are
being so lved by the researcher. The clescribed system is intended for
comprehension and details of processerj of formation of a graphic-
conceptual model under the natural conditions of operator activity, i.e.,
it is in tended for description and interpretation of a living process of
receipt and processing of information, rather than only its artificial
laboratory analogs.
156
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
- ~OR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~everal in?portant conc7usions ensue from these theses. The system of
information receipt and processing is polystructured and heterarchic.
In the course c.~f its function, there may be involvement of different
combinations, rather than all units. The general rule is that the units
do not have their own, strictly fixed place and, consequently, the time
characteristics of their function may vary. Regardless of the number of
units that constitute a real process, the system is an organized entity,
i.e., it is characterized by a specific arrangement of its elements and
specific types of coordination of their interactions. Organization of
the information processing system is highly dynamic, and its dynamics
are determined both by the movement of information and relations to the
environment. In the described system, the productive units are the
least fixed: manipulator unit and semantic processing unit. In a number
of situations they "shift" virtu~lly to the input of the visual system,
when retrieval of the meaning of a situation appears to precede its
perce~tion. At present, hypotheses are being expounded and f:Lnding some
confirmation concerning the existence of precategorial'selection,
quasisemantic transformations that are made on the levels of iconic
memory and even sensory register. _
At present, investigators of short-term memory are looking for new con-
ceptual schemes to describe it. Unit models of inemory are being replaced
with multidimensional spatial models. Experimental and theoretical
- research is overcoming the popular chronological and hierarchic models,
and raising the question of designing models.that adequately describe
the effects of simultaneous processing of sensory and semantic informa-
tion. Explanation of such effects requires reference to psychological
and psycholinguistic studies of values and meaning on the figurative and
verbal levels [51, 67]. Such studies are indicative of the similarity
(and even identity) of semantic structures of figurative and verbal
representation of phenomena on levels of deep semantics. In other
words, gradually the rift between sensory and perceptual standards,
mnemic schemes, nonverbalized programs of motor instructions and
significance is being overcome, i.e., that which appeared to be a
lower, presemantic level may very well be next to the cons dous level of
verbal information processing or even superior to it in a number of para-
meters and, first of all, productivity. Ergonomics and engineering psy-
chology cannot fail to pay attention to these studies of cognitive
activity, since optimization of figurative, sign-related and symbolic con-
ception of information on display devices constitutes a substantial
reserve for improving the efficiency of operator performance in man-
machine systems.
Thus, microstructural analysis of cognitive processes is departing more
- and more from the initial simplified conceptions inherent in the
informational-cybernetic approach. Considerably more attention is being
given to psychological features of operations and functional units; the
postulate of simple sequence of performance of elementary operations
157
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
has been overcome. The data from microstructural analysis are being -
used well for interpretation of processes of prepara tion of information
and decision making. Of course, it would be naive to assume that
complex thinking activity could be made up of functional units. At the
same time, the existing results of microstructural analysis are indica-
tive of the inadequacy of many conceptions of thinking, which appeared �
without consideration of the real complexity of transformations, includ-
ing semantic ones, that occur on the levels of perception, memory,
perceptual-motor schemes, etc.
4. Preparation of Information for Decision Making
The urgency of studying processes of preparing information and ~ecision
making is related to the most important distinctions of man-rmachtne
systems (MMS). These systems must be able to solve creative prob.lems that
occur in the course of practical behavior. The practical behavior of the
system or its function occurs under conditions of a large uumber of
dynamic and interrelated factors, ~cahich together create much vagueness
in the choice of optimum action. As a rule, an MMS operates on a real
time scale, and always with a shortage of time. Finally, the MMS
functions under conditions of a changing external situation and presence
of competing, conflicting factors (which renders it, in essence, a game
system). For this reason, it must be capable of taking into consideration
the changes occurring in the external situation, de termining the laws of
occurrence of these changes in order to foresee them and adjust to them
in advance or to counter them. An MMS, considered as a complex organism,
must develop a model of these conditions or, in other words, a model of
the external situation and of its own state. Since the external situa-
tion and the systemts state change all the time, the system must
continuously construct, alter and refine the models it creates. But,
since it is possible to construct virtually an infinite number of models
of the same situation, the control system must cons truct models that are
consistent with the problems confronting it at a given time, i.e., it
must convert the information to a form that is convenient for decision
making and performance of executory actions. The d ecision that is made ~
must take into consideration the sta~~a of variable and conflicting factors; _
a plan of behavior must be prepared for the imm~diate and more distant
point in time. Decision making in the presence of uncertainty and con-
flict, which arise in MMS operation, is the preroga tive of the human
operator. Operators who make decisions in these si tuations are operator-
investigators and operator-administrators, who work in the mode of
operational thinking. The result of operational thinking or decision
making in the MMS is the construction of the image of a new situation
and of sequence of actions with controlled objects, by means of which the
existing situation can be changed to the desired (including that dictated
by the conditions) state. Operational thinking is closely related to
practical thinking, the typical features of which were singled out by
158
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
B. M. Teplov [57]: the decision must be positive and the b~st under
specific prevailing conditions (negative results are also theoretically -
valuable); the decision must be specific (on the basis of analysis of
complex material, with mandatory distinction of what is important, one
has to synthesize a decision that provides simple and definite theses);
the decision must be strictly limited as to time.
In descriptions of operative thinking anddecision making, much attention
is given to ir,tuition,i.e., the ability to rapidly discern a complex
situation and almost instantaneously find the right decision. Intuition,
or insight, is referable to the final stage of the thinking process,
to appearance of the idea of the decision. The preceding stages are
given much less attention, and this is reflected in the paucity of
psychological interpretations of intuition phenomena. In spite of this,
we can mention some of the features of intuitive decision and, although
they were obtained by self-observation, they are apparently objective
in nature, since there has been repeated and independent mention of them.
- These signs are: sense of complete confidence in accuracy of the result -
and clarity of what has to be done next [66, p 127]; sense of order,
of the "required form" of the result, which is sometimes not reached
immediately~, but having been reached generates a f eeling of confidence
[65, p 150]; automation of actions after insight, performance of
technical operations without ref lection, with complete confidence that
the desired result will be obtained [65, p 193].
Such features also characterize ~he result-producing part of operational
thinking. However, the interesting feature of the final part of the
decision making act can be defined only on the basis of comprehension of _
its pr~paratory stages, which have not been thoroughly studied by far.
Information preparatior~ for a decision refers to the aggregate of actions
and operatio~s pertaining tn receipt and processing of information about
the external environment, state of the control system, progress of the -
controlled process, as well as ancillary and business [o~ service]
information. In the course of these acrions and operations, which include -
processes of information retrieval, detection, identification, recognition, _
recoding and transformation of information, presented on displays, the
~ operator constructs a graphic-conceptual model (GCM) of the situation.
If this stage of operator activity were to be compared to the numerous
descriptions of the cr.eative process, it is closest to the stage of
inception af the topic.
This stage of activity is characterized by the fact that information is -
translated into the language of images, schemes, operative units of per-
ception, i.e., which the operator knows. Subsequent processing of
information is performed in this language, the language of the operator's
GCM. At the second stage, the operator analyzes and compares the situation
to the system of rating criteria and gages, which he has or which is
159
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
specially developed for a given concrete case, which determine the nature
and direc tion of changes i.n GCM of the situation. In des~riptions of
the creative process, this stage corresponds to the stage of perception
of the topic, analysis of the situation and recognition of the problem.
The main task at this stage is to transform the GCM into a model of
the problem situation that arose in connection with the choice of topic.
This new model, which is adequate for the objectively formed problem
- situation, is the area of crystallization of the problem that is to be
solved. The first and second 5tages constitute conscious work directed _
toward creation of the GCM and model of the problem situation, its _
skeleton, scheme, i.e., self-styled functional organs of the individual.
While vagueness or an excessive number of degrees of freedom in the
situation are fixed at the stage of formation of the GCM, at the stage
of formation of the problem situation there is recognition (and inter-
pretation) of the contradiction or conflict generating this uncertainty. _
As a result of this work, it often becomes possible to visualize the
mental "landscape," in which events must take place and intuitive
conception of their course.
At the third stage, there is intensive work to solve the problem. It
consists of operating with base and transformed data, and it takes
place in the form of purposeful actions, or else in the form of un-
conscious and automated operations, that are not always by far verbal.. _
On the basis of studies of operator activity with graphic information
models, it can be concluded that, at this stage, visual-spatial trans-
formations and manipulations with elements of the problem situation
or situation as a whole are the main factor. Attention is devoted
chiefly to determination of different correlations between elements
or complexes thereof that became contradictory and generated a conflict
situation. In the course of such activity, a fuller idea is gained
about the objective content of the situation, possible directions of
its development, with structuring of the significance
of contradictory elements, complexes and properties of the situation.
The result of this work may be generation of new images, creation of new
visual forms carrying a certain meaning and rendering the significance
structured and visible. This type of activity is most often called
visual thinking [64, 74]. At this stage, preparation of informa.tion
for a decision changes into the decision making process.
The fourth stage is decision making proper. It is most often described
as an instantaneous act of illumination, although it is preceded by
lengthy work. Its main aspect is described in terms of occurrence of
an idea, seeing the meaning and nature of previously detected contra-
diction or conflict. Nevertheless, the nature of '~illumination" is
still unclear, and it has yet to be investigated. Finally, the last
stage--implementation of the decision--is the stage of executory actions,
and it does not require any special explanations.
160
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Processes of preparing decisiontaaking information are not unbiased. They
are subject to the influence of so-called subjective factors, personal
meaning, which include motives, subjective goals, sets, volition, etc.
These infl~?ences affect the means of interpretation and tr~nsformation
of conditions, and ob~ective content of problems, accuracy of result ob-
tained and the style in which it is implemented. Personal meaning
elements affect processes of information preparation and decision making
much more than the simpler executory and cognitive actions. This is
attributable to the fact that rating criteria in complex situations, also -
characterized by a shortage of information about the environment, are
usually evolved by the subject of activity. And this process of developing,
putting them in one order and then in another, reorganization, is continu-
ous -in the.'__:..caurse of thinking activity. Expressly it leads to a change
in goals, development and formulation of new goals.
The significance of the role of unconscious elements in intormation pre-
paring processes and decision making proper, as in any creative field,
raises the problem of objective study thereof. Of course, the self-
observation method cannot furnish accurate enough data, although very
much can be extracted with it. This is also confirmed by the above
description, in which we summarized chiefly self-observation data.
In both psychological and applied engineering psychological research,
various experimental methods are sought and tested for analysis of
thinking activity and its stages, phases, components. This search has
not yet ended. The object of this research is so complex, that to study
it one must use the most diverse methods, including those that would
help differentiate between the isolated stages. At the present time,
more successful studies are being made of the stages related t~ preparation
-f irformation and implementation of decisions than the stage. of actual
decision making.
Efforts have failed to use the characteristics of oculomotor behavior
to predict time spent by an operator working in the mode of preparing
information for a decision. With this form of work, there is impairment
of regularity of saccadic movements (which is present in information re-
trieval problems [4J), and the time of visual fixation fluctuates over
a very wide range, from 200 ms to many seconds. The reason for this is
that other actions begin to participate in this form of activity, and there
is also a change in composition of operations. For this reason, before
formulating metric problems, one must demonstrate the composition of
ac[ions involved in decision-related preparation of information and, in
particular, formation of the GCM and model of a problem situation.
