PERCEPTUAL AUGMENTATION TECHNIQUES PART TWO: RESEARCH REPORT
Document Type:
Collection:
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST):
CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Release Decision:
RIFPUB
Original Classification:
S
Document Page Count:
155
Document Creation Date:
November 4, 2016
Document Release Date:
October 24, 2002
Sequence Number:
1
Case Number:
Publication Date:
February 1, 1975
Content Type:
REPORT
File:
Attachment | Size |
---|---|
CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7.pdf | 7.59 MB |
Body:
proved For Release 2003/03/10 :CIA-F7000400030001-7
Final Report
Covering the Period January 1974 through February 1975
PERCEPTUAL AUGMENTATION TECHNIQUES
Part Two: Research Report
By: HAROLD E. PUTHOFF and RUSSELL TARG
Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory
Classification Determination Pending.
Protect as though classified SECRET.
Certain individual pages of this document are UNCLASSIFIED. However, the association and com-
pilation of this material may indicate defense information, the unauthorized disclosure of which
could reasonably be expected to cause serious damage to the national security; hence, an overall
classification of SECRET is warranted.
P ry
r
This document consists of 153 pages. Copy No ....................
STANFORD RESEARCH INSTITUTE
Menlo Park, California 94025 ? U.S.A.
proved For Release 2003/03/10: CIA-FSECRcE9"F000400030001-7
Covering the Period January 1974 through February 1975
Part Two--Research Report
By: Harold E. Puthoff and Russell Targ
Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory
SRI Project 3183
Classification Determination Pending.
Protect as though classified SECRET.
Approved by:
Earle Jones, Director
Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory
Bonnar Cox, Executive Director
Information Science and Engineering Division
Certain individual pages of this document are UNCLASSIFIED. However,
the association and compilation of this material may indicate defense
information, the unauthorized disclosure of which could reasonably be
expected to cause serious damage to the national security; hence, an
overall classification of SECRET is warranted.
SECRET
Approved For Release 2003/ 3/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003IC:RIE-qbP79-00999A000400030001-7
I ABSTRACT
II PROGRAM RESULTS--APPLIED RESEARCH EFFORT
A. Remote Viewing
1. Long-Distance Remote Viewing: Sponsor-Designated Targets
(Exploratory Research)
a. West Virginia Site (S3)
b. West Virginia Site (Si)
c. Urals Site (Sl)
d. Summary of Exploratory Research
2. Long-Distance Remote Viewing: Sponsor-Designated Target
(Operational Target, Semipalatinsk, USSR)
3. Long-Distance Remote Viewing: SRI-Designated Targets
(Exploratory Research, Costa Rica Series)
4. Short-Distance Remote Viewing (Cipher Machine Analog)
5. Short-Distance Remote Viewing (Technology Series)
B. Detection of Secret Writing Target Material
Screening Tests
Page
iii
15
17
1. Remote Viewing of Natural Targets Under Standard Protocol 36
Conditions
a. Subject Sl (Experienced) 38
b. Subject S4 (Learner/Controls) 40
c. Subjects S2 and S3 (Experienced) 46
d. Subjects S5 and S6 (Learner/Controls) 49
e. Sponsor Subjects (Learner/Controls) 49
f. Summary of Remote-Viewing Experiments (Standard 53
Protocol) 66
2. Four-State Electronic Random Number Generator
a. Machine Description 67
b. Data from Experiments 69
B. Identification of Measurable Characteristics Possessed by 76
Gifted Subjects
Approved For Release 2003 1C:RlE TDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasUMc0'3I1ASS1"E 0P99A000400030001-7
2. Psychological. Evaluation
a. Evaluation by Clinical Psychologist Administering Tests
b. Evaluation by Chief Clinical Psychologist
3. Neuropsychological Evaluation
79
_80
83
88
C. Identification of Neurophysiological Correlates That Relate 94
to Paranormal Activity
1. Remote Strobe Experiment 97
2. Mid-Experiment Monitoring of Physiological Parameters During 103
Routine Experimentation in Remote Viewing
D.
Identification of the Nature of Paranormal Phenomena and Energy 106
1. Experiments with Physical Apparatus
106
a.
Experiments
with
Geiger Counter
106
b.
Experiments
with
Laser-Monitored Torsion Pendulum
107
c.
Experiments
with
Superconducting Differential Mag-
108
netometer (Gradiometer)
d.
Discussion of Physical Perturbation Effects
113
2. Discussion of Possible "Mechanisms" in Remote Viewing
117
3. Communication Theory Approach to Channel Utilization
121
4. Soviet Efforts
129
5. Conclusions
130
IV PROGRAM SUMMARY
REFERENCES
APPENDICES
A. Remote Viewing Transcript for Subject S6, Learner/Control,
First Experiment':
B. Instructions to Subject: EEG Experiment
C. Universal Randomization Protocol
Approved For ReleaU N e /A l FED 999A000400030001-7
*0 60
Approved For Releamp2p(:/p3AQc @Iq,tlll6~p0999A000400030001-7 fto
1. Maps of West Virginia Site Drawn by Subject S3
2. Map and Detail of Site Drawn by Subject Si
3. Artist's Rendering of Site
4. Costa Rica Site Drawings and Photographs
5. Abacus/Clock Drawings and Photograph
6. Video Terminal
7. Typewriter
8. Xerox Machine
9. Drill Press
10. Procedure for Card Sorting by Sequential Sampling
11. Swimming Pool Complex (Sl)
12. Pedestrian Overpass (S4)
13. Bicycle Shed (S4)
14. Tennis Court (S2)
15. Palo Alto City Hall (S3)
16. White's Plaza, Stanford University (S6)
17. Merry-Go-Round
18. Windmill
19. Four-State Random Number Generator Used in This Experiment
20. Data Summary for Subject S2
21. Drawings and Interpretations by Associative Visual Agnosia Patients
22. Shielded Room Used for EEG Experiments
23. Occipital EEG Frequency Spectra of Subject S4
24. Polygraph Data from Subject S4
25. Superconducting Differential Magnetometer (Gradiometer)
26. Gradiometer Data
27. Enhancement of Signal-to-Noise Ratio by Sequential Sampling Procedure
28. Operating Characteristic Curve for Sequential Sampling Procedure
29. Average Sample Number for Sequential Sampling Procedure
Approved For ReleINC/L36 SS4-F4Ii'D0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release ,2Q.O Q3/A 1.CAi ff fSQ0999A000400030001-7
1. Subject, Target Selection Procedure, and Figure Numbers for Tech-
no1=.~gy Series.
2. Rank. Ordering Match of Drawings to Target Locations (Blind Judging,
Te(inology Series)
3. Critical Values of Sums of Ranks for Preferential Matching
4. The Probability of M Correct Guesses of N Distinct Items
5. Key for Secret Writing Experiment
6. Raw Data Call Sheet for Secret Writing Experiment
7. Distribution of Rankings of Transcripts Associated with Each Target
Selection for Subject Si, Experienced.
8. Distribution of Rankings of Transcripts Associated with Each Target
Location for Subject S4, Learner/Control
9. Distribution of Rankings of Transcripts Associated with Each Target
Location for Subjects S2 and S3, Experienced
10. Distribution of Rankings of Transcripts Associated with Each Target
Location for Subjects S5 and S6, Learner/Control
11. Distribution of Rankings of Transcripts Associated with Each Target
Location for Sponsor Subjects, Learner/Control
12. Summary--Remote Viewing of Natural Targets
13. Four-State Electronic Random Number Generator Summary
14, Randomness Tests for Machine Ml Output during Successful Experimental
Series
15. Randomness Tests for Machine M2 Output during Successful Experimental
Series
1.6. Randomness Tests for Entire Machine Output During Successful Experi-
mental Run
17. Subject S2 Selections on Machine Ml during Successful Experimental
Series
18. Subject S2 Selections on Machine M2 during Successful Experimental
Series
19. Neuropsychology Test Battery
20. EEG Data for Subject S4
21. Geiger Counter Experiment Summary
22. Five-Bit Code for Alphanumeric Characters
Approved For Relea _CI A Z4'F FEgMD999A000400030001-7
Approved For Re1eCf3N STffb00999A000400030001-7
As a result of exploratory research on human perception carried out
in SRI's Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory, we initiated a program
to investigate a perceptual channel whereby individuals can access by
means of mental imagery and describe randomly chosen sites remote from
their physical location. This ability appeared to be sufficiently well
developed in certain individuals to allow them to at times describe cor-
rectly--often in great detail--geographical or technical material, such
as buildings, roads, laboratory apparatus, and the like. In this final
report (Part Two--Research Reportt), we document in detail the 12-month
study at SRI of this human information-accessing capability which we call
"remote viewing," the characteristics of which appear to fall outside the
range of well-understood perceptual or information-processing abilities.
This phenomenon is one of a broad class of abilities of certain indivi-
duals to access, by means of mental processes, and describe information
sources blocked from ordinary perception and generally accepted as secure
against access.
The phenomenon we investigated most extensively was the ability of
individuals to view remote geographical locations (up to several thousand
kilometers away), given only coordinates (latitude and longitude) or a
person at a location on whom to target. The development at SRI of suc-
cessful experimental procedures to elicit this capability has evolved
to the point where (a) visiting personnel of the sponsoring organization
without any previous exposure to such concepts have performed well under
controlled laboratory conditions (that is, generated target descriptions
of sufficiently high quality to permit blind matching of descriptions to
targets by independent judges), and (b) subjects trained over a two-year
period have performed well under operational conditions (that is, provided
data of operational significance later verified by independent sources).
Our accumulated data thus indicate that both specially selected and un-
selected persons can be assisted in developing remote perceptual abilities
to a level of useful information transfer. The primary achievement of the
SRI program was thus the elicitation of high-quality remote viewing by
For summary, see Part One--Executive Summary
Approved For Rele e/1,13A.Os94-FI IED0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea /?E/A:Sg ff I 0999A000400030001-7
individuals who agreed to act as subjects.
In carrying out this program we concentrated on what we considered
to be our primary responsibility--to resolve under unambiguous conditions
the basic issue of whether this class of paranormal perception phenomenon
exists. At all times the researchers and SRI management took measures
to prevent sensory leakage and subliminal cueing, and to prevent deception,
whether intentional or unintentional. All experiments were carried out
under protocols in which target selection at the beginning of experiments
and blind judging of results at the end of experiments were handled inde-
pendently of the researchers involved in carrying out the experiments,
thus assuring evaluations independent of the belief structures of both
experimenters and judges.
The program was divided into two categories of approximately equal
effort-?-applied research and basic research. In Section TI we summarize
the results of the applied research effort in which the operational
utility of the above perceptual abilities was explored. In Section ITT
we summarize the results of the basic research effort, which was directed
toward. identification of the characteristics of individuals possessing
such a.bi.lities and the determination of neurophysiol.ogical correlates
and basic mechanisms involved in such functioning. With an eye toward
selection of future subjects, individuals possessing a well-developed
natural ability in the area under investigation underwent complete
physical, psychological, and neuropsychological profiling, the results
of which suggest some hypotheses for developing a screening procedure.
The program summary is presented in Section IV.
With regard to understanding the phenomenon, the precise nature of
the information channel that couples remote locations is not yet under-
stood. However, its general characteristics are compatible with both
quantum theory and information theory as well as with recent developments
in research on brain function. Therefore, our working assumption is that
the phenomenon of interest does not lie outside the purview of modern
physics and with further- work will yield to analysis and specification.
Finally, it is concluded by the research contractor (SRI) that the
development of experimental procedures and the accrual of experience in
Approved For Relea ibt/A l fbpLZPP999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaJN I43AS HFE00999A000400030001-7
three years of successful effort constitutes an asset that could be
utilized in the future, both for operational needs and for training others
in the development and use of the remote-sensing capability.
A. Remote Viewing
As mentioned in the Abstract, the phenomenon we investigated most
extensively was the ability of individuals to view remote geographical
locations (up to several thousand kilometers away), given only coordinates
(latitude and longitude) or a person at a location on whom to target.