Analysis of the microstructure of transformations of information led to
the belief that information can be delivered to the GCM from different
functional units, both in terms of primary reflection of reality and in
terms of secondary or Nth reflection (figure 17). The same situation can _
161
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
be successively (or simultaneously) reflected by various operative units
- of per~eption and memory in the GCM. In other words, the GCM is a multi-
dLmensional reflection of reality, a reflection that is described in
different perceptual, symbolic and verbal languages.* Accordingly,
interpreted information extracted from the situation, rather than
visually given base information, may be transfarred into the functional
unit for verbal recoding.
lst phase 2d phase 3d phase 4th phase
~ ~
1 ~
~ `
~ i
~
- I
~ = ,i, -
~
~x ~
~ 'S-~ .
t t t t, t t t t ,t t t t, t t t t,
ana s,s o ~ construct'~n
aboutesituation properties and Cso~Cti n of new so~u-
correlat~~on~ be- var~an~s tion variants
lst phase t een s ua ion ~ frequ~n~Y of
2d hase e~~ents transitibn to final
3d phase ~ tra~ition to solution phase of solution _
4th phase 0 phases ,
Figure 18. Frequency of transition between different phases of
problem solving of total numher of transfers~ ;
- unlined section--frequency of transfers to decisicin
, phases; li*~ed sections--frequency of transf ers to
final stage of decision making _
On the basis of microstructural analysis of various conversions of informa-
tion in the visual and auditory systems, one could conclude that perceptual,
cogritive and mnemic actions are involved, not only in information prepa-
ration of a thinking act, but in making a substantial contribution to
implementation of the latter. In the course of solving problems, a -
rather broad range of changes in information can occur, from scanning to
- nonverbal semantic transformations, at one step of information retrieval
n
*At Ehe same time, in our opinion, the hypotheses of several psycho-
linguists of existemce of deep semantic structures that are invariant
in relation to all these lan~uages [67] are highly plausible.
162
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
(i.e., in the ti~re that equals the duration of one visual fixation). -
Depending on the difficulty of the problem solved, the number and type of
transformation change, and this is manifested, in particular, by duration
of visual fixation. This means that a man solving the problem has the
ability to ad~ust ["tune in"] to the perceptual or semantic complexity
of the information field. This capacity is similar, to some extent, to
adjustment of the visual system to intensity of light flux. While the
latter is manifested by pupillary reactions, adjustment for complexity
is manifested by the duration of visual fixations and amount of informa-
tion processed.
This is confirmed by studies of the rate of information processing in
the case of data list coding. Alphanumeric lists were displayed to ,
subjects in the same place in the field of vision. The problems
changed from series to series. In the test, determination was made of
- the fnterstimulus interval between presentations of the lists, with
~ which the ~iib~ects gave at least 90% correct answers. A rather wide
range of change in rate of information processing was found, with '.:h~
same amount and mode of presentation thereof, as well as in different
problems solved by the o p~ator. This rate varies from 1 to 100 symbols
per second. Maximum speed was obtained in problems involving detection
of a particular s;:abol using a well-learned system of coding information.
The minimum speed was obtained when the means of semantic information
processing required of the operators were excessively complicated.[27].
As a rule, during actual operator work, the rate of information process-
ing is not constant. This is related to the fact that the operator
changes f,rom tne retrieval mode to the mode of construction of the GCM
and solutiun mode proper. As we have indicated above, operator work
takes place in stages, or phases. Phasic cognitive activity was
demonstrated in studies of processes of solving operational problem on
a power system mnemo nics simulator. In this case (unlike information
retrieval problems), the operators did not have any concrete or distinct
ider~tifying standards and rating criteria, and they had to form them
in the actual solving process, basing themselves by the previously
learned system of rules. In this study [40], there was polyeffector
recording of several functional systems involved in preparation of informa-
tion and decision making: EOG, EMG of the lower lip and EEG of the
occipital part of the brain.
The operators were asked to analyze the state of individual power units -
or the system as a whole. Upon detection of deviations from normal,
the subject had to make a decision as to the means of restoring the
normal state. Parallel recordings were made of functional parameters of
several physiological systems. It was found that, accordi.ng to the
electrooculographic data, one could distinguish four phases of oculo-
motor behavior differing in amplitude of saccades and duration of fixations.
In the first phase, there were high amplitude saccades and in the second,
low amplitude. In the first two phases, the fixation time was relatively
163
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONI,Y
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
short, in the range of 0.3-1.0 s. Then comes the third phase of long -
fixations (up to S s), alternating with high-amplitude saccades, and,
finally, the fourth phase characterized by absence of macromovements of
the eyes. This last phase could last tens of seconds. Depression
of a-rl~~~ti~m was Che least marked in the first and third phases (to 40%
of the background). Ma~;imum depression of a-rhythm was observed in the
f.olirth phase of the problem-solving process~(80% of background). Accord-
ing to findings on electromyograms of the lower lip, the art~iculation
system was involved at the final stages of problem solving. When
solving the most complex problems, alternate involvement of all three
recorded systems was observed; however, in this case, the share of the
~ articulation system in the solving process remained small. The data
obtaina~d from studies of problem solving of this type do not offer
g~c~unds for singling out a special phase of operating with a verbal
ref`lection of the problem situation.
Psychologically, the demonstrated phases could be interpreted as follows. `
At the first two phases mentioned, the subject becomes acquainted with
elements of the situation and analyzes the properties and relations of
elements. In other words, these phases are responsible for creation of
the GCM and model of the problem situation. When solving relatively
simple problems, one observes a transfer to the third phase, which can
be interpreted as the phase of recognition of the situation, which is
directed toward forming and assessing the suitability of the program of
actions. The latter is constructed on the basis of several rules and
modes of activity as~5imilated in the course of training. At this phase,
a choice of variant is made out of a series of standard solution variants.
Finally, in the more difficult cases, when the fourth phase is recorded,
we are dealing with internal activity in the proper sense of the word.
This activity is related to construction of an utterly new variant of
solution on the basis of manipulation and transformation of the GCM.
Analysis of the correlations between the above-described four phases re-
vealed that the process of solving complex problems is recursive in
nature. There can be transfers from the first phase directly to the
fourth, returns from the fourth to the first or second, etc. Transitions ~
from the first to the third and fourth phases are the most probable. At
the same tj.me, there is maximum probability of transition from the third _
or fo~rth phase to the final stage, the stage of preparation of the
solution in the context of internal speech and formation of an answer.
Thus, the recording of parameters of function of different physiological _
systems provides the grounds for objective evaluation of the functional
structure of complex cognitive activity and characteristics of conver-
sions that an operator makes in a problem siCuation.
Studies of the functional structure of microstructure. of activity are
necessary to optimize existing variants of ir~formation models. The
164
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
information model should connect the operator to the control elements,
rather than be a barrier separating him from them. -
BIBLIOGRAPHY
1. Marx, K., and Engels, F. "Works," Vol 23.
- 2. Alyakrinskiy, B. S. "Time Scanning of Human Work Operations," in
10Problemy kosmicheakoy biologii" [Problems of Space Biology],
Moscow, Nauka, Vol 34, 1977.
3. Anan'yev, B. G. "PsycholQgy of Sensory Learning," Moscow, Izd. APN
~ [Academy of Pedagogic Sciences] RSFSR, 1960.
- 4. Berezkin, B. S., and Zinchenko, V. P. "Research on Information
~Re.trieval," in "Problemy inzhenernoy psikhologii" [Problems of
Engineering Psychology], Moscow, Nauka, 1967.
5. Bernshteyn, N. A. "Modern Biomechanics and Problems of Labor
Safety," GIGIYENA,BEZOPASNOST' I PATOLOGIYA TRUDA [Industrial Hygiene,
Safety Practices and Pathology], No 2, 1930.
6. Idem, "On the Construction of Movements," Moscow, Medgiz, 1947.
7. Idem, "Essays on Physiology of Movement and Physiology of Activity,"
Moscow, Meditsina, 1966.
8. Bespalov, B. I. "Studies of Visual Transformations of Geometric
Shapes," in "Ergonomika. Trudy VNIITE" [Ergonimics. Works of the
All-Union Scientific Research Institute of Aesthetic Styling in
Engineering], Moscow, Vyp 12, 1976�
9. Idem, "Microstructural Analysis of Sensorimotor Action," Ibid,
Vyp 16, 1978.
10. Buyeva, L. P. "Man: His Activities and Communication," Moscow,
Mysl', 197s.
11. Velichkovskiy, B. M. "Microgenetic Aspect of Research on Perception,"
"Psikhologicheskiye issledovan~ya" [Psychological Research], Moscow,
Izd-vo Moscow University, Vyp 6, 1976.
12. Idem, "Visual Memory and Models of Human Information Processing,"
VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII [Problems of Psychology], No 6, 1977.
13. Velichkovskiy, B. M.; Zinchenko, V. P.; and Luriya, A. R. "Psychology -
of Perception," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University, 1973.
165
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
- 14. Velichkovskiy, B. M., and Leonova, A. B. "Psychology of Set, and the
_ Microstructural Approach," in "Bessoznatel'noye" [The Unconscious,
Tbilisi, Metaniyereba, Vo~ 1, 1978.
15. Vvedenskiy, N. Ye. "Physiological Phenomena From the Biological
Point of View," in "Fiziologiya nervnoy systemy" [Physiology of the
Nervous System] by I. M. Sechenov, I. P. Pavlov and N. Ye.
Vvedenskiy, Moscow, Medgiz, Vol 1, 1952.
16. Vuchetich, G. G., and Zinchenko, V. P. "Scanning Successively Fixed
Traces in Short-Term Memory," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 1, 1970.
17. Gorbov, F. D., and Lebedev; V. I. "Psychoneurological Aspects of
Operator Work," Moscow, Meditsina, 1975.
18. Gordeyeva, N. D.; Grechina, A. P.; and Mnatsakanyan, S. A. "Stages
of Construction of Sensorimotor Image of Space," in "Ergonomika.
Trudy VNIITE," Moscow, Vyp 16, 1978.
19. Gordeyeva, N. D.; Devishvili, V. M.; and Zinchenko, V. P. "Micro-
structural Analysis of Executory Activity," Moscow, Izd. VNIITE, 1975.
20. Idem, "Functional Structure and Criteria of Instrumental Spatial
Actions," in "Problemy kosmicheskoy biologii," Moscow, Nauka, Vol 34,
1977.
21. Gordeyeva, N. D.; Zinchenko, V. P.; and Rebrik, S. B. "Formation of
Complex Spatial Actions," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 3, 1978.
22. Gordeyeva, N. D.; Ligachev, V. I.; and Sirotkina, Ye. B. "Comparative
Analysis of Formation of Spatial Action Under Stable and Dynamic
Conditions," in "Ergonomika. Trudy L~NIITE;'Moscow, Vyp 16, 1978.
23. Gordon, V. M. "Studies of External and Vicarious Perceptual Actions
in the Structure of Problem Solving," in "Psikhologicheskiye
- issledovaniya," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University, Vyp 6, 1976.
24. Idem, "Studies of Functional Distinctions of Processes of Recognition
of and Operating With Images," in "Ergonomika. Trudy VNIITE;'Moscow,
25. Gordon, V. M., and Zinchenko, V. P. "Structural and Functional
Analysis of Mental Activity," in "Sistemnyye issledovaniya. Yezhegodnik
1977" [Systems Research, 1977 Annual], Moscow, Nauka, 1978.
26. Goryainov, V. P.; Zinchenko, V. P.; and Lepskiy, V. Ye. "Designing
External and Internal Means of Activity," in "Ergonomika. Trudy
VNIITE," Moscow, Vyp 12, 1976.
166
~ FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
- FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
27. Grechishchev, A. S.; Zinchenko, ~1. P.; Tsyguro, T. V.; and Shlyagina,
Ye. I. "Influence of Semantic Difficulty of Problems on Rate of
Information Processing by an Operator," it~ "Ergonomika. Printsipy i
rekomendatsii10 [Ergonomica: Principles and Recomanendations], Moscow,
Izd. VNIITE, Vyp 6, 1974.
28. 2aporozhets, A. V. "Development of Voluntary Movements," Moscow, Izd.
APN RSFSR, 1960.
29. Idem, "Development of Perception ar~d Activity," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII,
No 1, 1967.
30. Zaporozhets, A. V.; Venger, L. A.; Zinchenko, V. P.; and Ruzskaya, A.G.
"Perce tion and Action," Moscow~ Prosveahcheniye, 1967.
~ ,*r~~
31. Zinchenko, V. P. "Perception and Action. Reports 1 and 2," DOKLADY
APN RSFSR (Reports of the RSFSR Academy of Pedagogic Sciences], No 2
and 5, 1961.
32. Idem, "Theoretical Problems of Psychological Per~~eption," in
"Inzhenernaya psikhologiya" [Engineering Psycholr,gyJ, Moscow, Izd-vo
Moscow University, 1964.
33. Idem, "Productive Perception," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOGII, No 6, 1971.
34. Idem, "Microstructural analysis of Perceptual Processes," in
"Psikhologicheskiye issledovaniya," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University,
Vyp 6, 1976.
35. Idem, "Set and Activity. Is a Paradigm Necessary?" in
"Bessoznatel'noye," Tbilisi, Metsniyereba, Vol 1, 1978.
36. Zinchenko, V. P., and Vergiles, N. Yu. "Formation of Visual Image,"
- Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University, 1969.
37. Zinchenko, V. P., and Gordon, V. M. "Methodological Problems of
Psychological Analysis of Activity," in "Sistemnyye issledovaniya.
Yezhegodn~c1975," Moscow, Nauka, 1976.
38. Zinchenko, V. P., and Mamardashvili, M. K. "The Problem of Ob3ective
Method in Psychology,: VOPROSY FILOSOFII [Problems of Philosophy],
No 7, 1977.
39. Zinchenko, V. P.; Munipov, V. M.; and Smolyan, G. L. "Ergonomic
Bases for Organization of Labor," Moscow, Ekonomika, 1974.
40. Zinchenko, V. P.; Munipov, V. M.; and Gordon, V. M. "Studies of
Visual Thinking," VOPROSY PSIKHOLOG~I, No 2, 1973.
167
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~ 41. Zinchenko, P. I. "Involuntary Retention," Moscow, Izd. ~APN RSFSR,
1961.
42. 'Linchenko, T. P.; Kireyeva, M. M.; Ryab inkina, L. I.; Buryy, G. V.;
and Ostromoukhov, M. Z. "Studies of Cha racteristics of Eye Move-
ment During Information Retrieval as Re 1 ated to the Problem of
Coding Visual Information," in "Ergonomika. Trudy VNIITE," Moscow,
Izd. VNIITE, Vyp 16, 1978.
43. Kvasha, Ya. B. "Statistical Studies of Mechanization of Lahor,"
Moscow Nauka, 1959.
44. Kozlovskaya, I. B. "Afferent Control of Voluntary Movements," Moscow,
Nauka, 1976.
45. Kots, Ya. M. "Organization of Voluntary Movements," Moscow, Nauka,
1975.
46. Kiylov, A. A. (editor) "Methodological Studies in Engineering
Psychology and Industrial Psychology," Leningrad, Izd-vo Leningrad
University, Pt 2, 1975.
47. Leont'yev, A. N. "Problems of Mental Development," Moscow, Izd-vo
Moscow University, 1972. '
48. Idem, "Activity. Consciousness. Personality," Moscow, Politizdat,
1975.
_ 49. Leshly, K. S. "The Brain and Intellect," Moscow--Leningrad,
Ogiz-Sotsekgiz, 1933.
50. Lindsey, P., and Norman, D. "Information Processing in Man,"
,Ioscow, Mir, 1974.
51. Petrenko, V. F. "Semantic Analysis of Sensory Images," in
"Vospriyatiye i deyatel~nost [Percep tion and Activity], Moscow,
Izd-vo Moscow University, 1976.
52. Rebrik, S. B.; Gordeyeva, N. D.; and Kr ichevets, A. N. "Types of
Relations Between Cognitive and Executory Components of Instrumental
Spatial Action," in "Ergonomika. Trudy VNIITE," Moscow, Izd. VNIITE,
Vyp 16, 1978.
53. Samoylov, A. F. "I. M. Sechenov and His Ideas About the Role of
Muscles in Our Knowledge of the World," in "Fiziologiya nervnoy
sistemy," by I. M. Sechenov, I. P. Pavlov and N. Ye. Vvedenskiy,
Moscow, Medgiz, Vol 3, 1952.
168
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
54. Sechenov, I. M. "Essay on Work Movements of Man," St. Petersburg,
1901.
55. Idem, "Selected Philosophical and Psychological Works," Moscow, 1947.
56. Sokolov, Ye. N. "Memory Mechanisms," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow
University, 1959.
57. Teplov, B. M. "Problems of Individual Differences,~' Moscow, Izd.
APN RSFSR, 1961.
- 58. Ukhtomskiy, A. A. "Selected Works," Zeningrad, Nauka, 1978.
59. Faynburg, Z. I., and Kozlova, G. P. "On the Question of Grouping
_ Workers According to Content of Their Labor,�t in "Sotsial'nyye
issledovaniya" [Social Studies], Moscow, l~auka, Vyp 2, 1968.
60. Sherrington, C. S. "Integrative Activity of the Brain," Leningrad,
Nauka, 1969.
61. Shekhter, M. S. "Psychological Recognition Problems," Moscow,
Prosveshcheniye, 1967.
62. Yudin, E. G. "The Systems Approach and Principle of Activity,"
Moscow, Nauka, 1978.
63. Adams, J. A. "Issues for Closed-Loop Theory of Motor Learning,"
in "Motor Control: Issues and Trends," New York, Academic Press, 1976.
64. Arnheim, R. "Visual Thinking," Berkeley, 1969.
65. Bartlett, F.. "Thinking. An Experimental and Social Study," London,
G. Allen, 1958.
66. Claparede, E. "Genesis of Hypotheses," ARCHIVES DE PSYCHOLOGIE,
Vol 24, No 93-94, 1934.
67. Clark, H. H., and Clark, E. V. "Psychology and Language," New York,
Harcourt, 1977.
68. Estes, W. K. (editor) "Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes,"
New York, L. Erlbaum and Ass., Vol 1-7, 1976-1978.
69. Festinger, L.; Burnham, C. A.; Ono, H.; and Bamber, D. "Efference
and the Conscious Experience of Perception," J. EXP. PSYCHOL.,
monogragh, Vol 74 (4, Pt 2), 1967.
169
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
70. James, W. "Principles of Psychology," New York, Holt, 1890.
71. Neisser, U. "Cognition and Reality," San Francisco, W. H. Freeman,
1976.
72. Phillips, W. A. "On the Distinction Between Sensory Storage ~nd
Short-Term Visual Memory," PERCEPTION AND PSYCHOPHYSICS, Vol 16(2),
1974.
73. Randhawa, B. S., and Coffman, W. E. (ed.) "Visual Learning, Thinking
_ and Communication," New York, Academic Press, 1978.
74. Shepard, R. N. Form, Formation and Transformation of Internal
Representations," in "Information Processing and Cognition: the
Loyola Symposium," R. Solso (ed.), New Jersey, Erlbaum and Ass.,
1975.
75. Smidt, R. A. "The Schema as a Solution to Some Persistent Problems
in Motor Learning Theory," in "Motor Control: Issues and Trends,"
New York, Academic Press, 1976. -
n
76. Sperling, G. The Information Available in Brief Visual Presentation," '
PSYCHOL. MONOGR., 74 (11, whole No 498), 1960.
77. Sperling, G., et al. "Extremely Rapid Visual Search. The Maximum
Rate of Scanning Letters for Presence of a Numeral," SCIENCE, Vol 174,
1971.
78. Turvey, M. T. "Preliminaries to a Theory of Action With Reference to
Vision," in "Perceiving, Acting and Knowing: Toward an Ecological
Psychology," R. Shaw and J. Bransford (eds.), Hillsdale, New York,
Erlbaum, 1977. -
79. Idem, "Contrasting Orientations to the Theory of Visual Information
Processing," PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW, Vol 84(1), 1977.
80. Woodworth, R, S. "The Accu�racy of Voluntary Movement," PSYCHOL. -
MONOGR., Vol 13, 1899. ~
170
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02148: CIA-RDP82-00850R040240090051-4
I
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
CHAPTER V. ERGONOMIC BASES OF TECHNOLOGICAL DESIGN
Until recently, it was considered sufficient to solve problems of designing
new equipment on the basis of considerations of its productivity, relia-
bility and economy of operation. Now this view is being submitted to
more definition and expansion.
The drastic increase in the role of the human factor in national production
and real need for comprehensive development of man, due to the scientific
and technological revolution, which are growing immeasurable in this
- period of developed socialism, compel us to take into consideration not
only the economic, but social ~ffectiveness of the new equipment designs
that are being developed. "It is important here for the n.atural sciences
to be fully aware of the studies, developments and conclusions of the
social sciences pertaining to advancement of man's role in the man-
machine system, the essence, forms and development of sociological, socio-
psychological, ergonomic and ecological factors" [3, p 69].
In a fully developed socialist society, there is drastic increase in sig-
nificance of qualitative characteristics of activity and labor; accordingly,
increased demands have been made of refinement of consumer qualities of
new technology [3J. More and more often, it is suggested that the
traditionally used main parameters of technology (productivity, reliability
and economy of operation) be augmented by indicators of ergonomicity,
ecologicity and aesthetics, which provide for reaching social results
related to safeguarding human health and development of the human per-
sonality, and on this basis for increasing the efficiency and quality _
of activity in the most varied areas. Social implications of new tech-
nology have become an important condition for realizing its potential _
economic effect. If, for example, optimum conditions are not provided
for interaction between man and technology, one can hardly expect
complete achievement of this effect.
"Socioeconomic effectiveness should become the main, if not only, criterion
of formation of technological policy. For this purpose, already at the
stage of scientific development and design of new technology, one must
determine not only the economic effect of the newly developed technology,
but the positive and negative effects that are potentially contained in
171
- � FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
technology, and consider their influence on the socioeconomic life of all
members of society in plans for development of industry on all levels,
from individual enterprises to the national economy as a whole" [2, p 25].