Individuals exhibiting this faculty include not only SRI participants
but also visiting staff members of the sponsoring organization who par-
ticipated as subjects so as to critique the protocol.
As observed in the laboratory, the basic phenomenon appears to cover
a range of subjective experiences variously referred to in the literature
as autoscopy (in the medical literature); exteriorization or disassociation
(psychological literature); simple clairvoyance, traveling clairvoyance,
or out-of-body experience (parapsychological literature); or astral pro-
jection (occult literature). We choose the term "remote viewing" as a
neutral descriptive term free of occult assumptions or-bias as to the
mechanisms involved.
We begin our report in subsections 1 and 2 with experiments under
the control of the sponsor. These experiments were designed to provide
a vehicle whereby the sponsor could establish independently of SRI, some
degree of confidence as to the existence of the long-distance remote
viewing faculty.
1. Long-Distance Remote Viewing: Sponsor-Designated Targets
(Exploratory Research)
So as to subject the remote-viewing phenomena to a rigorous
long-distance test under external control, a request for geographical
coordinates of a site unknown to subject and experimenters was forwarded
to the sponsor's group responsible for threat analysis in this area. In
response, an SRI experimenter received a set of coordinates identifying
Approved For ReleaU UCLA M0999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasUf 1A SS I r l E D'4999A000400030001-7
what is hereafter referred to as the West Virginia Site. The SRI experi-
menter then carried out remote-viewing experiments with two subjects on
a double-blind basis, that is, with target content blind to experimenter
as well as to subjects. (Following the experiment both subjects claimed
unfamiliarity with the West Virginia area.) The experiment had as its
goal the determination of the utility of remote viewing under conditions
approximating an operational scenario.
a. West Virginia Site (S3)*
Date: 29 May 1973, 1634 to 1640 hours, Menlo Park, Cali-
fornia. Protocol: Coordinates 38?23'45"'to 48"N, 79?25'00"W, described
simply as being in West Virginia, were relayed to experimenter Dr. H.E.
Puthoff by telephone, who then relayed this information to subject S3
to initiate experiment. No maps were permitted, and the subject was
asked to give an immediate response. The session was recorded on video
tape. The oral response is reproduced here from the tape:
This seems to be some sort of mounds or rolling hills. There
is a city to the north (I can see the taller buildings and some
smug), This seems to be a strange place, somewhat like the
lawns that one would find around a military base, but I get
the impression that there are either some old hunkers around,
or maybe this is a covered reservoir. There must be a flagpole,
some highways to the west, possibly a river over to the far east,
to the south more city.
The map in Figure l(a) was drawn by the subject.
On the fallowing morning, S3 submitted a written report
of a second reading, dated 30 May 1973, 0735 to 0758 hours, Mountain
View, California:
Cliffs to the east, fence to the north. There's a circular
buiading (a tower?), buildings to the south. Is this a former
Nike base or something like that? This is about as far as I
could go without feedback, and perhaps guidance as to what was
wanted. There is something strange about this area, but since
I can't know particularly what to look for within the scope of
the cloudy ability, it is extremely difficult to make decisions
on what is there and what. is not. Imagination seems to get in
the way. (For example, I seem to get the impression of something
S3 identifies a subject. A key to numerical designations for subjects
is available from the sponsor's Contracting Officer Technical Repre-
sentative (COT[.).
Approved For ReleaI4 Nc/A pE9 ft999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
'iew S4 1 ".
L~011 =W13
I 6
FIGURE 1 MAPS OF WEST VIRGINIA SITE DRAWN BY SUBJECT S3
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releau ffefA Prt1b0999A000400030001-7
underground, but I'm not sure.) However, it is apparent that
on first sighting, the general location was correctly spotted.
The map in Figure 1(b) also was drawn by the subject.
b. West Virginia Site (Sl)
As a backup test, the coordinates were given to a second
subject, Sl. The task was presented to the second subject independently
of the first subject, both to prevent collaboration and to prevent any
sense of competition.
Date: 1 June 1973, 1700 hours, Menlo Park, California.
Protocol: Coordinates 38?23'45" to 48"N, 79?25'00"W were given (with no
further description) by experimenter Dr. H.E. Puthoff to subject S1 by
telephone to initiate experiment.
On the morning of 4 June 1973, S1's written response
(dated 2 June 1973, 1250 to 1350 hours, Lake Tahoe, California) was re-
ceived in the mail:
Looked at general area from altitude of about 1500 ft above
highest terrain. On my left forward quadrant is a peak in a
chain of mountains, elevation approximately 4996 ft above sea
level. Slopes are greyish slate covered with variety of broad-
leaf trees, vines, shrubbery, and undergrowth. I am facing
about 3?to 5? west of north. Looking down the mountain to the
right (east) side is a roadway--freeway, country style--curves
then heads ENE to a fairly large city about 30 to 40 miles
distant. This area was a battleground in civil war--low rolling
hills, creeks, few lakes or reservoirs. There is a smaller
town a little SE about 15 to 20 miles distant with small
settlements, village type, very rural, scattered around. Look-
ing across the peak, 2500 to 3000 ft mountains stretch out for
a hundred or so miles. Area is essentially wooded. Some of
the westerly slopes are eroded and gully washed--looks like
strip mining, coal mainly.
Weather at this time is cloudy, rainy. Temperature at my
altitude about 54?F--high cumulo nimbus clouds to about 25,000
to 30,000 ft. Clear area, but turbulent, between that level and
some cirro stratus at 46,000 ft. Air mass in that strip moving
WNW to SE.
1318 hours--Perceived that peak area has large underground
storage areas. Road comes up back side of mountains (west
slopes), fairly well concealed, looks deliberately so. It's
cut under trees where possible--would be very hard to detect
flying over area. Looks like former missile site--bases for
launchers still there, but area now houses record storage area,
microfilm, file cabinets; as you go into underground area
Approved For Relea 3/ ~0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003SE:RIE-DP79-00999A000400030001-7
through aluminum rolled up doors, first areas filled with
records, etc. Rooms about 100-ft long, 40-ft wide, 20-ft
ceilings, with concrete supporting pilasters, flare-shaped.
Temperature cool--fluorescent lighted. Personnel, Army 5th
Corps Engineers. M/Sgt. Long on desk placard on grey steel
desk--file cabinets security locked--combination locks, steel
rods through eye bolts. Beyond these rooms, heading east,
are several bays with computers, communication equipment,
large maps, display type, overlays. Personnel, Army Signal
Corps. Elevators.
1330 hours--Looked over general area from original location
again--valleys quite hazy, lightning about 30 miles north
along mountain ridge. Tempterature drop about 6?F, it's about
48?F. Looking for other significances: see warm air mass
moving in from SW colliding with cool air mass about 100 miles
ESE from my viewpoint. Air is very turbulent--tornado type;
birds in my area seeking heavy cover. There is a fairly large
river that I can see about 15 to 20 miles north and slightly
west; runs NE then curves in wide valley running SW to NE;
river then runs SE. Area to east: low rolling hills. Quite
a few Civil War monuments. A marble colonnade type: 'In this
area was fought the battle of Lynchburg where many brave men of
the Union and Confederate Armys (sic) fell. We dedicate this
area to all peace loving people of the future--Daughters G.A.R.'
On a later date Sl was asked to return to the West Virginia
site with the goal of obtaining information on code words, if possible.
In response, Sl supplied the following information:
Top of desk had papers labeled "Flytrap" and "Minerva".
File cabinet on north wall labeled "Operation Pool..."
(third word unreadable).
Folders inside cabinet labeled "Cueball", "14 Ball", "4
Ball", "8 Ball", and "Rackup".
Name of site vaguely seems like Hayfork or Haystack. Per-
sonnel: Col. R.J. Hamilton, Maj. Gen. George R. Nash, Major John C.
Calhoun (??).
c. Urals Site (S1)
After obtaining a reading on the West Virginia Site, Sl
volunteered that he had scanned the other side of the globe for a Communist
Bloc equivalent and found one located in the Urals at 65?00'57"N, 59?
59'59"E, described as follows:
Elevation, 6200 ft. Scrubby brush, tundra-type ground hummocks,
rocky outcroppings, mountains with fairly steep slopes. Facing
Approved For Release 20031 6! RF- DP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 200310$EGRETP79-00999A000400030001-7
north, about 60 miles ground slopes to marshland. Mountain
chain runs off to right about 35? east of north. Facing south,
mountains run fairly north and south. Facing west, mountains
drop down to foothills for 60 miles or so; some rivers running
roughly north. Facing east, mountains are rather abrupt,
dropping to rolling hills and to flat land. Area site under-
ground, reinforced concrete, doorways of steel of the roll-up
type. Unusually high ratio of women to men, at least at night.
I see some helipads, concrete. Light rail tracks run from pads
to another set of rails that parallel the doors into the moun-
tain. Thirty miles north (5? west of north) of the site is a
radar installation with one large (165 ft) dish and two small
fast-track dishes.
The two reports for the West Virginia Site, and the report
for the Urals Site were verified by personnel in the sponsor organization
as being substantially correct. The results of the evaluation are con-
tained in a separate report filed with the COTR.
d. Summary of-Exploratory Research
The observation of such unexpectedly high-quality descrip-
tions early in our program led to a large-scale study of the phenomenon
under secure double-blind conditions (i.e., target unknown to experimenters
as well as subjects), with independent random target selection and blind
judging. The results, presented later, provide strong evidence for the
robustness of this phenomenon, one whereby complex remote stimuli can be
detected by a human perceptual. modality of extreme sensitivity. Before
discussing these results, however, we consider further examples of both
operational and operational-analog experiments.
2. Long-Distance Remote Viewing: Sponsor-Designae Target (Oper-
ational Target--Semipalatinsk, USSR)
To determine the utility of remote viewing under operational
conditions, a long-distance remote viewing experiment was carried out on
a sponsor-designated target of current interest, an unidentified research
center at Semipalatinsk, USSR.
This experiment, carried out in three phases, was under direct
control of the COTR. To begin the experiment, the COTR furnished map
coordinates in. degrees, minutes, and seconds. The only additional infor-
mation provided was the designation of the target as an R&D test facility.
The experimenters then closeted themselves with subject Sl, gave him the
Approved For Release 2003/0S'EcREITP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/(9fC. MP79-00999A000400030001-7
map coordinates and indicated the designation of the target as an R&D test
facility. A remote-viewing experiment on the target was then carried out.
This activity constituted Phase I of the experiment.
Figure 2(a) shows the subject's graphic effort for building
layout; Figure 2(b) shows the subject's particular attention to a multi-
story gantry crane he observed at the site. Both results were obtained
by the experimenters on a double-blind basis before exposure to any addi-
tional COTR-held information, thus eliminating the possibility of cueing.
These results were turned over to the client representatives for evalua-
tion. For comparison an artist's rendering of the site as known to the
COTR(but not to the experimenters until later) is shown in Figure 3(a),
with crane detail shown in Figure 3(b).
Were the results not promising, the experiment would have stopped
at this point. Description of the multistory crane, however, a relatively
unusual target item, was taken as indicative of possible target acquisi-
tion. Therefore, Phase II was begun, defined by the subject being made
"witting" (of the client) by client representatives who introduced them-
selves to the subject at that point; Phase II also included a second
round of experimentation on the Semipalatinsk site with direct participa-
tion of client representatives in which further data were obtained and
evaluated. As preparation for this phase, client representatives purposely
kept themselves blind to all but general knowledge of the target site to
minimize the possibility of cueing. The Phase II effort was focused on
the generation of physical data that could be independently verified by
other client resources, thus providing a calibration of the process.
The end of Phase II gradually evolved into the first part of
Phase III, the generation of unverifiable data concerning the Semipalatinsk
site not available to the client, but of operational interest nonetheless.
Several hours of tape transcript and a notebook full of drawings were
generated over a two-week period.