One can assure the social effectiveness of new technolcgy provided the
indices of ergonomicity, ecologicity and aesthetics will, along with the
traditional indicators, def ine the general functional structure of the
man- machine systems tha t are being developed. The social indicators
of technology, including ergonomic ones, are a preset condition, for the
implementation of which one selects the most economic variant that assures
reaching a concrete, specif ied social goal with the least capital and
operating expenses [4]. This does not refer to affirmation of the priority
of man or machines in control systems, but to construct man-machine systems
on the basis of knowledge about the ob~ective [object-related] and struc-
tural patterns of man-machine interaction processes [15]. Only then will
technology solve complex, i.e., dual purpose, problems, perform specific
production engineering tasks and aid in creating optimum working conditions;
alld in any case it would prevent the adverse social consequences of using
new technology in industry. "Endless appeals to the sense of social res-
ponsibility cannot protect modern man against the deleterious effects of
technology; we need a real system of ways and means of controlling this
under socialism. And the extent to which this struggle is conducted
serves as an objective indicator of the progressiveness of the social =
regime, the extent to which its advantages are used" [18, p 78J.
Use of ergonomic achievements in designing technology and its operating
conditions aids in increasing the interest and attractiveness of labor,
preservation of health and, ultimately, creation of conditions that
are favorable to comprehensive development of working man. This provides
for greater efficiency and quality of work, convenience in operation and
upkeep of equipment, shorter period of learning to use it, better working
conditions, saving of phys ical and mental energy of working man, and
maintaining high efficiency.
1. Structure of Ergonomic Properties and Indices of Technology
Let us disclose the content of the concept of "ergonomicity of technology,"
which is a concrete manif estation of the activity-oriented approach in
ergonomics. We submit here the structural diagram of ergonomic indicators
of technological equipmen t. This is an hierarchic, dynamic structure con-
sisting of several levels. Ergonomic properties and indices (essential
features) of each preceding level are the basis for formation of the
ergonomic indices of the next level. Here, the same general princip].e
applies as the one governing interlevel relations between the structure of
human activity, and which consists of the fact that the existing highest
level is always the leading one, but it can express itself only by means of
lower lying levels and depends on them in this respect [10].
172
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The highest level of this hierarchic structure, ergonomicity of technolo-
gical equipment--its integral characteristic--is organically related to
the ind ices of productlvity, reliability and economy of operation. Ergo-
nomicity grows from a number of ergonomic properties, which include
controllability, serviceability, assimilability and habitability. The
first three describe the properties of equipment, with which it is
organically included in the optimum psychophysiological structure of
activity of a man (group of people) pertaining to control, servicing and
assimilation of technology. Habitability refers to the ergonomic pro-
perty of equipment, with which the conditions of its operation are close
to optimal, from the standpoint of vital functions of working man (group),
as well as provide reduction or elimination of deleterious consequences
to the environment of equipment operation. The ergonomic properties of
~equigmenE~constitute certain prerequisites, possibilities of human
activ3ty referable to its ob~ective conditions.
- Ergonomic properties are formed on the basis of cotnplex ergonomi.c indi-
cators, which represent various but interrelated aspects of these proper-
ties. Complex ergonomic indicators are formed on the basis of group
ergonomic indices, which represent the aggregate of homogeneous isolated
ergonomic indices: sociopsychological, psychological, physiological and
- psychophysiological, anthropometric and hygienic.
This structure permits representation of various levels of integration
in ergonomics, each of which has certain qualitative specifics that
cannot be reduced to automatic [mechanical] combination of its component
indices. It is important for the deaigner to know not only the nomen-
clature and characteristics of erganomic indices, but how ergonomic
properties of designed objects are formed on their basis. In this
aspect, design problems are the closest to ergonomics, the inception of
which as a scientific discipline was largely determined by solving the
problem of disclosing the patteras of transitions of some of the
indicator levels discussed and equipment properties into others. Each
step o n rhe road toward solving this extremely complex problem again
sets off the limitation and temporary nature of what is called consider-
ation of the human factor, and it creates realistic conditions for
developing a scientifically substantiated tool for purposeful formation
of ergonomic properties of equipment in the designing process. In other
words, there is substantial change in the role and place of ergonomics
in technological design: from solving specific special problems related
to par tial improvement of man's work in already designed, specified
technological systems, it moves toward full participation in the design
of the general functional structure of man-machine systems.
We ref er to the fact that, from the very start, the man-machine system.
is des igned, rather than only the equipment which, only at the stage
of practical "adjustment" to man, become elements of this system. The
origin of the concept of "consideration o.f the htiman factor" in
173
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02148: CIA-RDP82-00850R040240090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
developing systems is related, not without grounds, to the fact that systems -
analysis considers man as an external factor and the technological part
as the main component of the system.
'Phe methodological significance of the proposed structure of properties and
indices is that it opens up the way for truly comprehensive ergonomic
description of man-machine systems which, in turn, permits the construc-
tion of models reflecting the corresponding patterns of their function.
The theoretical bases of this structure have much in common with the
theses of the systems analytical approach that is being developed for
the study of the main element in the man-machine ~ystem, i.p., man, and -
the system as a whole [11]. This structure is an effective tool of
ergonomics, by means of which appropriate study of man-machin~ systems
is possible on a functional level.
The structure of ergonomic properties and indices of tec:inological
equipment stimulates the process of revision of some established concep-
tions of inethods of planning it, which has already begun, and thereby
is instrumental in its passage to a new, higher level. " Even now, it
is apparently time to think of another direction, development of a
technological assignment proceeding from ideas of the se condary, servic-
ing function of machines and, consequently, with consideration primarily
of the positive qualities of man as the real subject of labor, i.e., that
which c.:onstitutes his advantages over a machine, rather than his short-
comings. On this road we find basically new reserves fo r increasing
labor productivity, i.e., solving one of the most important problems
under the current five-year plan" [17, p 63].
With reference to designing as a process that initiates changes in an
artificial environment (see [6]), attention is focused on a task that
was named "design forecasting" [13J, which is a specific form of scientific
programming of social and other consequences of design work. The
large number and complex nature of such consequences, the fact that they
are significantly deferred from the start and progress of the desig^ing
process proper--all this requires not only the use of new methodology,
but participation of a large number of highly skilled specialists in
collective preparation of designs. Tn design,forecasting the "static"
object of design is replaced by a"self-developing" object, and for this
reason there is a possibility of choice of optimum variants and sifting
out of wrong versions in the course of designing, even b efore their
adverse consequences could become and irreversible facto r in reality.
Scientific research, both applied and basic, turns out not to be merely _
an ancillary element, but an internal necessity that evo lves organically~ _
from the very nature of designing work. _
The above studies include investigation of the patterns of optimum inter-
action between man and machines. One of the central pr oblems of erg4nomics
as a scientific discipZine is to study the interlevel transitions in the
174
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
hierarchic structure of ergonomic properties and indices, and first of all -
to demons[rate the patterns of formation of ergono^d c properties of
equipment, namely: controllability, serviceability, assimi~ability and
habitability. "The main thing is that one cannot overlook the circum-
stance tha[, in interlevel studies, we are not dealing with unilateral =
wovement, but with bilateral and helical at that: with fo nnation of _
higher levels and "separation into layers"--or alteration--of lower levels
that, in turn, determine the possibility of further development of the
system as a whole. Thus, interlevel studies, while remaining inter-
disciplinary, at the same time preclude interpretation of the latter
as reduction of one level to another or the desire to find their corre-
lations and coordination" [10, p 233].
The problem of designing the activity of man (group) f or the conr.rol
(use) of technology is becoming an organic part of ttie general process
of design. This problem can be solved satisfactorily if one is governed _
by.ergonomics.:,and other disciplines that deal with man and his activities.
Such enrichment and complication of designing is consistent with the -
substance of technology which is "human" in its purpose and the progress
of which occurs in accordance with the laws of development of humsa labor
[12, 16]. K. Marx repeatedly stressed that technology refers to "organs
of the human brain created by human hands" [I, p 215].
The real process of interaction between man and machine cannot be
accommodated within the f ramework of general recommendations and concrete
requirements of ergonomics. Basing oneself on these data, in the course _
of designing equipment one must solve the problem of comprehensive modeling
of human activity and conditions under which it is performed. In other
words, it is imperative to have a clear enough and comprehensive idea
about what and how man will do with a given form of equipment and under
what conditions. Development and evaluation on this basis of proposed
designs to create convenient and safe equipment are distinguished in a
spec. sl area of ergonomic design of man-machine syste~s. Being charac-
terized by certain specifics, ergonomic design is governed by the general
patterns and methods of designing work. ~
2. Consideration of Ergonomic Requirements in Equipment Design
With the existing practices and mer,hods, equipment design, at best,
only takes into consideration the ergonomic requirements at different
stages of development, which permits achi2vement of some optimization of -
activity of a man (or group) in the man (group)-machine system and,
accordingly, increasing efficiency of system operation as a whole.
Ergonomic requirements of equipment are determined by psychological, anthro-
pometric and biomechanical features of man, and they are set in order to
optimize his performance. Ergonomic requirements refer to features
which, being embodied in equipment, become its properties and indices [5].
175
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
,
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
- FOR OFFICIAL IISE ONLY -
Integ.al '
ergonomic ~ F.rgonomicity of technology
feature j
Ergonomic Controllability
propert ies -
Complex Conformity of distribution of functions between man (group
_ indices of people) and equipment with optimum structure of their
interaction in reaching set goals.
Conformity of desi~n of equipment (or different elements
thereof ) and organization of work place with optimum psycho-
physiological structure of activity to control it.
_ Conformity of content of control activity set by equipment
with optimum level of complexity and diversity of man~s _
actions.
Conformity of intensity of activity set by equipment with
' minimal strain, with which maximum efficiency of control
is reached. ,
Conformity of requirements made by equipment of quality
of control activity with optimum accuracy, spe ed and
reliability capabilities of man. -
Conformity of equipment-set rhythms of work processes
with optimum time structure of actions of working people.
Group Sociopsychological Psychological
indices
Conformity of design of equip- Conformity of equipment
Isolated ment and organization of work with: capabilities and
indices places with nature and degree distinctions of percep-
of group interaction. tion, memory, thinking,
_ Degree of inediation of inter- psychomotor activity,
~ersonal relations by the fixed and newly formed _
ccntent of juint activity to skills of working man.
control techn~logical
equipment . -
~ Structural diagram of ~rgonomic propert~es and indices of equipment
~.:hoi t e;~tends to �ollowing page~
176
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Serviceability Assimilability Habitability
Ccnformity of design Equipment has the provisions Conformity of equi
of equipment (or its for speedy assimilation ent operating con-
components) with (to acquire the necessary ditions with bio-
optimum psychophysio- knowledge, ability and logically optimum
logical structure of control akills). parameters of work
activity dealing with Requ~rements set by the environmenC, assur-
operation, upkeep and equipment as to level of ing man's normal
repair thereof. devel~pment of occupa-- development, good
tionally significant ealth and high
psychophysiological and efficiency.
psychological functions of Possibility of
an. reducing or elimi-
Requirements set by the ating equipment
equipment as to nature and operating condi-
degree of grc,up interaction tions that are
- for control thereof. deleterious to the
natural environment
P~ hysiological, Anthropometric Hygienic
~psycho~h siolo ical
Conformity of equip- Conformity of equipment Indices: lighting,
ent with: force, ith dimensions and shape ventil?tion, tempe-
speed, energy, visua of working man, distri- rature, humidity,
tactile, olfactory bution of his weight pressure, intensity
~ capabilities of man of magnetic & elec-
tromagnetic fields,
dust, radiation,
toxicity, noise,
vibration, G forces
and accelerations
Structural diagram (continued)
177
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONL�
.
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Ergonomic design and research problems are solved in relation to concrete
types of man-machine-environment systems, specific types of man~s activity.
Consideration of ergonomic requirements must permeate all stages of
preparing designs and expert evaluation thereof. At the stage of develop-
ment of a technological assignment in general form, determination must
be made of the ergonomic requirements of the object of design and of
the need to conduct special ergonomic studies. It is very important to
correctly translate the problem froin the language of engineering design
- to the language of ergonomics by means of analysis of this problem in
the context of the specific problems of the human factar. For this,
analysis is made of the purpose of the object to be designed and related
operating requirements; determination is made of the place and role of
man in solving problems ensuing from the above-mentioned purpose.
The specialist in ergonomics, who becomes part of a team of designers and
participates in the process of equipment design, is dealing with a
special object of design, man and his activity, the means of which are
knowledge about man and relevant special methods and procedures [8].
Man's activity in the system is the beginning and end of ergonomic design,
evaluation and study. Already at the first stage of design, a tentative
professiograr~ is plotted, which defines the goals and objectives of work,
psychophysiological conditions, composition and content of operations it
involves, as well as the concrete requirements made in a given instance
of man and machines.
Professiography is a complex and precise art. Specialists who analyze
work activity can be compared to a keen therapist, in whose practice
scientific methods are combined with rich intuition and experience.
Occasionally, the ergonomist himself mastars the work activity, at least
to an elementary degree, and thus is able to analyze it "from the inside."
A professiogram is the starting point of ergonomic research and the
foundation for all work dealing with consideration of relevant requirements
in designing technological equipment.
Ana~ysis of analogs and protctypes pinpoints knowledge about the purpose
and principles of action, as well as structural features of a machine; it
determines its characteristics as applied to the goals of work activity
and its optimization, including creation of optirsum conditions for
operating, servicing and repairing the equipment designed.
Distribution of functions between man and machir_e is an important
designing task. It cannot be performed so].ely on the basis of engineer-
ing approaches to distribution of functions in a man-machine system,
especially since none has the necessary universality and effectiveness
of applications [14].
178
FOR OFFICI':~ USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The problem of choice of degree of automation and mechanization of func-
tions is rather complex and important. For example, planning the work -
of a pilot during an automated landin~ made it possible to determine the -
desirability of semiautomatic, rather than automatic control on the
' approach path, since in this case the pilot's readiness to take over
- manual control in the event of sudden malfunction of automatic equipment
is sustained at a high level thanks to retention of the status of his
readiness for emergency action, in the first place, and retention of
closer contact with the controlled object, in the second place [7]. It
is very important not to disrupt some integrity of structure of man's
activity when choosing the variant of rational distribution of functions.
When selecting a variant of distribution of functions (and different forms -
thereof), general methodological considerations must also be bornF in
mind, with respect to man's social function as the subject of labor,
as well,as the results of concrete ergonomic, psychological, physiological
and otlier studies. It is also apparent that, at the present stage, sub-
stantiation of rational or even optimum distribution of functions must be
- based on quantitative estimates of the quality of man's (and machine)
performance and evaluation of the influence of this quality on the overall
efficiency of the system. Thus far, there has not been sufficient develop-
ment of such criteria; however, by no means can this serve as justifica-
tion for the disregard for quantitative evaluation m~~~:hods that is still
- encountered.
- There are some substantial flaws in the existing methods of qualitative
evaluation baseu on lists of advantages and limitations of man and
machines. These~lists are too general, and they do not take into con-
sideration of the specifics of man-machine interaction, limitations
and factors of an economic and social nature, as well as questions of
human motivation. Finally, they are far from strict coverage of
existing (and quite insufficient) time and accuracy parameters of
operations performed by man.
When trying to apply quantitative methods to substantiate distribution
of functions, the main difficulties arise more because of the absence of
data pertaining to some important parameters of the man-machine system at
the early stages of designing, which is precisely the time when the
problem of distribution of functions must be solved, than because of the
lack of refinement ot formal optimizaticn procedures.
~
It is quite obvious that progress in solving the problem of distribution -
of functions can only be made along the lines of combining qualitative _
� and quantitative evaluations, with prevalence of the latter. Such com-
bination must, of course, be based on a clearcut classification of the
problems to be solved and analysis of their components, primarily the
_ concrete operations, the performance of which makes up the process of
_ work pertaining to control of equipment.
179
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
After determination is made of the sequence of operator performance of
functions (according to subproblems, operation units, main operations,
etc�,) and of the required scope and form of information display, as
well as at least general determination of reliability, time and
precision requirements of man's work as a whole, one can furnish sub-
stantiated answers to the following questions of developers of man-
machine systems: How many people and what qualifications are required to
solve problems of MMS and expressly what functions must they perform?
What algorithms and computer programs must be developed? What equipment
must be designed or taken from existing system? Then the following is
determined: 1) final complement of specialists for the MMS, their func-
tional duties and organization of work; 2) composition of collective and
individual means of displaying information, control elemements at work
places and control consoles; 3) arrangement of ineans of displaying
iniormation and controls at the work places, and arrangement of work
_ places in industrial premises.
We submit an example of general procedure for choosing a variant of
rational distribution of functions, which was developed in designir.~
"Man (group) - ship engineering" systems, the basic theses of which are
applicable to other man-machine systems as well [5, p 143] (Table 1).
High professional requirements are made of the analytical stage of an
ergonomist's work, since his findings should constitute the solution of
~ basic ergonomic problems of upgrading an existing piece of technological
equipment or a new one being designed. At this stage, there is the
most effective manifestation of collaboration between the ergonomist,
designing engineer and designer, the mutual understanding of which usually
enriches the conception of each of them of the general object of design,
and permits finding the most productive designs, including those based
on knowledge of psychophysiological patterns of human activity. Deter-
mination of the necessity and purpose of experimental ergonomic studies
- is made from the results of the analytical stage.
Ergonomic analysis of work activity and distribution of functions between
man and machine create the necessary foundation to develop first general
and then detailed algorithms of human work. The essence of development
of algorithms consists of differentiation of work activity into quali-
d tatively different components, definition of their logical interrelation
and order of following one another [9]. Algorithmic description of work
allows us to turn to definition of the psychological and physiological
functions that implement separate elementary actions and logical
conditions. ~
After performing the above actions, we turn to direct development of ergo-
nomic requirements of equipment and its operating conditions, its separate
elements and work places, which are Chen embodied in the design and
organization of all of the items mentioned. The system of developed
180
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
designs and expert evaluation thereof, which assure consideration of
ergonomic requirements, is not a unidirectional process of successivQ
movement from stage to stage; rather, it often includes movement in the
opposite direction, with subsequent return to the initial position
and further advancement forward.
Table 1. Procedure for selection of a varlant of rational distribution
of functions
Problem solving stages Content of problem solving
Preliminary distribution of 1. Compilation of total list of all
functions functions imposed on the designed
man-machine system.
2. Determination of characteristics of
each function by means of expertise
' methods.
3. Choice of functions that should be
performed, in principle, by techno-
logical devices.
4. Ranking the remaining functions
according to one or several features.
5. Distribution-of functions between
man and machines by means of one of
the qualitative methods
Evaluation of adopted variant 1. Development of general abgorithms and
of distribution of functions formulation of structure of man's
activity referable to performance
of all functions assigned to him.
2. Obtaining base data for quantitative
evaluation of activity according to
appropriate indices.
Redistribution of functions (in 1. Reduction (increase) in number of
the event the obtained indices functions delegated to man and in-
do not meet the requirements of crease (reduction) of machine func-
the technological assignment) tions, and, consequently, of expenses
and determinatic~n of number of to develop the equipment.
specialists in each man-machine 2. Creation of group work place if one
system (individual or group cannot rationally distribute functions
work place) with one person.
3. Determination of number of specialists
- for each work place.
_ 4. Determination of total individual
work places in each MMS
5. Determination of mode of MMS function
(continuou~ periodic, episodic)
181
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Development of a n artistic design for a hydraulic copying lathe with
programmed control can serve as an example of practical implementation of
the above general scheme of ergonomic designing (in a somewhat reduced
form). Although, in this case a new lathe was not developed, but an
existing one updated, ergonomic designing was rathEr deep a~d multi-
~ faceted.
The work started with becoming closely acquainted with the main principles
and technological distinctions of operation of semiautomatic copying
lathes, that distinguish them from general purpose lathes and semi-
automatic ones of other types.
At the second stage, the tasks confronting the designer group in the
ergonomic aspect were formulated and defined on the basis of the obtained
data and preliminary professiogram of lathe operator work: spatial organi-
zation of work places for lathe operator and repairman [trouble-shooter];
rational lay-out of control elements and display means in order to
reduce fatigue related to distinctions of professional activity; lowering
the probability of erroneous use of control elements; reducing time
required to service the lathe during work.
The place of the lathe operator, as the representative of the largest
occupation in operating lathes, rather than the work place of the repair-
man, was chosen as the main place to be remodeled. This was also attri-
butable to the fact that the work of a lathe operator is notable for
high monotony, stereotypism and repetition of operations within a limited
- space. As we know, with monotonous activity attention declines and
fatigue develops rapidly which, in turn, could lead to cases of industrial
traumatism. For this reason, improvement of organization of the lathe
operator's work place, reduction of static and dynamic muscle load, and
improvement of. organization of the sensorimotor field should aid in
creating optimum conditions for work activity, longer maintenance of
a high level of efficiency, economy of human resources, as well as ~
more efficient operation of the lathe.
The third stage dealt with professiographic analysis of lathe operator '
work under industrial conditions, with both domestic and foreign lathes
analogous to the one being updated. The main work operations were
singled out; operation-by-operation time studies were made; determination
was made of the frequency of using various controls and of the nature of
monitoring the technological process.
_ The fourth stage consisted of ergonomic analysis of organization of the
work places of the lathe operator and repairman on a prototype of the
lathe in the adjustment and automatic modes. It is known that the
spatial arrangement of lathe working elements largely determines the
' scope and nature of the operator's sensorimotor activity and, consequently,
182
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000204090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
the efficiency of his labor. Hence, much attention was given to the study
of the speci�ics of work operations taking place in the work zones of the
lathe.
t.nalysis of each operation made it possible to rank the work operations
according to their significance to the technological process, to deter-
mine the preferred work zones for each group of operations and relate them
to existing designs of equipment. For a lathe operator, the main zone
_ was found to be the one related to installation of work pieces jor parts].
There were more such aones for the repairman: in addition to the zone
related to installation of pieces in common for bot:~ the worker and
repairman, the latter can also work in the zone of the programming
array [matrixJ located in a se~arate cabinet, in the zone of the
lathe's drum and fine manual adjustment of cutting tools; there are also
several ancillary work zones, in which episodic, one-time operations are
performed.
Graphic analysis of the layout design was made by means of superposition
on orthogonal projections of the lathe prototype of the outline of
maximum range of the sensorimotor field determined experimentally. From
this, it was easy to see that all controls, disp?ay devices and work sur-
_ faces were within reach of a working man in two main working positions.
The conditions were not optimal for demonstration of his work activity
as a whole, because the work surfaces were not always correctly oriented.
For ergonomic optimization of the general lay-out, it was also suggested
that the depth of the lathe be reduced over its entire length.
The most apprecia~le flaw in organization of the work place was the
spatial separation of zones of control and monitoring the object of
labor and tool, i.e., the zones of motor and sensory activity of the
worker, which leads to unnecessary expenditure of his muscular and mental
energy. Such an excessive load is particularly inadmissible in the
work of the repairman, since the quality of the entire series of
articles subsequently produced on the lathe depends on accuracy and
quality of adjustment. Some inconvenience also arose in performing
fine manual adjustment with the use of graduated circles, upon turning
of which the worker blocks the disks with graduations with his hand.