The data describing the Semipalatinsk site were evaluated by
the sponsor, and are contained in a separate report. In general, several
details concerning the salient technology of the Semipalatinsk site ap-
peared to dovetail with data from other sources, and a number of specific
Approved For Release 2003/SEC R FTDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release of /eMs itV 99A000400030001-7
=CIQ
CI ate,
WOW
Ir
.w 10 I"
tM
(b) SUBJECT EFFORT AT CRANE CONSTRUCTION
FIGURE 2 MAP AND DETAIL OF: SITE DRAWN BY SUBJECT Si
10
Approved For Releas L5/LOA 099A000400030001-7
I L;
Approved For Release 200ECDP79-00999A000400030001-7
(b) CRANE COMPARISON
FIGURE 3
11
Approved For Release 2003SE?Rfi DP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasLIA:!JS-Wl'efy999A000400030001-7
Large structural elements were correctly described. The results contained
noise along with the signal, but were nonetheless clearly differentiated
from the chance results that were generated by control subjects in compar-
ison experiments carried out by the COTR.
Long-Distance Remote Viewing: SRI-Designated Targets (Explora-
t_oty Research. Costa Rica Series)
i'IW eexperimena procedures of Subsections 1 and 2 were designed
to provide a vehicle whereby the client could establish, independently of
:3 R1, some degree of confidence as to the existence of a long-distance re-
mote viewing faculty. Although the results were indicated to be positive,
from the standpoint of SRI personnel who could not participate in the
o vaLuation phase, it was considered necessary to supplement the above
aoxp eriments with a similar set under. SRI control. Therefore, SRI-controlled
experiments were undertaken to enable the experimenters to participate
directly in the evaluation phase of the remote-viewing experiments.
Two subjects (S1 and S4) were asked to participate in a long-
distance experiment focusing on a series of targets in Costa Rica. The
subjects said they had never been to Costa Rica.
In this experiment, one of the experimenters (Dr. Puthoff) spent
ten (lays traveling through Costa Rica on a combination business/pleasure
a:.rl_p. This information was all that was known to the subjects about the
raveler's itinerary. The experiment called for Dr. Yuthorf to keep a
detailed record of his location and activities, including photographs,
leach. of :-;even target days at 1330 PDT. A total of 12 daily descriptions
were coiiected before the traveler's return: six responses from Si, five
responses from 54, and one response from an SRI experimenter, wino acted
as a subject in one experiment on a day in which S4 was not available and
t_he other subject arrived late.
:'cat its illustrative value we consider first the single response
submitted by the experimenter filling in as a subject. The response, a
drawing submitted for a day in the middle of the series, is shown in
Figure 4 together with photographs taken at the site. Although Costa Rica
Ls a mountainous country. the subject unexpectedly perceived the traveler
at a bea:_h and ocean setting. With some misgivings, he described an
Approved For Releasb/:~9D999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea J N V/'A SCt ED0999A000400030001-7
AIRPORT IN SAN ANDRES, COLOMBIA, USED AS REMOTE VIEWING TARGET
FIGURE 4 AIRPORT IN SAN ANDRES, COLOMBIA, USED AS REMOTE VIEWING TARGET ALONG
WITH SKETCH PRODUCED BY SUBJECT IN CALIFORNIA
Approved For ReleaUN LA:SS-IctE-D999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release fi%UG:V1Jk: fORM-LQ999A000400030001-7 ric
airport on a sandy beach and an airstrip with the ocean at the end (correct).
An airport building also was drawn, and shown to have a large rectangular
L'verhanv, (correct). the traveler had taken a one-day unplanned side trip
i..o an oifshore island and at the time of the experiment had just disem-
barked from a plane at a small island airport as described, 4000 kilometers
from SRI. The sole discrepancy was that the drawing showed a Quonset-hut
type of building in place of the rectangular structure.
the above description was chosen as an example to illustrate two
Major points observed a number of times throughout the program. First, in
opposition to what might be expected, a subject's description does not
n.ecessarily portray what might reasonably be expected to be correct (an
educated or "safe" guess) but often runs counter to even the subject's
own expectations. Second, individuals other than those with putative
anor-mal ability" are able to exhibit a remote viewing faculty.
The remaining submissions provided further examples of excellent
correspondences between target and response. (A target period of pool-
side relaxation was identified, a drive through a tropical forest at the
base of a truncated volcano was described as a drive through a jungle
below a large bare table mountain, a hotel room target description, in-
cluding such details as rug color, was correct, and so on.) So as to
determine whether such matches were simply fortuitous, i.e., could rea-
sonably be expected on tare basis of chance alone, when Dr. Puthoff re-
tturned he was asked to blind match the 12 descriptions to the seven
target locations. On the basis of this conservative evaluation procedure,
which vastly underestimates the statistical significance of the individual
descriptions, five correct matches were obtained (two each of subjects Sl
;nd S4, and the single submission by the experimenter). This number of
matches is significant at D = 0.02 by exact binomial calculation.*
The probability of a correct daily match by chance for any given tran-
script is p = 1/7. Therefore, the probability of at least five correct
matches by chance out of i2 tries can be calculated from
l2
1
999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releaste- ~/1A:S {~TO
Approved For ReleaUN j/1A:55ifT'ED999A000400030001-7
Therefore, this pilot study, completely under control of SRI, provided
confirmatory data supporting that obtained under sponsor control, indi-
cating the existence of an apparent long-distance remote viewing faculty.
4. Short-Range Remote Viewing(Cipher Machine Analog)
As a further test of operational utility of the remote viewing
faculty, the COTR tasked the contractors with an experiment designed to
duplicate as closely as possible an operational situation of current
interest, the remote viewing of an abacus-type device. During a trip
to the East Coast, the experimenters were to proceed to New York, where
they were to purchase locally an abacus to be used as a target in a
remote viewing experiment. (The abacus was to constitute a target anal-
ogous to a cipher machine of particular interest.) Following the purchase
they were to contact a subject who lived there (S3) by telephone with
a surprise request to come to the experimenters' hotel room later that
day to participate in a remote-viewing experiment.
The above steps were carried out in preparation for the experi-
ment. In this case the experimenters knew what the target was, an ex-
ception to the double-blind rule followed in all our other work. There-
fore, while awaiting the subject's arrival, a preamble for the experiment
was prerecorded by one of the experimenters (Targ) and carefully checked
to ensure against verbal cueing:
Hal and I have brought a present for you. We wandered around
New York this morning and we bought an object. This object is
of the type that one interacts with, and Hal will use it for
its normal purpose. Today is Friday, September 26, 1974. As
in all our remote viewing experiments, we'd like to ask you to
describe the object as you see it rather than attempting to
give the object a name.
When the subject entered the hotel room, this instruction tape
was played by one experimenter (R.T.) while the other experimenter (H.P.)
took a large locked suitcase containing the target object into an adjacent
room, locked the door, and removed the abacus, shown in Figure 5(a), actions
verified earlier as being inaudible. Thus the only available cue was an
upper bound on the size.
The subject produced the outline drawing I of Figure 5(b) in
approximately one minute. (The large purplish-silver object corresponds
Approved For Relea JN?f43S HFE00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
(a) ABACUS/CLOCK TARGET
(TECHNOLOGY SERIES)
(b) SUBJECT S3 RESPONSES I AND II
TO ABACUS/CLOCK TARGET
He Described Target as "Game Box
with Little Balls"
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaU U LA REWD0999A000400030001-7
to the suitcase interior and is not taken to be evidential.) The experi-
menter remaining with the subject asked for more detail, and the subject
produced the drawing II of Figure 5(b), describing the object as a "game
box with little balls." The entire experiment was tape recorded and
extreme caution was taken to prevent cueing of any kind. The experiment
took place in five minutes total time.
Considering the high-strangeness factor of the target item, and
essentially total lack of restriction on the possibilities as far as the
subject was concerned, the correlation of subject drawings and target was
taken as indicative of a potential utility for remote viewing of tech-
nological targets, and resulted in a decision to experiment further in
this area.
After the target was shown to the subject, a short follow-up
experiment was carried out to determine whether the position of the balls
on the abacus could be determined by remote viewing, but this degree of
resolution was found to be beyond the subject's capability.
5. Short-Range Remote Viewing (Technology Series)
So as to measure the resolution capability of the remote viewing
phenomenon, a series of experiments targeting on remote laboratory equip-
ment within the SRI complex was carried out.
Thirteen experiments were carried out with five different sub-
jects, two of whom were sponsor staff personnel. A subject was told that
one of the experimenters would be sent by random protocol to a laboratory
within the SRI complex and that he would interact with the equipment or
apparatus at the location. It was further explained that the experimenter
remaining with the subject was kept ignorant of the contents of the target
pool to prevent cueing during questioning. (The remaining experimenter
only knew that from time to time, on a random basis, previously used
targets would be reinserted into the target pool to provide an opportunity
for multiple responses to a given target, and that during sponsor visits
the targets might be selected by sponsor staff personnel rather than by
the established random protocol procedures.) The subject was asked to
describe the target both verbally (tape recorded) and by means of drawings
during the time-synchronized 15-minute interval in which the outbound
experimenter interacted in an appropriate manner with the equipment in the
Approved For ReleYer2UD AYA "CINA 00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releas / 999A000400030001-7
target area.
In the 13 experiments eight targets were used: a drill press,
comput^r-driven flight simulator (Link trainer), Xerox machine, video
terminal, chart recorder, four-state random target generator (used in
screening tests described Later), typewriter, and machine shop. Three
oL these were used twice (drill press, video terminal, and typewriter)
and one %Xerox machine) came up three times. As an example of drawings
generated by subjects, all of the subject outputs generated for the latter
three (video terminal, typewriter, and Xerox machine) are shown in Figures
6. 7, and 8. A summary of subject and target selection procedure is given
in Table I.
L ',s is apparent from the illustrations alone, certain of the ex-
periments provide circumstantial evidence for an information channel of
useful bit rate. This includes experiments (Experiments 4 and 13) in
which sponsor staff personnel participated as subjects to observe the
protocol.
1'u obtain independent objective judgment of the quality of
the remote viewing of technological targets, various analyses based on
blind judging were employed.
In the first judging procedure, a judge was asked to blind-match
the drawings alone (i.e., without tape transcripts) to the targets.
Multiple subject responses to a given target were stapled together, and
thus there were seven subiect-drawing response packets to be matched to
the seven different targets for which drawings were made. (No drawings
were made for the Link trainer.) The judge did not have access to our
puotograpns of the target locations, used for demonstration only (as in
Figures 6 through 8), but rather proceeded to each of the target locations
by list. While standing at each target location, the judge was required
to rank order the seven subject-drawing response packets (presented in
random order) on a scale one to seven (best to worst match), as shown
in Table 2. The statistic of interest is the sum of ranks on the diagonal,
lower values indicating better matches. For seven targets, the sum of
ranks could range from seven to forty-nine. The probability that it given
sum of ranks s or less will occur by chance is given by:
alt
Approved For Relea,((/3 SCE00999AO00400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
m
v
SUBJECT (S4) DRAWING OF "BOX WITH LIGHT COMING OUT SECOND SUBJECT (V2) SAW A COMPUTER TERMINAL
OF IT . . . PAINTED FLAT BLACK AND IN WITH RELAY RACKS IN THE BACKGROUND
THE MIDDLE OF THE ROOM"
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasUN to 95.II 399A000400030001-7
TECHNOLOGY SERIES
TYPEWRITER TARGET
s~vs f? Mso&e u4 a Ys
aw "tu% 0tap 11Lw odaI -
a me" 4, 2pw~s.
srcEl+ foeuow -C
'!4 9y4L w*d it ut;i
4 r aaccowt
FIGURE 7 DRAWINGS OF A TYPEWRITER TARGET BY TWO SUBJECTS
20
Approved For ReleasLW. 00 A%IJS4 D99A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
m
v
TARGET LOCATION: XEROX MACHINE
(TECHNOLOGY SERIES)
FIGURE 8 DRAWINGS BY THREE SUBJECTS (S2, S3, AND V3) FOR XEROX MACHINE TARGET
When subject (V3) was asked to describe the square at upper left, the subject said, "There
was this predominent light source which might have been a window, and a working surface
which might have been the sill, or a working surface or desk." Earlier the subject had said,
Approved IFS" 12Mgaf4Vi2lbOl3P0,4:'CIIR--WbVDp9aC MgA,b( M0f oXyO * . " Shown at right.