Several design flaws were demonstrated by analysis of ancillary operations.
- In particular, the ill-c?~osen location of the top master form requires
much physical force to immobilize the pieces, unnecessary movement of the
repairman during work; the ill-chosen design of protective shields leads
to appearance of superfluous operations and prolongs the processing cycle.
Ergonomic analysis revealed flaws in the control console, on which a
significant number of controls, displays and monitoring devices is
concentrated. Expressly in the zone of the console the greatest part
of the mcst important operations is performed as indicated, in particular,
183
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-44850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
~y the frequency, with which the repairman refers to this zone. When
operating in the automatic mode, the console becomes the principal
element of the work place, i.e., concentration of the worker's motor
activity. As shown by analysis, a significant part of the console is
below the optimum zone of sensorimotor activity of both the repairman and
lathe operator, while there is no standard principle of grouping
controls according to functions, sequence of actions, etc. From the
standpoint of the repairman, the horizontal arrangement of controls and
displays pertaining to work with the top copying support is inconvenient.
The need to move controls to the far right to operate this support makes
it preferable to arrange them vertically, rather than horizontally.
As a result of the analytical work done by ergonomists, there was an
assignment given to designers, which amounted to the following main
items in general form: improve conditions for coordination of sensory
(mainly visual) and motor activity of lathe operator and repair man;
improve conformity of spatial parameters of the lathe with anthropometric
data for individuals working on it; have the programming matrix
together with the control console; optimize arrangement of controls on
the console in accordance with the distinctions of the work of the
main group of specialists operating and servicing this lathe.
The concrete ergonomic recommendations consisted of the following: to
- try to reduce the depth of the lathe; install back-up controls on the
~ back mandrel; move the device for presetting the support to the control
console; raise the console so that all controls are in the optimum zone;
tilt the console; group controls and monitoring devices according to
function; arrange controls vertically instead of horizontally; addi-
tionally provide visual distinction of each functional group of controls
(for example, by color); the working elements related to the top copying
support should be placed in the right top corner of the control console
- at a height of 120-150 cm above the floor; controls for the bottom
copying support should be arranged next to them or so~newhat lower.
In addition, several special comments were made: provide for the possibi-
lity of installing pieces of any size without removing protective Shields;
revise the design of protective screen handles; provide local light for
the lathe; provide a device to support the master form, especially when
its length is significant, to alleviate the installing operation and
thereby reduce the worker's phyaical tension.
A special stand was produced for experimental ergonomic studies, which
permits dynamic reproduction of spatial conditions of lathe operator work.
On this stand, several three-dimensional models of the lathe and work zone
were reproduced successively by means of sliding rods and suspended equip-
- ment simulating the main working elements of the lathe (clamping chuck,
rear mandrel, etc.). Bioelectric activity of muscles was recorded while
sub~ects worked on the models. The obtained myograms made it possible to
choose one out of several tested models, the d~mensions and geometric shape
of which involved minimal muscular tension for the lathe operator to
maintain a work pose.
184
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Table 2, Sample content of ergonomic work at the different stages of
designing equipment ,
Stages Main ergonomic tasks Resul~s of work
1. Engineering 1.1. Determination of purpose of equipment, Preliminary
assignment analysis of analogs & prototypes, and professiogram
~heir erognomic description
1.2. Ergonomic analysis of man's work in Preliminary
real MMS (or preparation of program ergonomic re-
for planning man's work in a newly quirements of
developed system) man, equipment,
work place,
industrial
environment
1.3. Tentative distribution of functions Assignment to
, in man-machine system [MMSJ conduct ergono-
mic researclz
1.4. Tentative ergonomic requirements on
the basis of existing standards,
ergonomic reference material and ~
results of items 1.1, 1.2 and 1.3
2. Engineering 2.1. Definition of distribution of func- Specification
: proposal and tions in MMS and preparation of of ergonomic
design general algorithms of man's work requirements
- sketch 2.2. Specification & implementation of and implementa-
proposals and draft of tentative tion thereof
ergonomic requirements of worker, in engineering
equipment, work place, industrial proposal and
environment. design sketch.
2.3. Ergonomic evaluation of design Results of
variants expert's evalua-
2.4. Studies in laboratory and under tion of design
industrial conditions to define
algorithms of work and ergonomic
requirements
~ 2.5. Preliminary evaluation of degree
of implementation of ergonomic
requirements by analytical and
modeling methods
3. Engineering 3.1. Final distribution of functions in Ergonomic re-
design MMS and preparation of detailed quirements and
algorithms of man's work. their implemen-
3.2. Determination of final ergonomic tation in
requirements & their implementa- engineering plan.
tion in plan. Resuits of expert
3.3. Evaluation of degree of implementa- evaluation of
tion of ergonomic requirements by designs
. analytical and modeling methods
185
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
'rable "L (continued).
Stages Main ergonomic tasks Results
4. Preparation 4.1. Analysis and experimental evaluation Ergonomic char-
of drawings of designed produr_~ under real acteristics of
- and operating conditions to determine equipment,
testing extent of satisfying ergonomic work places,
requirements industrial
4.2. Proposals to refine (upgrade) environment;
product and corresponding amendment proposals to
of design improve them;
4.3. Ergonomic characteristics (evalua tion) requirements
of product quality referable to
4.4. Preparation of ergonomic requirements operation and
pertaining to instructions on upkeep of
operation and upkeep . equipment
The final stage of the work was to compare two variants of the prototype
lathe to the design of an updated lathe. Graphic-analytic metnods and _
electromyography (recording biopotentials of muscles) were the main
techniques used.
Graphic-analytic methods combined with photography were used mainly to
analyze the characteristics of the work space zones, main working body
positions and visual monitoring zones. Electromyography was used to
analyze overall energy expended by the worker to perform the main opera-
tions with change in spatial organization of the motor zone (a comparison
was made of data for the prototype and modified latYee variant). The
studies were conducted on the above-mentioned stand, which permitited
rapid reproduction of any spatial conditions Af activity (for example, the
parameters of the main work zones of the prototype and modified variant
when performing the operation of placing an article, etc.). These
methods were als~ used to determine optimum location of fine ad~ustment
controls (levers with dials).
Analysis of the obtained data revealed that there was considerable decrease
in muscular fatigue (particularly of muscles of the back and abdomen) and
decreased asymmetry of function of the strongest muscles carrying the
static load, which generally reduced energy expended by the body, when
working on the modified variant of the lathe (both lathe operator and
repairman). At the same time, there was faster and more accurate reading
of display and monitoring instruments, and in the course of comparative
analysis it was found possible to additionally refine the design of some
units and parts of the lathe.
186
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
As a result of expert ergonomic evaluation of the two 1 athe variants,
there was demonstration of the advantages of the developed artistic
design, from the standpoint of providing optimum working conditions and
increasing efficiency of lathe operation. Routine and adjustment work
was alleviated by placing the panel with the set of programs and control
console of the lathe in the same place, next to one another, on the
same plane, in a zone convenient to the warker; replacement of manual
setting of stops with instruments to measure cycles; back-up control
console was placed on the rear mandrel; glassed off area of protective
shields was enlarged and their weight reduced; notches were cut on the
front and sides of the lathe to provide a normal foot position and
posture of the worker; there were also other improvements that simplified
servicing and upkeep of the lathe. Rational design of electrical and
hydraulic systems of the lathe, a search for which was prompted also
by the task of providing for convenient servicing and upkeep of the lathe,
reduced its dimensions: the length by 250 mm and width by 253 mm, as a
result of which the area it occupied decreased by 1.5 square meters. In
addition, unifortnity of composition and Color of the lathe was provided.
At the same time, it must be conceded that the fact that it was impossible
to make radical changes in the construction and arrangement of this
lathe limited substantially the opportunity to come close to optimum
workin~ conditions for the lathe operator and repairman.
- Many ergonomic norms and requirements have been reflected in GOST's:
= Man-machine systems, Systems of standards for labor safety practices
(SSBT), Sanitary norms and rules, Standards for terms and nomenclature
- or ~ rgonomic indices of quality, as well as other stand ard-setting
- documents. Consideration of ergonomic requirements in designing techno-
logical equipment implies unfailing adherence to the standard-setting
documents listed. Table 2 lists the exemplary content of work that must
be done in full with regard to consideration of ergonomic requirements at
- all stages of development of complex technological equipment.
BIBLTOGRAPHY
1. tiarx, K., and Engels, F. "Works," Vol 46, Pt 2.
2. Vilenskiy, M. A. "Socioeconomic Effectiveness of Scientific and _
Technological Progress," in "Metodologicheskiye voFrosy opredeleniya
sotsial'no-ekonomicheskoy effektivnosti novoy tekhniki" [Methodolo-
- gical Problems of Determing the Socioeconomic Eff e ctiveness of New
Technology], Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
3. Gatovskiy, L. M. "Scientific-Technological Progres s and Economics of
Developed Socialism," Moscow~ Nauka, 1974.
187
FOR OFFICIAL USE ~NLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE nNLY
4. Gatovsl;iy, L. M. "Cri[eria for Determining the Economic Effectiveness ~
of Using New 'Cechnology in the National Economy," in
"Sovetsko-amPrikanskiy simpozium ekonomistod' [Soviet-American
Symposium of EconomistsJ, Moscow, Progress, 1978.
5. Cubinskiy, A. I., and Yevgrafov, V. G. Ergonomic Design of
Ship Control Systems," Leningrad, Sudostroyeniye, 1977.
- 6. Jones, G. "Engineering and Artistic Design. Modern Methods of
Design Analysis~" translated fro~n English, edited by V. F. Venda and
V. M. Munipov, Moscow, Mir, 1976.
7. Dobrolenskiy, Yu. P.; Zavalova, N. D.; Ponomarenko, V. A.; and
Tuvayev, V. A. "Methods of Engineering Psychology Research in
Aviation," Moscow, Mashinostroyeniye~ 1975.
8. Dubrovskiy, V. Ya., and Shchedrovitskiy, L. P. "Engineering Psychology
and Development of Systems Analytic Deaign," in
"Inzhenerno-psikhologicheskoye proyektirovaniye" [Planning in the
Aspect of Engineering Psychology], Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow University,
Vyp 2, 1970.
9. Zarakovskiy, G. M. "Psychophysiological Analysis of Work Activity,
Moscow, Nauka, 1966.
10. Leont'yev, A. N. "Activity. Consciousness. Personality," Moscow,
Politizdat, 1975.
11. Lomov, B. F. "Means of Constructing Theory of Engineering Psychology
on the Basis of the Systems Analysis Approach," in "Inzhenernaya
psikhologiya. Teoriya, metodologiyt~, prakticheskoye primeneniye"
[Engineering Psychology: Theory, Methodology and Practical
Applications], Moscow, Nauka, 1977.
12. Meleshchenko, Yu. S. Technology and Patterns in Its Development,"
Leningrad, Izd-vn Leningrad University, 1970.
13. "Fundamentals of Aesthetic Styling in Engineering (Expanded
Th~sea)," Moscow, Izd. VNIITE [A1~-Union Scientific Research
Institute of Aesthetic Styling in Engineering], 1970.
14. Ronzhin, 0. N. "Information Methods of Studyin~ Ergatic Systems,"
Moscow, Energiya, 1976.