C
z
TO ADD INTEREST TO TARGET In
LOCATION EXPERIMENTER WITH 1
HIS HEAD BEING XEROXED
V9
N
T
v
Approved For ReleaTia,QQ/1Q :ff itB999A000400030001-7
Experiment
Target
Target
Selection
Procedure*
Subject
1
Link trainer
a
SRI-S4
2
Video terminal
a
SRI-S4
3
Drill press
a
SRI-S4
4
Xerox machine
b
Sponsor-Vl
5
Xerox machine
b
SRI-S2
6
Random number
c
SRI-S4
generator
7
Machine shop
b
SRI-S4
8
Typewriter
b
SRI-S4
9
Typewriter
c
SRI-S3
10
Chart recorder
c
SRI-S3
11
Xerox machine
c
SRI-S3
12
Drill press
c
SRI-S3
13
Video terminal
c
Sponsor-V2
*Target selection procedures
a. Outbound SRI experimenter selects target site arbitrarily after
leaving subject area.
b. Visiting sponsor staff member selects target site arbitrarily
after leaving subject area.
c. Standard protocol, in which a target is issued to outbound
experimenter by division director who selects the target by
random number technique from a target pool stored in a secure
safe.
Approved For ReleaU N(C3/'A 5CRYWc0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea IAQL A AFDP9?0999A000400030001-7
TO TARGET LOCATIONS (BLIND JUDGING, TECHNOLOGY SERIES)
Subj ect
Place Drawing
Visited Number
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
Video terminal
1
7
5
6
4
3
2
Random number
generator
4
6
5
7
2
3
1
Xerox machine
6
1
2
5
7
4
3.
Machine shop
5
2
1.
3
4
6
7
Drill press
7
3
5
1
2
4
6
Typewriter
3
7
5
6
1
2
4
Chart recorder
4
7
5
3
6
1
2
Note on judging procedure: When standing at target locations shown on
left, each of the seven subject-drawing response packets (originally
labeled in random order) are rank ordered one to seven (best to worst
match) by the judge. Statisitic of interest is the sum of ranks on the
diagonal, lower values indicating better matches (see text). The sum
in this case (18) is significant at p < 0.04.
Approved For Release X003/Q3/"1 Q K!V999-0999AO00400030001-7
Approved For ReleaseUNCILpI lf*D99A000400030001-7
Prob(s or less) = -1n--
N
s k
(n) (i-N l)
A =0 n-l
where s = obtained sum of ranks
N = number of assignable ranks
n = number of occasions on which rankings were made
takes on values from zero to the least positive integer k
in (i-n)/n.
Table 3 is a table to enable easy application of the above
formula to those cases in which N = n. The sum in this case (1.8) is
significant at p < 0.04.
In tie second judging procedure, another judge was given 12
subject-response packages, which included drawings and tape transcripts,
and asked to blind match, without replacement, the 12 response packages
to 12 target locations, which he visited. (The thirteenth location, the
machine shop, included in the first judging, was left out of this judging
by an oversight.) In the forced-choice matching without replacement,
(that is, each response packet used only once), the judge obtained four
direct hits, the Link trainer (Experiment 1), video terminal (Experiment
:2), drill press (Experiment 3) and Xerox machine (Experiment 5). (The
Link trainer, for which no drawings were made, was matched on the basis
of tape--recorded transcript alone. It is a standard computer-controlled
flight simulator that resembles the cockpit of an aircraft. It was
cramped quarters for tie outbound experimenter who flew the trainer ac-
cording to a printed flight plan book laid over his arm. The windows
were frosted and translucent. Subject S4 gave a description of the ex-
perimenter crowded into a very small space illuminated by gray diffuse
light and with a long paper, such as a waiter's towel, over his arm.
Although not unambiguous, the subject's description was nevertheless
essentially devoid of incorrect statements.)
As indicated by Table 4, the probability of obtaining by chance
four direct hits out of 12 matches is p = 0.015; thus, this judging pro-
cedure also indicates that, from a statistical standpoint, there is
significant evidence of useful information transfer.
In a third judging procedure the COTR arbitrarily selected the
Approved For Relea LILl&.,S x-kFJ-E-999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
TABLE 3
Critical Values of Sums of Ranks for Preferential Matching
C
z
Ir
Number of
Assignable
Ranks (N)
Probability (One-Tailed) that the Indicated Sum of Ranks or Less Would Occur by Chance
----- } - ~' - _ _
0.20 0.10 0.05 0.,04 ,0.025 0.01 0.005 0.002 0.001fi3.0005 10-4 10-5 10-0 10-7
4
7
6
5
5
5
4
4
5
11
10
9
8
8
7
6
6
5
5
6
16
15
13
13
12
11
10
9
8
7
6
7
22
20
18
18
17
15
14
12 1
12
11
9
8
8
29
27
24
24
22
20
19
17
16
15
13
Il
9
8
9
37
34
31
30
29
26
24
22
21
20
17
14
12
10
10
46
42
39
38
36
33
31
29
27
25
22
19
16
13
11
56
51
48
47
45
41
38
36
34
32
28
24
20.
17
12
67
61
58
56
54
49
47
43
41
39
35
30
25
22
C
z
tA
T
v
This table applies only to those special cases in which the number of occasions on which objects
are being ranked (n) is equal to the number of assignable ranks (N). Each entry represents the
largest number that is significant at the indicated p-level.
Source: R. L. Morrisl
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
V
N 1
2
F3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
0
...
.5000
3333
.3750
.36667
.36806
.36786
.367882
.367879
.367879
.3678794
.3678794
1
1.000
..
.5000
.3333
.37500
.36667
.36806
.367857
.367882 1
.367879
.3678795 1
.3678794
2
.5000
...
.2500
.16667
.18750
.18333
.184028
.183929
.183941
.1839396
.1839397
3
.1667
...
.08333
.05556
.06250
.061111
.061343
.061310
.06131:17
.0613132
4
1
.0417
...
.02083
.01389
.015625
.015278
.015336
.0153274
.0153284
5
.00833
...
.00417
.002778
.003125
.003056
.0030671
.0030655
6
.00139
...
.000694
.000463
.000521
.0005093
.0005112
7
.00020
...
.000099
.000066
.0000744
.0000728
8
.000025
...
.000012
.00000>3
.0000093
9
.000003
...
.000001.4
.0000009
10
.000000
...
.0000001
11
.0000000
...
12
.0000000
Source: N. Feller2
m
v
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003S:1C:RIE-TDP79-00999A000400030001-7
data of Experiment 3 (drill press/S4) as a test case. An analyst of the
sponsor organization, blind as to the target and given only the subject's
taped narrative and drawings (Figure 9), was able, from the subject's
description alone, to correctly classify the target as a "man-sized ver-
tical boring machine."
In general, it appears that use of multiple-subject responses
to a single target provides better signal-to-noise ratio than target iden-
tification by a single individual. Further, our observation is that most
of the correct information is of a nonanalytic nature pertaining to shape,
form, color, and material rather than to function or name. That is, we
often observe the correct description of basic elements and patterns
coupled with incomplete or erroneous analysis of function. As a result,
we have learned to urge our subjects simply to describe what they see as
opposed to interpreting the perceived data. One should not infer that
analytic functioning in the remote viewing mode is never observed, however,
as indicated by codeword retrieval in the West Virginia Site experiment
discussed in Subsection 1-B above, and by the sponsor-staff-member-generated
response of Figure 6.
B. Detection of Secret Writing (SW) Target Material
To determine whether documents containing secret writing (SW) could
be differentiated from other documents, SRI carried out a double-blind
experiment under sponsor control. Twenty-seven numbered envelopes con-
taining target drawings of variable content and preparation, sealed and
specially secured by the sponsor, were submitted to SRI researchers for
sorting. The goal was the differentiation of the 12 envelopes containing
the SW drawings from the envelopes containing either pencil drawings (6)
or blanks (9). This distribution was the only datum given to researchers
and subject. The key, shown in Table 5, remained under sponsor control
until the experiment was completed and the data were submitted to the COTR.
A series of sorting runs to detect SW material was carried out with
SRI subject Sl. The series consisted of 24 runs through the 27 cards,
choosing 12 cards each run, the goal being to choose the 1.2 SW cards.
Thus, each run consisted of a sort into one of two binary channels, non-
SW or SW, say (0,1). The numbered envelopes containing the target
Approved For Release 200SEic Ri T DP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releas QQ3/f ftoA ( 9?jQ.q(Q99A000400030001-7
BELT DRIVE FOR DRILL
PRESS (CAN BE SEEN ONLY
FROM ABOVE MACHINE)
TARGET: DRILL PRESS
(TECHNOLOGY SERIES)
SUBJECT (S4) DRAWING OF DRILL PRESS SHOWING BELT DRIVE, STOOL AND
"VERTICAL GRAPH THAT GOES UP AND DOWN"
Approved For Releas q4 311 %S jjff tQd@99A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 20035ftC:RIE -DP79-00999A000400030001-7
KEY FOR SECRET WRITING EXPERIMENT
(Kept blind to experimenters until experiment completion)
Card No.
Target Material
Target Content
1
Pencil
Large X
2
Pencil
Small A
Pencil
Large A
Blank
Blank
Blank
Blank
Pencil
Large 0
7
Pencil
Small x
8
Pencil
Small o
9
Blank
Blank
10
Blank
Blank
11
CD-294
Small A
12
CD-294
Large X
13
CD-294
Large A
14
CD-294
Small o
15
Blank
Blank
16
CD-294
Small x
17
Blank
Blank
18
CD-294
Large 0
19
CD-175
Large 0
20
Blank
Blank
21
CD-175
Large X
22
CD-175
Small A
23
Blank
Blank
24
Blank
Blank
25
CD-175
Small o
26
CD-175
Large A
27
CD-175
Small x
Approved For Release 200971'FOl%&A~RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release ~q /1A :?~.f T76~9~Q999A000400030001-7
material were randomized before each run and placed inside unnumbered
opaque envelopes betore being presented to the subject for sorting.
The annpropriate analysis technique for a binary sort (0,1) is the
method known as sequential sampling. The sequential method gives a
rule of procedure for making one of three possible decisions for each
card tol_iowing a given binary sort: accept 1 as the bit being carried
1w the card; reject 1 as the bit being carried by the card (i.e., accept
0); or continue sampling of the card under consideration. The sequential
sampling procedure differs from fixed-length statistical analysis procedures
in that the number of sorts required to reach a final decision on a card
;pit. is not fixed before sampling, but depends on the results accumulated
with each sampling run. The primary advantage of the sequential sampling
procedure as compared with the other methods is that, on the average,
fewer sorts per final decision are required for an equivalent degree of
rre-pliability.
Use of the sequential sampling procedure requires the specification
of parameters that are determined on the basis of the following consid-
orations. Assume that a labeling bit (0 or 1) is being carried by each
card. From the standiDoint of the sorter, the probability of correctly
identifying the bit being carried is some value p because of chance alone.
An operative sensing channel could then be expected to alter the proba-
bility of correct identification to a higher value p = p, + '. Good psi
functioning on a repetitive task is observed to result in = 0.12, as
reported by RyzL." Therefore, let us assume a baseline psi parameter
,1~ h=0..12.
The question to be addressed in the case of sorting 12 SW cards from
among 27 cards is whether a given card is sorted into the SW channel at
i low rare p commensurate with the hypothesis H0 that the card in ques-
tion is a non-SW card, or at a higher rate pl commensurate with the hy-
pothesis H, that the card in question is indeed an SW card. The decision-
making process requires the specification of four parameters:
(1) p,_k: The probability of sorting incorrectly a non-SW (0) card
oto tile SW (1) channel. In the sort of 12 SW cards from among
27, the probability of correctly sorting a non-SW (0) card into
Approved For Release ( ( I f jkSSg1'JECV9A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
UNCLASSIFIED
the non-SW (0) channel is p = pc + 'fib = 15/27 + 0.12 = 0.676.