15. Smolyan, G. I.. C~nception af Man and Machine Interaction: Origin,
- Development and Signific~nce," VOPROSY FILOSOFII [Problems of
Philosophy], No 4, 1978.
188 -
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
16. Stslov, V. Ya. "Labor Under Conditions of Fully Developed Socialism,"
Moscow, Nauka, 1976.
17. "There Must be Strengthening of Relation~ Between Social, Natural and
Engineering Sciences," KOMMUNIST, No 1, 1978. _
18. Anisimov, S. F., and Dryakhlov, N. I. (editors) "Formation of Man's
Spiritual World, and the Scientific and Technological Revolution
(Methodological Problems of Analysis of the Spiritual World of
Man in a Fully Developed Socialist Society)," Moscow, Izd-vo Moscow
University, 1977.
~
189
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
I
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
1 j ~
i _ L~ ~ '
i
~ . . . ~ ~ 1 . ' .
,~~~~E . fi. I ~~~HE1~~~ ~#~~L~ I ~ ti -
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
CHAPTER VII. OPTIMIZATION OF TNFC'�~1,TT1~N DISPLAY SYSTEMS AND MEANS ~
1. Operator Work With Information Models '
Development of industry in the 20th century is increasingly characterized
by mechanization and automation of production processes. In a number of
cases this results in the fact that it is not so easy to specifically
indicate and define the ob~ect of labor and its results wi th respect to
many types of activity. The fact of the matter is that the means of
work are beginning to take the place of its ob~ect in thE worker's
consciousness, while the object itself is, so to .speak, "dematerialized."
This process of dematerialization occurred gradually. There were and
are many situations when the required accuracy of direct observation and
evaluation exceed the resolution capacity of human sense organs. Various
sensors, information from which is submitted in analog or digital form,
, began to be used to increase the accuracy of direct observation. This
information duplicates in part the direct perception of the ob~ect of
labor or work process. Instrument information is displayed in the most
convenient form for perception. The use of such dual sources of informa-
tion is the start of "splitting" of the object of work. Man is beginning
to deal not only and, in some cases, not so much *aith dir ectly observed
properties, as with instrument-measured properties of the work ob~ect.
Such situations are typical of many transport-related occupations,
metallurgistsstool mskers [or workersj, etc. As man removes himself
more and more from the object of labor, by virtue of impossibility or
hazard of direct ob~ervation, diverse means of remote monitoring and
control, special information display devices, began to be increasingly
used. The latter are intended to display to man the data characterizing
objects of control or their parameters, progress of a technological pro-
_ cess, presence of energy resources, state of automation equipment,
communication channels, etc. These da.ta are displayed to man in quanti-
tative and qualitative, including pictorial, form.
_ As a result of introduction of systems of remo~ monitoring and control,
information display equipment began to be used as the only source of
information about the controlled ohject, work process, as we11 as the
state of the remote control system or man-machine [MMS] system. The
_ operators of such systems do not work with real ob~ects, but with their
w
190
' FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
" FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
substitutes or images that simulate them, i.e., with infor~:ation madels
of. real objects. The latter, being the means of operator work, not
infrequently also hecome its object.
- '1'tic i.nformation model iq a set of information, organized in accordance
with a certain system of rules, about the state and operation of the
object of control and envir~nment. It is a unique simulstor for the
operator, which reflects all of the properties bf real object that
are essential to control, i.e., the source of informat:+.on, on the basis
of which he forms an image of the real situation, analyzes and assesses
the formed situation, plans control action, makes decisions that assure
proper operation of the system and performance 'of tasks imposed on it,
as well as observes and evaluates the results of this performance.
In the methodological philosophical literature,; modei refers to functional
homomorphic transfer (reflection) of part of ttie outside world to a
system of concepts (images, visualized picture~, symbols, signs, etc.). -
' This reflection is not unequivocal, i.e., isom~rphic; however, it retains
the relations that exist between elements c~f tYte outside world. The
latter property enables the model to be not on]iy descriptive, but
predictive. In accordance with this definition, the important components .
of the model are: 1) concepts (terms, signs, s}rmbols); 2) postulates
(axioms or laws); 3) rules of transformation (zules of calculation);
4) rules of conforn+ity, reflection, which permit comparing the results
of calculations to experimental or practical r~sults. These four general
theses can describe theory models, as well as yery simple models.
Working ["operational"] definitions of a modellare also common. A
' system is a model if it is capable of answering questions about the
outside worZd. An important advantage of a woxking definition is ~
that it includes not only theory models, but c~?bernetic systems created !
with computers. - I
I
According to the generally recognized thesis tl~at a model that is too
abstract is useless, while one with t~o many d~tails leads into error, -
the volume of information included in a mo3e1 nd rules of organizing
it should conform with the obaectives and meth~ds of control. Physically,
an information model is rendered by means o~ diverse devices for display
of information. '
I
The most essential distinction of man~s work w~.th an information model
is the need to relate ~?nformation obtained fro~n instruments, screens,
mnemonics, signal pan~ls, etc., both to one another and to real
_ concrolled objects. The entire activity of anloperator is based on pro- _
cedures of relating such information. Hence, ~it is understandable that
construction of an adequate information model ~.s one of zhe most important _
tasks of designing a control system as a whole~.
~ _
i
i
191 ~
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY~ �
- I
I -
= ~
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
In work deali.~g with creation of information models, wh=ch precedes the
choice of technical means of implementing it, i.s., information display
~ means, one must be governed by the following ergonomic requirements:
with regard to content, information models must adequately reflect objects
of control, work processes, the environment and state of the control
system itself; with respect to amount of information, information models =
must provide the optimum balance of information and should not lead to
such undesirable situationa as shortage or surplus of information; with
respect to form and composition, information models must conform with
the ob~ectives of the work process and man's capabilities pertaining
to receipt, analysis, evaluation of information and execution of
_ controlling actions.
Comprehensive consideration of these requirements in the course ot
designing results in the required flexibility [dynamics] and accuracy of
man's work and, in particular, efficient performance of functions by the
MMS.
The information models of modern MMS's do, in most cases, adequately re-
flect objects of control and state of the control system. Nevertheless,
the work of an operator with them often fails to meet the requirements
of flexibility and precision.
Experience shows that operators often encounter difficulties, which are
the result of the fact that the designer proceeds from t~e wrong or
incomplete conceptions of human capacity for receiving and processing
information. This is also related to miscalculations, such as a poor
choice of coding system, display of excessively large volumes of informa-
tion or too rapid succession thereof, not to mention disregard for
elementary psychophysiological requirements. The main reason for this
is that the information model is not infrequently based on the system of
correlations of a real object, which does not take into consideration the
specific distinctions of psychological structure of man's work with this
object.
The objective [object-related] content of operator work is quite diverse.
This diversity is reflected in the classification of automated control
systems (ASU). One sh~uld only add to it the control system proper and _
its e.lements, which emerge as the special objective content of work of
operators engaged in ft~nctional monitoring and servicing of automated
equipment. The description of objective [object-related] content of
objects of control must necessaril,y include time-space and dynamic
parameters of their existence, operation and interaction.
Incidentally, in order to illustrate the diversity of objective content
of operator work, it should be recalled that his own functional state
also serves in this capacity. This is typical of the biomedical,
psychological and ergonomic studies conducted by cosmonauts.
192 -
FOR OFFICIAL US~ ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Analysis of ubjective content of work is the basic and mandatory condition _
for solving any ergonomic problems. The details of objective content of
activity are particularly needed at the stages of development of informa-
~ ti.~n models and operator training.
A description of the psyc:~ological content of operator work was offered
in the works of D. Yu. Panov and V. P. Zinchenko [8, 9], after which it
was reproduced numerous times, with additional details and definition as
related to various forms of operator work. Here, it should be stressed
rhat ergonomics and engineertng psychology study and plan expressly work
with information (and executory) models. The term, "interaction of man
with automation equipment," is not infrequently used in engineering
psycliology. However, this term does not permit fixing the specifics of
human activity. As we know, automation deviees can interact with one
another even without the help of man. It would not have been necessary
to discuss this if the terms, "information interaction," "information
exchange," etc., did not set the wrong methodological orientation for
studies in ergonomics and engineering gsychology.
The concept of activity is also applicable when dealing with man-machine
dialog. There is always a leading partner in any dialog. The only
change in man-machine dialogs in automated control system is that the "
operator has considerably greater freedom of operation with an information
model, as compared to first generation ASU. Evidently, in the future,
operators themselves will determine, to a certain degree, the content
and form of information model, addressing themselves to the information
software of ASU's.
The key problem of psychological analysis of operator work is related to
the content, form of permanent and operational graphic-conceptual
models (GCM) of the real and predicted situation, the control system
itself, potential and real problem situations. The GCM also contains a
system of evaluations and values, operational capabilities, a general
conception of time and space, and the specific mode of interaction
between the individual and the outside world. The problem of internal
models of the surroundings [environment] emerged earlier in philosophy ,
and genera?. psychology than in engineering psychology. These models
were also called subjective, conceptual. (Incidentally, we could also
mention the use of the terms, "cerebral" and "mental [psychic] model,"
which are the ~ame in meaning but inadequate in form.)
Within the context of research in engineering psychology, the problem of
internal and conceptual models was formulate d in England in 1943; but ,
after this it could not be properly elaborated for a long time. Interest
in this problem was revived in the last few years, in connection with the
arrival of cognitive psychology to replace neobehaviorism and the informa-
tion approach. In our literature, there are many experimental psycholo-
gical studies dealing with the problem of formation and function of GCM.
193
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICI~AL USE ONLY
This is related to the main orientation of Soviet ergonomics and engineering
psychology toward formation of a system of rational actions in an operator,
rather than chains of reaction. Although requirements are made of man's -
work in an ASU with regard to speed, promptness, immediacy, this does
not mean that man has to develop reactive, irnpulsive forms of behavior.
To stress the significance of the GCM in operator work is to stress
the rational, conscious nature of his activity.
The difficulty of rational definition (and planning) of onerator work is
that he is put in the control system to perform functions for which it
is often impossible to elaborate clearcut and unequivocal instructions and
rules. The operator is entrusted with performance or monitoring of the
most important and responsible functions in the system. Rational action
is required of Che operator in unforeseen circumstances, often under
conditions of insufficient and, at times, unr~liable information. The work
of an operator, like that of the control system as a whole, proceeds on
a real time scale, and this imposes special requirements as to its speed
and accuracy.
Problems of optimizati~a and planning of operator work with information
models, preparation of specifications for information models, means of
forming permanent and operational graphic-conceptual models of a situation
have long since been the focal concern of specialists in ergonomics,
engineering psychology and information display technology. At the same
time, the content of this area of problems has undergone appreciable
changes in the last 15 years. Research on the speed of perceptual
processes, in particular, of information retrieval, has shifted to
the background. Significant refinement of the quality of displaying
information resulted in a d~ecrease in number of studies dealing with the
unequivocal nature of perception of symbolic and alphanumeric information.
Much more clarity has been obtained in understanding of the technical-
operational aspect of perceptual and cognitive processes. However, all
this did not minimize the urgency of studying the routes for the design
of information models and formation af conceptua~ models. The roots of
these problems pertain to the very essence of the activity of operators
of ASL's. In this type of activity, there is pertiaps considerably more
prominence than in others of a certain disproportion between the paucity
of reflection and richness, complexity and multistratal nature of re-
flection of reality that man must reconstruct, analyze and use in
accordance with the decision he has made. And in spite of the rapid
development of display technology, this disproportion persists (if it
does not increase with growth in scale and complexity of ASU's). Per-
sistence of this disproportion leads to a change in the problems of
perceptual and thinking processes that are studied.