Therefore, the probability of a non-SW (0) card being incor-
rectly sorted into the SW (1) channel is 1 - p = 0.324 = p
0.
(2) p 1: The probability of sorting correctly an SW (1) card into
the SW (1) channel. In the sort of 12 SW cards from among 27,
the probability of correctly sorting an SW (1) card into the
SW (1) channel is p1 = Pc + l'b = 12/27 + 0.12 = 0.564.
(3) a: The probability of rejecting a correct identification for a
non-SW (0) card (designated in statistics as a Type 1 error).
We shall take a = 0.1.
(4) (3: The probability of accepting an incorrect identification
for an SW (1) card (designated in statistics as a Type II error).
We shall take 0.1.
(Lower values for a and result in increased accuracy, but at the ex-
pense of requiring longer runs. Therefore, a compromise must be made
between the desire to maximize reliability and to minimize redundancy.)
With the parameters thus specified, the sequential sampling procedure
provides for construction of a decision graph as shown in Figure 10. A
cumulative record of sorts of a given card is compiled run by run until
either the upper or lower limit line is reached, at which point a decision
is made to label the card as 0 (non-SW) or 1 (SW).
As indicated in Figure 10, during the 24 runs carried out, SW cards
13, 18, 21, 22, 25, 26, and 27 correctly emerged through the upper limit
line to be labeled SW, along with pencil cards 1 and 2 and blank card 17,
the latter three incorrectly. We note that five of the six CD-175 cards
ended up correctly sorted. With regard to the lower limit line, pencil
cards 3, 6, and 8, and blank cards 4, 20, and 23 correctly emerged
through the lower limit line to be labeled non-SW, along with (incorrectly)
SW cards 12, 16, and 19. Thus, of the 19 cards that emerged through the
limit lines, 13 are correct. Although we cannot rule out the possibility
of obtaining 13 correct choices out of 19 labelings by chance (p = 0.09 by
Approved For ReleaU MQL/A S JFF iFo 999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
m
v
SW-27
SW-26
SW-13
2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16
RUN NUMBER, N
DECISION 3
CONTINUE
SAMPLING
DECISION 2
ACCEPT "0" AS
THE BIT BEING
CARRIED
9,4
?14,2 24
5 ,7,0
11,15
1
-1 1 18 20 22 24
UPPER AND LOWER LIMIT
LINES GIVEN BY:
E1 = d1 + SN,
Eo = -do + SN,
d1 =
-R
log 1
a
do =
log p1(1-P0)
- -
po(1-P1)
log (1-po
(1-p1)
log p1(1-p0}
Po(1-P1)
FIGURE 10 PROCEDURE FOR CARD SORTING BY SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING
Po = 0.324, p1 =0.564,a=0.1,/3=0.1
BROKEN LINE SHOWS SAMO SAMPLING SEQUENCE FOR CARD NO. 25
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
DECISION 1
ACCEPT "1" AS
THE BIT BEING
CARRIED
Approved For Rele1NL3/L3AOS 94-FIt f'D0999A000400030001-7
exact binomial calculation*), the result indicates a tendency toward cor-
rect labeling that could be explored further. With an increased number
of runs, the probabilities for a and (3 errors can be reduced while still
permitting a large percentage of labelings to be made. (For completeness
we include the raw data call sheet as Table 6.)
A second shorter series of 18 sorting runs through the 27 cards to
choose the six pencil cards yielded chance results.
*
Recognizing that the probability of a correct choice by chance is the
probability that an SW card is sorted into the SW channel, or a non-SW
card is sorted into the non-SW channel, we have
12 12 15 15
p(corr) = 27 x 27 + 27 x 27 = 0.506.
From this the probability of at least 13 correct choices by chance out
of 19 tries can be calculated from
19!
p 1!(19-i)! (0.506)1(0.494) 9-1 = 0.09
i=13
Approved For ReleaU d6A &S l 999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea tl4.?t-rfP,7g-6999AO00400030001-7
`CABLE 6
RAW DATA CALL SHEET FOR SECRET WRITING EXPERIMENT
SE
CT 12 PER RUN)
Card Ch
osen
I' P P I B P P P B B
SW SW SW SW B
SW B
SW SW B
SW SW B
B
SW SW SW
1
2 3
5
6 7
8
9
10
11 ,
12
13
14
1.5
16
17
18
19
20
21.
22
23
24
25
26
27
Run #
I x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
2
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
3
x
t x
x
x
x
x
4
x x
x x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
6
x x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
7
x x x x x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
8 x x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
x
99 x x
x x
x
x
x
1x x x x x x x
1.2 x x x x xx x x x x x
]3 x x x x x x x x x x x x
14 x x x x x x x x x x x x
1-5 x x x x x x x x x x x
16x x x x x x x x x x x
117 xxxx x x x x x x x x
18 x x x x x x x x x x x x
9 x xxxx x x x x x x x
0 x x x x x x x x
21 x
x x x x x x x x x X x
122 x x x x x x x x x x x x
23 X x x x x X x x x x x x
24 x x x x x x x x x x x
Approved For Relea 0k/ fl fbtED999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release NCEMISTfISO999A000400030001-7
In addition to experimentation carried out under conditions appropri-
ate to assessing the operational utility of paranormal abilities, approx-
imately 50 percent of the program effort was devoted to a basic research
effort that included:
(1) Identification of measurable characteristics possessed by
gifted individuals,
(2) Identification of neurophysiological correlates that relate
to paranormal activities,
(3) Identification of the nature of paranormal phenomena and
energy.
A. Screening Tests
To meet the above objectives, the first prerequisite was the estab-
lishment of criteria capable of differentiating individuals apparently
gifted in paranormal functioning from those who were not. This pre-
requisite was met by carrying out a series of screening tests under fixed
protocol conditions. The tests were designed to ensure that all conven-
tional communications channels were blocked, and that the outcomes could
be sufficiently unambiguous to determine whether paranormal functioning
occurred. Individuals gifted in certain areas of paranormal functioning
could then be differentiated from those who were not on the basis of
whether their results differed significantly from chance.
Two experimental paradigms were utilized as screening tests on the
basis that these tests had been useful for such purposes prior to this
program (in the sense that certain apparently gifted individuals did
exceedingly well in at least one of these tests, whereas the results of
unselected volunteers did not differ significantly from chance expecta-
tion). The tests were (a) the remote viewing of natural targets, and (b)
the determination of the state of a four-state random target generator.
The first type of test constitutes a so-called "free-response" paradigm
in which the subject originates freely about contents of his awareness;
furthermore, the channel in general may involve both direct perception
of the remote site and perception of the mental contents of an observer
at the site. In the second type of test, on the other hand, the target
Approved For ReleaseUN /{QAS5 F*ED 9A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 0 /1 M96999AO00400030001-7
UNCSS
is more abstract (an electronic state), the target is blind to all par-
ticipants, and the subject's choice is precisely constrained.
For- the purpose of screening, a result is considered unambiguously
paranormal if the a priori probability for the occurrence of the result
fy chance, under the null hypothesis, is p 0.50
To G
85
87
86
88
346
0.058
> 0.99
From B
85
82
90
87
344
0.395
> 0.90
R
91
91
83
92
357
0.591
> 0.80
Initial states
8
14
9
13
44
2.364
> 0.50
All states
365
353
356
372
1446
0.622
> 0.80
Nondiagonal
transitions
261
252
257
267
1037
0.466
> 0.90
Diagonal
transitions
96
87
90
92
365
0.468
> 0.90
TABLE 15
Randomness Tests for Machine M2 Output
During Successful Experimental Series by Subject S2
(Runs 1 through 8, 17 through 44, and 81 through 100)
Buttons
Number
Binomial
Yellow
Green
Blue
Red
of
Trials
Chi-Square
probability
Transitions Y
108
120
111
124
463
1.458
> 0.50
To G
107
131
136
119
493
4.095
> 0.20
From B
126
124
138
135
523
1.061
> 0.70
R
118
115
140
129
502
3.100
> 0.30
Initial states
16
15
13
12
56
0.714
> 0.80
All states
475
505
538
519
2037
4.149
> 0.20
Nondiagonal
351
359
387
378
1475
2.247
> 0
50
transitions
.
Diagonal
108
131
138
129
506
3.960
> 0.20
transitions
Approved For ReleUN"A~:~IkrRUP7500999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasU 0,1"I . ~tjfftbPNA000400030001-7
NUL
Buttons
Number
Chi-Square
Binomial
or
probability
Yellow Green
Blue
Red
Trials
Transtions Y
204 199
199
216
818
0.944
> 0.80
To G
192 218
222
207
839
2.578
> 0.30
From B
211 206
228
222
867
1.397
> 0.70
R
209 206
223
221
859
1.009
> 0.70
Initial states
24 29
22
25
100
1.040
> 0.70
All states
840 858
894
891
3483
2.364
a
> 0.50
Nondiagonal
612 611.
644
645
2512
1.736
> 0.50
transitions
Diagonal
204 218
228
221
871
1.399
> 0.70
transitions
Approved For ReleasbMd b~/1A:91~-rp1'f -~+ 999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReIeUeNCIL cS SJ.FiE D o999A000400030001-7
With regard to the possibility that the subject developed
an optimum strategy based on slight, even though nonsignificant, machine
departures from chance expectation, it is sufficient to determine the
most favorable strategy based on machine statistics and examine whether
use of such a strategy would be capable in principle of producing a re-
sult as significant as that produced by the subject.
For machine Ml the optimum strategy, according to Table
17 is: if in the initial state, press green; if yellow, press yellow;
otherwise, pass. Use of such a strategy would, in the 44 runs carried
out, result in 14 correct initial state selections and a scoring fraction
96/355 = 0.2704 on the remaining 44 x 24 = 1056 transitions, resulting
in 300 hits.
For machine M2 the optimum strategy, according to Table
18, is: if in the initial state, press yellow; if red, press blue; other-
wise pass. Use of such a strategy would, in the 56 runs carried out,
result in 16 correct initial state selections and a scoring fraction
140/502 = 0.2789 on the remaining 56 x 24 = 1344 transitions, resulting
in 391 hits. Thus, an optimum strategy derived from the machine distri-
bution post hoc yields a scoring fraction 691/2500 = 0.2764, significantly
less than the observed scoring fraction 0.2936. In any case, it is clear
from an examination of the compilation of subject choices (Tables 17 and
18) that subject selections, although extremely nonrandom, differed
widely from those strategies favorable to the production of results based
on machine statistics. Further, there is no evidence of learning to sup-
port the hypothesis that a successful strategy was developed. A more
detailed analysis of strategies, confirming these conclusions, was carried
out by the sponsor under the direction of the COTR.
When subject S2 was asked to repeat the entire experiment
at a later time, he was able to replicate successfully a high mean scoring
rate (27.88/100 average over 2500 trials, a result whose a priori proba-
bility under the null hypothesis is p = 4.8 x 10-4).
We thus conclude from the machine study that of the six
subjects tested, one subject (S2) was able to generate a significant and
replicable result. From these results, we conclude that there is evidence
for the existence of a human perceptual capability whereby electronically
Approved For ReIeANA~Ad':~ILFRRTJ00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasI Z / EAS S IIfffU99A000400030001-7
Transitions Y
To G
From 43
R
Pass
Initial states
All states
TABLE 17
SUBJECT S2 SELECTIONS ON MACHINE Ml
DURING; SUCCESSFUL EXPERIMENTAL SERIES
(Runs 9 through 16 and 45 through 80)
Buttons
Yellow Green Blue Red
51
45
30
149
73
14
362
47
12
17
58
36
4
174
35
13
2
37
13
6
106
141
70
38
82
108
19
458
73
30
16
110
116
1
346
TABLE 18
SUBJECT' S2 SELECTIONS ON MACHINE M2
DURING SUCCESSFUL EXPERIMENTAL SERIES
(Runs 1 through 8, 17 thresh 44, and 81 _throu h 100)
67
77
54
179
125
To G
68
2
14
107
38
From
13
50
22
2
40
15
R
208
96
38
31
111
Pass
105
33
22
129
348
Initial
states
21
7
1
27
0
All states
519
237
131
513
637
Approved For ReleasUMtlA SIS-PH!-?999A000400030001-7
Approved For Rele4NC3/L3 S -HFEO0999A000400030001-7
stored information can be accessed by means of a perceptual modality not
mediated by physical parameters as yet identified.