Since the operator is dealing, more and more, with insufficiently defined
space of possible problems, it often happens that he must retrieve, glean
from the information model and, accordingly, reconstruct the most diverse
194
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
I
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
objective content, different strata of reality. These strata may be
- external, characterizing for example the spatial arrangement of objects
or isolated properties thereof; they taay characterize the general func-
tional properties of groups of objects, or functional (rather than
only time and space) relations between different objects; finally, there
may be situations that require working with systems of more or less
interrelated categorial properties and qualities of objects, rather
than witt~ these objects proper.
Consideration of these circumstances, in which real operator work takes
place, requires more intensive study than before of the motivational,
_ goal-oriented in the broad sense, and personal aspects of perceptual
and cognitive activity.
The sequence and possible depth to which an operator delves into a
situation, its layers that are not directly visible, its meaning and
importance are of considerable scientific and practical interest. Here,
- such a feature as time of penetration into these layers, time of con-
struction of a GCM, which of necessity is special and, in a certain sense,
biased, is also important. The time of enlarging upon the model or .
replacing it is also important. But, perhaps, what is the most �
essential is determination of orientation towarc~ some o~jective content.
- The latter ~s determined by both the individualrs problems and the
objective content and, of c~~urse, means of retrieving and transforming -
its significance. This combinativn of circumstances leads to evolution
(or change) of the GCM, i.e., evolution of cognitive products of activity,
to a change in image of the situation, to setting new goals. Of course,
the real ob~ect, its real objective content that determines the subject's
action, is the main factor of this combination. At the same time, one
should not underestimate the possible (and perhaps mandatory) effect
of "reading into" the object the a priori experience and knowledge of
the sub~~~t. The latter requires particularly attentive consideration
of individual differences between people~, of their possible preference
for some strata of reality or other.
What we have stated about the objective content of operator activity
confirms the thesis of its "dematerialization." This thesis should be -
interpreted in the sense that, at each given moment of his activity,
the operator does not have an a priori conception of its concrete, _
objective content. He must retrieve it from the superfluous informati.on
model, construct the image of this objective content and, on this basis,
set and reach concrete goals.
For expressly this reacon, operator work is sometimes called creative, and
for expressly this reason it is so difficult to evaluate the efficiency
of operator performance in MMS's, just as it is to solve pressing prob-
lems of optimization and planning of operator work.
195 -
FOR OF~ICIAL USE ONLY
'
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAT, USE ONLY
Experience in developing and operating information models, as well as
special analysis of operator work with them, enable us to formulate
several of the most important features of information models.
~ l. The information model represents only the properties, relations,
- connections of controlled objects that are essential, that have a -
certain functional meaning, i.e., "which participate in the game." In
this sense, the model reproduces reality in a simplified form, and it is
always an idealization, to some extent, of reality. The degree and
nature of simplification and idealization can be determined on the basis
of analysis of I~IIrIS problems as a whole and analysis of the operator
problems.
2. The model must ba graphic, i.e., the operator must be able to receive
information rapidly and without painstaking analy~is. It is only under
such conditions that he will not require much ti;ne for information p~ocessing
of a decision, which includes the stages of formation of the GCM and, when
. necessary, format3on of a madel of the problem situation. The information
model can be graphic in different meanings. It can, for example, provide
a graphic idea about the spatial location of objects, i.e., it can be
- geometrically similar, to some extent, to their actual arrangement. In
this case, the operator will have a graphic idea about such properties
of controlled objects as the distance between them, their reference to .
a given territorial group, etc. If other signs are important to the
- operator, other properties of controlled objects must be presented
graphically, for example, their reference to the same type or state.
When the system is in operation, there can be periods when graphic
idea is needed of some properties of controlled objects and periods when
other properties have to be_considered. _
It is not always easy to obtain a graphic information model, since there
are not uncommon instances when the ob~ects of control, their properties
and interactions do not in themselves have graphic features. In such
cases, one has.to solve problems that are simil.sr to what is defined in
scientif ic methodology as visualization of concepts.
3. One of the most important means of obtaining easy perceptibility,
or "readability" of an information model is proper organization of its
structure. This means that it is not collections or a set of informa-
tion that is put in some order or other that must be represented in the
information model, but that they must be in a specific and obvious
interaction.
With a"good" structure or gestalt of an information model, the operator _
performs ordinary functions; impairment of "good" structure is indicative
of occurrence of deviations from normal operating mode, which require
immediate intervention of the operator. A good structure provides for
rapid and correct perception of the situation as a whole. Deviations
- 196
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
Erom it are perceived by the operator as potential problems or conflicts,
- and compell him to make a detailed analysis of the situation in order to
detect the source of conflict and find the means of eliminating it. One
of the means of achieving a good structure is proper arrangement of the
information model. In this sense, development of the information model
is a task that is equivalen~, to some degree, to the task of good
layout of a painting. Just like a well-composed painting, the informa-
tion model can help perceive the situation as a whole, if it is not
overloaded with details that impair integral perception. An important
task for an artist is to select what is essential and typical, what
enables him to convey his idea to a viewer with utmost effectiveness.
In the very same way, when creating an information model, it is extremely
essential to select functionally significant information and informative
data [hat must be presented to the operator. This applies equally to
reflection of conflict situations, awareness of which is made easier
upon encounter of contradictory images, tendencies, properties, etc.
4. It is easier to perceive a situation as being problematic if the
following is provided in the information model: Reflection of concrete
changes in properties of situation elements, which occur when they
interact; in such cases, the changes in properties of individual elements
are not perceived separately, but in the context of the situation as a
whole; moreover, a change in properties of one element is perceived as
a symptom of change in the situation as a whole, and this prompts a
- search and identification by the operator of a given set of symptoms;
reflection of dynamic relations of controlled objects, where the
relations and interactions must be reflected in the information model
as they develop; it is permissible and useful to also have an exaggerated _
or amplified reflection of trends in development of situation elements, _
their relations or the situation as a whole; reflection of conflicts
- into which enter the elements of the situation.
S. Information about objects of control is not displayed to the operator
in its natural form, but it is coded. The problem of creating a
special language, understandable to man and, at the same time, usable
by the machine, the problem of matching human and machine "inputs" and
"outputs" becomes a particularly important one.
When constructing an information model, it is imperative to find the
most effective code, i.e., the system of symbols (which we shall call
"alphabet" of the code in question), with which information is presented
about controlled objects. The choice of coding system is closely
related to the possibility of rapid interpretation of information presented
to the ogerator.
6. The volume of information of one kind or another that can be well-
assimilated by the operator cannot be arbitrarily set. It must be deter-
mined for specific working conditions or already on the basis of existing
- 197
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPR~VED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
quantitative evaluations of operat~r work, or by means of a special experi-
ment. If this volume of information has been determined, along with the
chosen coding system, it helps form an idea about the degree of
complexity of the in�ormation model that is permissible under given
- conditions.
The degree of complexity of an information model is deternined chiefly
by the requirement of dynamic function. -
The above description of properties of information models does not pre-
sume to be complete. The properties of information models that we have
mentioned can be considered in the course of a specific design pro~ect
to different extents, depending on the prevailing operator function
(detection, retrieval, problem solving, execution, etc.).
What we have stated above concerning the properties of information models .
_ applies equally to instances when all of the main features of the models
are determined at the MMS designing stages and when operators have con-
siderably greater freedom in wor::ing with data stored in computer
memory, and themselves participa~~ in c~nstruction of the information model.
Thus, when constructing an informatian model for a control system, one
must take very many factors into consideration. Of course, we cannot
presently mention all of the specifications that must be considered
in designing and constructing information models. However, even now,
we can suggest the following procedure for such conscruction:
1) Determination of problems for the system and order in which
they are solved
2) Determination of sources of information, methods of solving
problems, time spent to solve them, as well as req.uired
accuracy
3) Listing the types of ob~ects for control, determination of
their number and other parameters of system operation, which
must be considered when solving problems
4) Making lists of features of control objects of different
types, consideration of which is necessary to problem solving
5) Distribution of objects and tags according to importance;
choice of cratical objects and tags which must be considered
- first of all _
6) Distribution of functions between machines and operators and,
in particular, determination of the following: number of levels
_ of control and degree of, complexity of each of them so that the
carrying capacity of operators is not exceeded on each level;
types of information models on each level; automatic equipment
needed with the planned structure of the system.
198 -
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/48: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200094451-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
In a number of instances, the first stegs of the process of system desi~n-
ing must be repeated several times in order to come successively closer to
the optimum variant taking into consideration the economic aspect of
construction 4f the system.
= After the first stages of work to design a system have been completed,
~ one can turn to the next ones: -
7) Choice of coding system for control objects, their states and
tags for information models on different levels of control, that
is optimal from the standpoint of functional capabilities of
operators working in the system
8) developmeiit of general composition of information models that
would provide for predominant distinction of the most important
ob;ects, and states and tags that are critical to system
operation
9) Determination of the system of executory actions of operato~s,
which must be performed during the solving process and after it
(request for information, transmission of reports, instructions,
etc.)
10) Development of a mock-up of the game situation and testing on it
of the effectiveness of the chosen variants of information
models and information coding systems. Time and accuracy of
operator work, which must conform with the conditions of
proper operation of the system as a whole, serve as the criterion
of efficiency when working on:the mock-up
11) Change in composition of information models and coding systems
on the basis of results of experiments, and testing the effi-
~ ciency of each new variant on the mock-up
12) Determination on the mock-up of the required degree of operator
training, methods of training and optimum mode of operator work
_ in the control system, in accordance with the speed and accuracy
requirements of operator work
13) Preparation of inetructions for operator work in the game
control system
After selecting and testing the optimum variant of information model and
information coding system, one can begin to work on the engineering
design of display devices, that permit presentation of information in the
required form to the operator. The same applies to logical-infaYmation
machines, for which one must prepare algorithms for information pror_es~sing
and rendering it in a form that assures perception on a high operational
level.
At all of the stages of work pertaining to the design of information
models, specialists in several fields related to development of control '
systems must work together: systems analysts, specialists in research on
operations, mathematics and developers of display devices, engineering
psychologists and ergonomists.
199
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2007/02/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
APPROVED FOR RELEASE: 2047102/08: CIA-RDP82-00850R000200090051-4
FOR OFFICIAL USE ONLY
The procedure suggested above is only general in outline. It can change,
depending on the specif ics of partir,ular control systems or differences
in operator functions within the same control system. Much of what we
are dealing with here is considered intuitively wher. developing control �
systems, but usually far from adequately.
2. Spatial Characteristics of Visual Inforniation
Three groups of factors are considered in designing and operating means
of display: 1) iocation of displays in the wark place and operations rooms;
2) optimal dimensions of symbols and their elements in different display
systems; 3) optimum arrangement of symbols on display devices.
Arrangement of display devices in operations room: The arrangement of
displays in the observer's field of vision should take into consideration
optimum angles of vision and zones for the observer.
With regard to objects with complex configuration, as well as perception
of a three-dimensional images and those in perspective, the optimum angle
of vision in relation to the horizontal plane is 30-40�. To perceive
a flat image, as compared to a simple sybmolic display, an angle of vision
of 50-60� is recommended, which covers the region of vague discrimina-
tion of shape (within this