The characteristics of such a channel can be specified in
accordance with the precepts of communication theory. The bit rate asso-
ciated with the information channel is calculated from8
R = H(x) = Hy(x) , (1)
where H(x) is the uncertainty of the source message containing symbols
with an a priori probability p.
i
2
H(x) IT P; log,,P,
(2)
and Hy(x) is the conditional entropy based on the a posteriori probabil-
ities that a received signal was actually transmitted,
Hy(x) = L P(i,j) log2 pi(j) (3)
i,j=l
For S2's first run, with pi = 1/4, p.(j) = 0.2936, and an
average of 30 s/choice, we have a source uncertainty H(x) = 2 bits and a
calculated bit rate
R = 0.007 bits/symbol
R/T = 2 x 10-4 bits/s
.In a larger study for NASA, devoted specifically to the
question of whether learning could take place, 147 subjects were screened.9
Of these subjects, six showed a positive learning slope significant at
the 0.01 level or better; the binomial probability of this occurring by
chance is 3.8 x 10-3. At the other extreme, no subjects had a negative
slope at the 0.01 level or better, in contrast to those six who had a
positive slope at the 0.01 level. The slopes of the remaining 141 subjects
(448,000 trials) were found to be normally distributed.
Approved For ReleUJYCaLA(5SIL RbE0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release R 1 A q5 f tj999A000400030001-7
B. Identification of Measurable Characteristics Possessed Gifted
Subjects
1. Medical Evaluation
The medical evaluation of program participants was assigned to
the Palo Alto Medical Clinic. Coordination of the program was handled
by Dr. Robert Armbruster, Director of the Clinic's Department of Environ-
mental Medicine.
The testing procedures fall into six categories:
(I) General. physical examination, including complete medical
and family history.
(2) Laboratory examinations, including SMA-12 panel blood chem-
istries, protein electrophoresis, blood lipid profile,
urinalysis, serology, blood type and factor, pulmonary
function screening, and 12-lead electrocardiogram.
(3) Neurological examination, including comprehensive and
electroencephalogram (sleeping and routine).
(4) Audiometric examination, including comprehensive, Bekesy
bone conduction, speech discrimination, and impedance
bridge test.
(5) Ophthalmologist examination, including comprehensive,
card testing, peripheral field test, muscle test, dilation
funduscope, and indirect ophthalmoscopic and fundus
examination.
(6) EMI brain scan.
The detailed subject-by-subject test results are on file with
the sponsor. Following are the summary evaluations prepared by Dr.
Armbruster.
a. Subject Sl (Experienced)
This 55-year-old male completed an extensive medical evaluation
recently in conjunction with special studies being performed at
SRI for personnel gifted in paranormal functioning.
Aside from a duodenal ulcer in 1952, his past medical history
was essentially normal. At present significant defects on
physical examination were small, bilateral inguinal hernias,
and dental caries. An electrocardiogram was interpreted as
abnormal, showing characteristics of coronary artery disease.
Approved For Relea* /0{1A:S5 f IIED999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releasli ffethsSWTED 99A000400030001-7
Si was advised of this report by letter on August 9, 1974, with
recommendations for further evaluation by a cardiologist.*
Laboratory data--including waking and sleeping electroencephalo-
grams, protein electrophoresis, complete blood count, serology,
and urinalysis--were normal. He has blood type AB, Rh positive.
Blood chemistries were essentially normal with the exception of
slight elevations of uric acid and cholesterol with marked ele-
vation of triglycerides.
Ophthalmological examination revealed a minimal refractive error
corrected easily to normal. An extensive neurological examina-
tion and history were considered normal. The EMI computerized
brain scan was considered negative except for slight enlargement
of the right ventricle. No significance can be presently placed
on this finding.
Audiometry revealed a mild bilateral high-frequency hearing loss
at 3000 cps secondary to past exposure to high noise levels.
b. Subject S2 (Experienced)
This 31-year-old male research scientist completed an extensive
medical evaluation recently in conjunction with special studies
being performed at SRI for gifted psychic personnel.
His past medical history reveals a right inguinal herniorraphy
and appendectomy. Physical examination revealed no significant
abnormalities. Laboratory data--including electrocardiography,
protein electrophoresis, audiometry, pulmonary function, serology,
blood chemistries, urinalysis, waking and sleeping electroenceph-
alograms, and EMI brain scan--were normal.
Ophthalmological exam revealed a myopic refractive error cor-
rectable to 20/15 bilaterally.
Neurological examination was negative. History revealed the
presence of periodic muscular contraction headaches. Migraine
headaches have been completely relieved since institution of
biofeedback training.
c. Subject S3 (Experienced)
This 40-year-old male research consultant completed an extensive
medical evaluation recently in conjunction with special studies
being performed at SRI for gifted psychic personnel.
His past medical history, other than a case of hepatitis while
in the Armed Services, was essentially noncontributory to this
study. Physical examination was entirely within normal limits.
In spite of follow-up medical treatment, we note with sadness his death
in July 1975 due to a coronary.
Approved For Re lea I'9-?999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releasp_2Q0 /03/ W 7 9 J 0,L b00999A000400030001-7
Laboratory data--including electrocardiography, protein electro-
phoresis, audiometry, pulmonary function, complete blood count,
serology, urinalysis, and EMI brain scan--were normal. Blood
chemistries were normal with the exception of moderate elevation
of blood lipids. He has blood type A, Rh positive.
He has myopic astigmatism correctable to 20/20 bilaterally; other-
wise a normal ophthalmological examination.
Neurological history, examination, and both sleeping and waking
encephalograms were reported as normal.
d. Sul? ect S4 1Learner/ Contro.l)
This 53-year-old female photographic consultant completed a med-
ical. evaluation recently in conjunction with special studies being
performed at SRI for gifted psychic personnel.
Except for several surgeries, her past history is essentially
noncontribntorv to her medical record. Her physical examination
was normal with the exception of a minor gynecological problem.
Laboratory data--including electrocardiography, protein electro-
phoresis, audiometrv. pulmonary function, complete blood count,
sere,logy, 'lood chemistries, and urinalysis--were normal. Blood
type is 0, Rh positive. Ophthalmological examination was normal
except for a mild cuii ctable refractive error.
Both waking and sleeping electroencephalograms were normal as
were the neurological history and physical examinations.
The EMI brain scan was reported as suggestive of very mild frontal
atrophy. No other abnormal features are noted.
e. Subject SS (Learner/Control)
This 54-year-old male staff scientist completed an extensive med-
ical evaluation recently in conjunction with special studies being
performed at SRI for gifted psychic personnel.
His past history reveals a duodenal ulcer in 1964 but is other-
wise not significant. Physical examination was essentially neg-
ative.
Laboratory data--including electrocardiography, lipoprotein elec-
trophoresis, complete blood count, serology, blood chemistries,
blood lipids, urinalysis, and EMI brain scan---were normal. Audi-
ometry revealed a mild bilateral perceptive-type hearing loss
at 4000 cps probably due to noise exposure. Pulmonary function
suggested mild pulmonary obstructive disease secondary to ciga-
rette smoking.
Ophthalmological exam was normal except for a corrective refrac-
tive error. Neurological history revealed rare migraine aurae
without headache. Neurological examination was entirely normal.
Approved For ReIe a 2pQ 13110 , gt TEff0999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasUDn3et1 tItfpF'7-p-QQ999A000400030001-7
f. Subject S6 (Learner/Control)
This 34-year--old female completed an extensive medical examina-
tion recently in conjunction with special studies being performed
at SRI for gifted psychic personnel.
Her past medical history was essentially noncontributory except
for the vague but unlikely possibility of Reiters Syndrome.
Laboratory data--including electrocardiography, protein electro-
phoresis, audiometry, pulmonary function, complete blood count,
serology, blood chemistries, urinalysis, and EMI brain scan--were
normal. She has blood type A, Rh positive.
Ophthalmological examination, with the exception of a moderate
but corrected myopic astigmatism, was within normal limits.
Although her waking EEG was abnormal, her sleeping EEG and
neurological examination were perfectly normal. It was not
considered necessary or advisable to pursue this mild abnormality
any further, especially in view of a normal neurological exam
and EMI brain scan. The patient was not made aware of this
minor deviation. Neurological history substantiates period
muscular contraction headaches.
g. Summary of Medical Evaluation
In summary, it appears that the medical profiling is noncontributory
to the study, all subjects showing essentially normal medical
profiles without any discernible spread among the subjects.
2. Psychological Evaluation
The psychological evaluation of the program participants was
assigned to the Palo Alto Medical Clinic. Coordination of the program
was handled by Dr. J.E. Heenan, Chief Clinical Psychologist of the Clinic's
Department of Psychiatry. The testing itself was carried out by Dr.
Karen Nelson, Clinical Psychologist at the Clinic.
The tests administered included:
(1) In-depth interviews, including objective events and
subjective views relating to the discovery and enhance-
ment of paranormal capacities; socioeconomic, cultural,
familial, religious environment; outstanding emotional
peaks, traumas; values, motivation, interpersonal style.
(2) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)
(3) Bender Gestalt Visual Motor Test
(4) Benton Visual Memory Test
Approved For Relea9el L/'0[/A:55-FI )ED999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release iQOdQ3/J0C5 -MPJU0999A000400030001-7
(5) Wechsler. Memory Scale
(6) Luscher Color Test
(7) Strong Vocational Interest Blank
(8) Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI)
(9) Edwards Personality Preference Schedule (EPPS)
;10) Rorschach Inkblot
x;11) Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)
The detailed test results for each subject are on file with the
COTR. Due to the personal nature of the data we present here only the
summary evaluations, first by the clinical psychologist who administered
the tests and interviewed the subjects in depth, and second by the chief
clinical psychologist who analyzed the data on a blind basis.
a. Evaluation by Clinical Psychologist Administering Tests
The following is quoted from the psychologist's report:
During late summer and early fall, 1974, six subjects were re-
ferred to the Clinic for testing for the parapsychology study at
Stanford Research Institute. Three of the subjects were desig-
nated as sensitive subjects and three of the subjects were desig-
nated as controls. It was planned that I would do the testing
without knowledge of which subjects were considered sensitive and
which subjects were considered controls. However, in the course
of my contacts with these subjects, it proved impossible not to
know which subjects belonged to which group, since I was to inter-
view each person in depth. Since personal experience with appar-
ently extrasensory perception is a fairly dramatic event, subjects
could not avoid talking about these events and still be honest in
an in-depth interview. Consequently, a secondary plan was
developed in which 1 would do the psychological testing and write
individual reports or each subject, and the Chief Clinical Psychol-
ogist, Dr. Heenan, would read the test blind and see whether he
could pick out three test records which seemed more similar to
each other than the rest, thereby discriminating between sensitive
and non-sensitive subjects.
intellec t uei i_ Functioning
All of the subjects in this study displayed distinctly above-
average intellectual abilities. Most subjects reached the superior
range, and several of the subjects reached the gifted range. As
it happened, the control subjects tended to show higher average
intellectual functioning scores than did sensitive subjects,
although the difference could not be said to be significant,
given that: there were only three subjects in each group. Two
of the subjects from the sensitive group showed highly variable
Approved For Rele_sp~2Q Q3/AO~ ji-FIFEV0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea JC:Ll&.SiS-kFIIE-D999A000400030001-7
subtest scores within their intelligence test battery. That
is, some of the subskills would be'extremely high and other
subskills would be extremely low, The variable patterns shown
are consistent with ambivalent motivation as regards learning
tasks and academic situations. I was able to spot no consistent
trends as to which subskills tended to be high and which sub-
skills tended to be low. For all six subjects, verbal and per-
formance skills tended to be about evenly balanced, and memory
skills were approximately what would be expected, given the in-
telligence scores attained. The number scores on memory tests as
well as the performances of the subjects themselves reflect a
slight tendency toward better memory for material which is
organized logically or which appears in a meaningful context
than for rote memory material. In the control group, this
tendency seems less pronounced and in fact one subject showed
a clear preference for rote memory material. The subjects
themselves did not feel that any of the intelligence test
material tapped skills or propensities on their part which might
be linked to their extrasensory capabilities, and since the
patterns of strength and weakness within the test profiles
varied so widely, I am inclined to accept their judgment with
one possible exception. It is possible that sensitive subjects
tend to be holistic perceivers rather than analytic perceivers;
that is, to perceive in Gestalt rather than analytic elements.
This might underlie the tendency for better short term memory
of contextual logical material. Psychological tests which are
directly relevant to this difference in perceptual style appear
not be standardized as yet and so it is difficult to follow this
lead.
Personality Functioning
When looked at from the point of view of psychopathology, the
indicators both in projective and in objective testing do not
appear to me to show marked trends, either for the six subjects
taken together or for the subjects in each group. There does
appear to be an interesting similarity in defensive style, par-
ticularly when this is taken together with a similarity in
interests and vocational aptitude, which can be seen in a
large number of the subjects both in sensitive and control
groups. To elaborate, all six subjects tended to have high
feminine scores on the masculinity-feminity scale of the MMPI.
That scale does not measure sexual orientation but rather sex
role stereotype. For example, a person who is highly active
in expressing his aggression, who is self assertive and who
adopts "masculine" interest in, say, sports, mechanics, etc.,
is likely to get a high masculine score; a person who tends
to be fairly passive in expressing aggression, even manipulative,
who tends to be interested in the arts, in music, in aesthetic
sensitivities, is likely to gain a high feminine score. Both
the men and women in this group of subjects tended to have high
feminine scores. The trend is seen again in the vocational
aptitude survey, the Strong Vocational Interest Blank, wherein
all of the subjects tended to achieve high scores in music, art
Approved For ReleasU i1AssiFrE-IU99A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
UNCLASSIFIED
and writing, but particularly in writing was this consistent.
The score on writing aptitude appeared to be above average for
the general population in each subject and for several of the
subjects it was one of the highest scores obtained. These two
trends in the objective personality test data can be compared with
anot.ner trend found in the projective test data, namely on the
Rorschach. Here, the responses of the subjects tended to emphasize
animal or human movement and to de-emphasize color. This pattern
is common in people wno tend to be introspective, to have a rich
inner fantasy life. and in fact to prefer that kind of expression
of :.heir emotions to interpersonal expression. The capacity to
stand back from one's feelings, observe them, analyze them, even
to :-savor them, is common among artists and particularly among
wri*_ers.
Unfortunately, two of the subjects (Sl and S3) from the sensitive
group were highly defensive about test-taking and their defen-
siveness was most pronounced in the projective personality tests.
The result was that they gave very minimal records, very few
responses, and were close-mouthed in talking about their responses.
Hence, the pattern to which I refer can be seen more clearly in
the control subjects than in the sensitive subjects even though
it appears to occur for all six subjects.
In ,one course of the testing, the control subjects began to tell
me that as they participated in the SRI study, they appeared to
be developing more and more sensitivity on the experiments per-
formed and each was not certain that he should be properly
cla:-;sified as a control subject. In talking with Dr. Puthoff,
I learned that they did appear to be showing some sensitivity
but that their performances were not reliable and so they still
couid be said to be importantly different from the sensitive
subjects. If the sensitive subjects could be induced to be
less defensive in test-taking, it is possible that their records
wouid show a pattern which could be distinguished from that of
the control subjects. Since that is not the case, we are left
with a dilemna. A tendency toward artistic interests, a rich
fantasy life and an introversive style of emotional expression
may be accidental in all of these six subjects. It may be
characteristic of persons who are willing to participate in
parapsychological studies. It may be characteristic of persons
who have some extrasensory capacity, whether great or small, or
it .nay relate to some other variable which happens to be common
to L.hese six subjects.
Should the pattern of emotional style and aesthetic interest
prove relevant to extrasensory capacity, it would seem that the
Rorschach gets at the most fundamental level of this quality.
The objective tests are more likely to be measuring the end
products of that fundamental level of emotional expression.
Sin::e my reading of projective test material is likely to be
colored by my acquaintance with the subjects and what they said
about themselves, i will be interested to see whether Dr. Heenan
82 10 Approved For Release 200 3/03 /10 c l IF3Pj9?Q0999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasU f t/1A SIR-rF E D999A000400030001-7
can discern the same pattern, and for my own curiosity I would
like to be able to test the sensitive subjects again, without
them having read this report, to see whether I could put them
more at ease on a second contact and get more productive records
from them. Two of these subjects (Si and S3) said frankly that
they were alienated at the thought of psychological testing
because their experience was that people with extrasensory capac-
ity were written off as nuts and that psychologists and psychi-
atrists always examined them with an eye toward any pathology
they could discover. If they could be reassured that that was
not the point of interest and at the same time not be coached
as to what kinds of responses I was interested in, another
session of projective testing might be productive.
Karen L. Nelson, PhD
Clinical Psychologist
Palo Alto Medical Clinic
b. Evaluation by Chief Clinical Psychologist (On a Blind
Basis
An effort complementary to the overall analysis performed
by Dr. K. Nelson was carried out by Dr. J. Heenan, Chief Clinical Psychol-
ogist, Department of Psychiatry, Palo Alto Medical Clinic. He took on
as a task the ferreting out of responses to specific test items to deter-
mine whether a particular cluster of items might serve as the core of a
screening procedure. Dr. Heenan's analysis was carried out on a blind
basis, that is, without knowledge of which subjects were labeled sensitive
and which were labeled control. The following is quoted from Dr. Heenan's
report:
I have finished going over the psychological test data on the six
subjects tested and this is a summary of my thoughts, impressions,
clinical judgments, guesses and comparisons of various dimensions.
The six persons tested are labeled S1 through S6. Subject Sl
would not take the TAT test and did not return the EPPS test, and
there is not a Strong vocational interest test in the file on him.
I included him in the comparisons on the tests which he did take.
What I did was formulate some hypotheses and then examine the test
data, ranking people according to what their tests reflected on
those hypotheses, and from that arrived at which subjects might
have, according to the hypothesis, a more than ordinary ability
to communicate by non-ordinary means. First of all, I examined
all the test data rather carefully from a clinical psychologist's
point of view and without any specific hypotheses--that is, on
the basis of my overall intuition--made guesses, for each battery
of tests, whether or not I thought this person would be likely
to have unusual abilities. On this basis I guessed subjects S3,
S6 and S4 as the most likely ones to have been high achievers
Approved For ReleaU10~/AfPf~DQ999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 03/J 0c ~-P.j ff0999A000400030001-7
in your experiments.
The Following is a series of hypotheses on the Rorschach Ink
Blot Test and following each hypothesis are the three subjects
who Kest fit that hypothesis from the test data.
HYPOTHESIS #1 . White space responses reflect lower ability to
use non-ordinary means of communication.
Results: Subjects S3, S6 and S4 have the fewest white
space responses and therefore, according to this hypothesis,
would have the higher ability among this group.
HYPOTHESIS #2. Preoccupation with minor details (Dd) will be
inconsistent with the ability to communicate by non-ordinary
mean;.
Results: Subjects S3, S6 and S4 reflect the least use
of minor details in Rorschach responses.
HYPOTHESIS #3. Those persons with the highest percentage of
human movement responses will be those most likely to be able
to communicate by non-ordinary means.
Results: Subects S4, S6 and S2 are the three highest in
this regard.
HYPOTHESIS #4. The use of instant whole responses will be
greater in those persons with the ability to communicate by
non-ordinary means.
Results: Subjects S3, S6 and S4 are the highest in this
regard.
HYPOTHESIS #5. Using shading responses as an index for anxiety,
those who have the most. shading responses will do the least well
in communicating by non-ordinary means.
Results: Subjects S4, Sl and S2 have the most shading
responses.
HYPOTHESIS #6. Those subjects able to communicate best by
non-ordinary means will tend to be more childlike in their
general approach toiae and this will be reflected by higher
animal content percent on the Rorschach test.
Results: Subjects S5, S4 and Si.
HYPOTHESIS V. (This hypothesis is relevant to Hypothesis #6.)
Those subjects with the most animal movement responses will
tend to be able ti communicate more by non-ordinary means.
Results: Subjects S4, S6 and S2.
HYPOTHESIS #8. The persons who most use color in their re-
--------------
sponses will be most likely to be able to communicate better
by noon-ordinary means.
Results: There ir: no spread among the subjects on this
particular scorir; determinant.
HYPOTHESIS X19. Those subjects using the most emotional deter-
minants will be most likely to be able to communicate by
non-ordinary means.
Results: Subjects S3, S6 and S4 have the most use of
emotional determinants on the Rorschach Test.
Approved For ReleeRelf3/&OC~~-FJILj170999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releappq .gp2U ffff999A000400030001-7
On the MMPI, the following hypotheses were checked out.
HYPOTHESIS #1. Subjects who show the most unusual configurations
on the MMPI will be those most likely to be able to communicate
by non-ordinary means (scores above 70).
Results: Subjects S3, S6 and S2.
HYPOTHESIS #2. Those subjects who reflect the most emotional
energy as measured by the Ma score will be most likely to com-
municate by non-ordinary means.
Results: Subjects S6, S3 and S2--the opposite of this
hypothesis is that those with the lowest Ma scores were
subjects Sl, S5 and S4.
HYPOTHESIS #3. Those subjects who show the most interest in human
interaction will be most likely to do well in non-ordinary com-
munication as measured by the Si score; the rank among the sub-
jects from highest to lowest is S5, S4, S6, S2, Sl, S3. There-
fore, subjects S5, S4 and S6, according to this hypothesis,
would be the successful ones.
HYPOTHESIS #4. Those subjects showing the most depression would
be least likely to be able to communicate by non-ordinary means;
the rank on the depression score among the subjects is from
highest to lowest--S6, s4, s3, s2, S1, S5, with S6, S4, and S3
being the predicted least likely to do well at your tasks, and
subjects S2, Sl and S5 the most likely.
The Wechsler Bellevue Intelligence Scale hypotheses were simple
and easy to check. The first hypothesis on the results of the
Wechsler, HYPOTHESIS #1, is that higher intelligence as measured
by the IQ score will reflect higher ability to communicate by
non-ordinary means. Using the Full Scale IQ score, the rank from
highest to lowest on IQ is subjects S5, S2, S6, S4, Sl, and S3.
Therefore, S5, S2, S6, according to this hypothesis, would be
the subjects most likely to have succeeded. There is very little
difference in the ranking in general, using the verbal IQ and
the performance IQ. Taking a closer look at the subtest scores
of the Wechsler, the following hypotheses were checked out.
HYPOTHESIS #2 on the subtest scores: Persons with the highest
ability in visual motor coordination. as reflected by the Block
Design subtest, will be most likely to be able to communicate
by non-ordinary means. The rank on the Block Design subtest
from high to low is S5, S3, S6, S4, S2, and Sl.
HYPOTHESIS #3. Those with the best immediate memory as reflected
by the Digit Span subtest will be the most likely to achieve in
the non-ordinary communication modality. The rank for subjects
from highest to lowest on Digit Span is S3, S6, S4, S5, S2, and
Si with very little spread among them.
Other aspects of the Wechsler which were specifically checked
out were the Picture Completion subtest and the Arithmetic
subtest. The rank from highest to lowest in Picture Completion
is Si, S2, S5, S3, S4, S6, and the rank on the Arithmetic sub-
test is S5, S4, S2, S3, Si, and S6. I did not have a hypothesis
about these particular subtests since they are reflections of
Approved For Rele NC/L3AOSS4-FlIED0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releasg Z00(Q3/10 iCF3P~~9~Q0999A000400030001-7
higher IQ, which was already covered before.
Careful review of tl;e Strong Vocational Interest Blank results,
tabulating various scored categories and profile configuration,
revealed no pattern that separated any group of subjects from
any other group of subjects. This, however, is a multi-dimensional
test with many varialDles and perhaps a more complex statistical
analysis, such as analysis of variants, may show some clusters not
visible to this examiner.
On the Bender Gestalt Visual Motor test, the simple hypothesis
was made that the higher the ability to reproduce better designs,
the more likely would be the person's ability to communicate by
non-ordinary means. The Bender test results were ranked according
to quality in form, Gestalt and accuracy, and the following ranks
were. obtained. From highest to lowest, subjects S4, S3, S6, S5,
Si, and S2. No other evident material was reflected on the Bender
designs.
It appears to me that according to most of the hypotheses I came
up with, subjects Si, 36 and S4 are the most likely candidates.
The results of the Luscher and TAT tests, after careful examina-
tion, do not suggest any systematic means for breaking this
group of six into two groups of three. However, on the TAT
subjects S3, S6 and S5 appeared to this examiner to reflect
more spontaneity and childlike exuberance for living and there-
fore might. be inferrec to possess more sensitivity or awareness
to non-cognitive dimensions of experience; therefore, I think
subjects S3, S6, and S4 are the most likely ones to have done
the experiments well. I also note that those who couldn't
apparently were learning how, and therefore apparently whatever
this. ability is, it is a learnable one--of course, if such com-
munisation does exist. that should be true since we all come
with essentially the same basic equipment.
J.E. Heenan, PhD
Chief Clinical Psychologist
Palo Alto Medical Clinic
On a post hoc basis, we can examine the various hypotheses
suggested by Dr. Heenan anc: determine which ones tend to correlate with
observable paranormal functioning. However, given the small sample size,
no significant conclusions can be drawn--rather, these points simply
suggest hypotheses to be examined in future testing.
On the basis oz t he remote viewing and random target generator
experiments, experienced subjects Sl through S3 and learner/control S4
performed reliably in contrast to learner/control subjects S5 and S6.
There were four tests which tended to correlate with this partition in
the sense that three of the four successful subjects lacked a trait
Approved For Relea fV ffH qC"TPflM999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releowe/Q3/~ 0s I?b00999'0004000300017
which was possessed by both of the unsuccessful subjects. These were
the traits considered in Rorschach Hypothesis #5, MMPI Hypothesis #3,
and WBIS Hypotheses #1 and #2; the four hypotheses suggested by Dr. Heenan
were all counterindicated, that is, the responses suggested as probable
for successful subjects were found to hold for the unsuccessful ones.
On the basis of this small sample, therefore, one might consider investi-
gating the following traits as potentially indicating a lesser ability
in paranormal functioning: low anxiety index as indicated by low degree
of shading response in the Rorschach, a high degree of interest in human
interaction as measured by the Si score of the MMPI, an exceptionally high
IQ (gifted range) as measured by the Wechsler Bellevue Intelligence Scale,
and excellent visual motor coordination as reflected in the Block Design
subtest of the Wechsler Bellevue Intelligence Scale. It must be emphasized,
however, that although subjects scoring highest with regard to the above
factors did least well in the tests of paranormal functioning, all sub-
jects scored higher than the norm in these psychological factors, so it
would be erroneous to extrapolate on the basis of these data that low
scoring might indicate paranormal ability. It is simply that extremely
high scores are observed to correlate negatively with success on the
particular paranormal tasks investigated. Finally, we reiterate that the
correlation as observed on the basis of such a small sample may be gratui-
tous and should therefore only be considered as a basis for further hy-
pothesis testing.
Approved For Relep EAS SI fF4FE?DOO999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea,sg NOeUS S I F I E Do999A0004ooo30001-7
3. Neuropsycholo ic-al Evaluation
Neuropsychological profiles on the six subjects were obtained
by the administration of the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychology Test Battery
as well as other tests known to be sensitive to brain dysfunction. These
tests have proven useful in predicting, for example, both the presence
and location of brain damage in a variety of neurological diseases.
Since, when no damage is present these tests also reflect abilities
dependent on brain function, it was hoped that some meaningful pattern
of test performance would emerge for the program subjects. The testing
and evaluation was handled by Dr. Ralph Kiernan, Clinical Neuropsy-
chologist, Department of Neurology, Stanford University Medical Center,
Stanford, California.
The following is his evaluation;
All subjects were given the folowing tests:
(1) Halstead Category Test
(2) Tactual Performance Test
(3) Speech Perception Test
(4) Seashore -Rhythm Test
(5) Finger Tapping Test
(6) Trail Making Test
(7) Knox Cube Test
(8) Raven Progressive Matrices
(9) Verbal Concept Attainment Test
(10) Buschke Memory Test
(ll) Grooved Pegboard Tests
Two additional tests were added after several subjects had
been tested and were not administered to all subjects. These
(12) The Gottschaldt Hidden-Figures Test
(13) The spatial relations subtest of the SRA Primary
Mental Abilities Test.
A description of these tests along with subject scores is
given in 'Table 19.
Since other psychological testing was completed previously
on these same subjects at the Palo Alto Medical Clinic,
the results of two of these tests (The Wechsler Adult In-
te1Lisence Scale and the Benton Visual Retention Test) were
consulted in the overall neuropsychological evaluation.
Verv few of the results are common to all six subjects.
In fact, the only ones that are common involve general
Approved For ReleaU JM/H J J 1 f 1 C LJ 999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
51
S2
S3
S4
S5
S6
Y
0
Halstead Category
Nonverbal test requiring abstraction of conceptual relationships.
7
14
33
26
6
28
Test
Score: Total errors.
p
15
15*
SD
5
5
Tactual Performance
Requires placement of 10 geometrically shaped blocks in their
Test
correct locations on a formboard while blindfolded.
16.4
11.8
7.7
7.7
11.4
6.9
u
11.4
14.7
Separate RT, LT, and bimanual trials.
SD
3.6
4.8
Score: Total time (minutes).
Speech Perception
Discrimination of nonword speech sounds.
p
4
5.5
Test
Score: Total errors.
4
2
0
2
5
3
SD
3
2.5
Seashore Rhythm Test
Discrimination of nonverbal rhythms. Score: Number correct.
27
25
28
29
26
29
P
26
25.5
tD
3
3
MAX
Finger Tapping Test
Measure of finger oscillation rate for 10-second period,
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
p
50/43
44/39
both RT and LT hand trials. Score: Number taps per 10 seconds.
53/50
53/49
48/47
54/53
47/47
48/43
SD
6/6
11/1
Trail Making Test
Requires connecting numbered circles in order from 1 to 25
(Part A)
.
Paper and pencil task. Score: Total times (seconds).
40
16
18
19
30
27
p
26
33
SD
11
9
Trail Making Test
Requires connecting alphabetic and numbered circles by alterna-
62
(Part B)
ting 1+A*2-'-B, and so on. Score: Total time (seconds).
56
50
55
50
54
53
p
79.5
SD
16
31.0
Knox Cube Test
Measure of attention span and immediate visual memory.
13
13
Score: Number correct.
1.3
14
13
16
17
17
S
SD
4
4
MAN
18
18
Raven Progressive
Nonverbal intelligence test involving spatial matrices.
35
42
Matrices
Score: Number correct.
39
53
49
55
60
54
SD
10
10
MAX
60
60
Verbal Concept
Requires abstraction of verbal conceptual relationships.
p
21
21
Attainment Test
Score: Number correct.
22
24
27
23
21
24
SD
5.4
5.4
MA)
27
27
Buschke Memory
Requires learning a 20-word list in a maximum of 12 trials with
Total:
Total/ List
Test
repetition of words omitted after each trial. Score: Maximum
14/20
17/20
18/20
19/20
20/20
20/20
18/12
number words correctly remembered; List: Number words consis-
List:
p
SD
3/2*
tently remembered.
8/20
14/20
11/20
16/20
15/20
16/20
MAY
20/20
(8 Crials)(7 tria
s)
Grooved Pegboard
Requires insertion of 25 pegs in their holes in a pegboard.
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
RT/LT
l
RT/LT
p
61/66
70/7
Test
Both RT and LT hand trials. Score: Total time (seconds).
76/74
69/70
58/67
59/67
72/70
48/50
SD
9/9
10/1
Spatial Relations
Requires mental rotation and identification of figures rotated
Subtest of the PMA
in two dimensions. Score: Number correct minus number of errors.
60
52
p
SD
28
14
28
14
Gottschaldt Hidden
Requires tracing outline of simple figure hidden within lines
Figures Test
of more complex figure. Score: Time and number correct.
Poor
Avg.
V. good Outst.
Outst.
None
Available
TABLE 19. NEUROPSYCHOLOGY TEST BATTERY
Description
V
~/?
m
v
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaU N A &SIOEgD 999A000400030001-7
intelligence as measured by the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Scale. All subjects were in the bright average to very
superior range of intelligence with full scale IQ's ranging
from 116 to 134, the average IQ being 125. Performance IQ's
tended to be slightly higher than Verbal IQ's (126 average ver-
sus 123). All other test performances ranged widely from
the mildly below average to the very superior range.
A consistent pattern of test results does emerge, however,
when four of the subjects are looked at in a single group.
These four subjects are S3 through S6. All tests which
depended heavily on spatial abilities were extremely well
performed by these subjects. The block design subtest of
the WAIS is the most sensitive subtest to brain injuries
which disrupt spatial abilities. Although these subjects
obtained excellent WAIS scores in general, their near-per-
fect performances on this subtest are significantly
better
than most
of the other subtest scores. The Tactual
Per-
formance
Test (TPT) is also very sensitive to brain
dys-
function
involving spatial abilities. This test was
ex-
tremely well performed by these subjects with three
of
them obtaining total times of 7.7 minutes or less.
Times
of less than eight minutes are very rarely achieved on this
test. The TPT and block designs are two of the most sensi-
tive tests to variations in spatial ability. A third test,
the spatial relations subtest of the Primary Mental Abilities
test, was given to only two of the four subjects in this
group. Again, very superior scores (quotient scores greater
than 130) were obtained by each. This test is not highly
correlated with general intelligence, and high scores in-
dicate special proficiency in visual-spatial ability.
Two additional tests which appear to measure general
ability but which depend upon visual-perceptual ability for
their correct performance were performed in the superior
range. These are the Raven's Progressive Matrices and the
Gottschaldt Hidden Figures.
Other test performances varied substantially among these four
subjects. Three of the four had difficulty on the Category
Test and on the Buschke Memory Test. No sensible interpre-
tation of these results is readily apparent.
The two remaining subjects, Sl and S2, were quite different
in their test. performances from the above group. S2, who
obtained the second highest full scale IQ, did well on the
spatial tests described above but not as well as any of the
four above. His spatial abilities appeared to be less well
developed than his verbal skills. Sl was even less like
the group than S2. His spatial test performances were only
average for his age, and the TPT and Gottschaldt tests were
poorly performed.
90
Approved For ReleaUN9;/"SSd-EdFEO0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea ~q (~cT/A9..g0999A000400030001-7
In summary, the single, most compelling conclusion from the
test data is that six subjects studied are of significantly
above-average intellectual ability. In addition, there is
consistent evidence that four of the six subjects were parti-
cularly proficient on measures of visual and tactual spatial
ability. The performance of tests which measure this ability
is most seriously impaired by lesions which involve the
right, posterior cerebral hemisphere. There is more than
presumptive evidence that normal performance of these tests
is mediated by the right hemisphere. Therefore, at least
four of the subjects obtained test results consistent
with proficiency on these right hemisphere related tasks.
It should be pointed out that this finding can be, at best,
considered as a basis for hypothesis formation regarding
paranormal ability. Verification of such hypotheses would
depend on the results of future research.
The test results for S2 are not in conflict with the above
interpretation. Those obtained for Si, however, are in
conflict with this hypothesis and are not readily reconciled
with it.
As pointed out above, further research is necessary to
elucidate the relationship between spatial abilities, the
right hemisphere and paranormal abilities. Nonetheless,
it can be said at this point that many of the tasks per-
formed by the group of subjects at SRI have at least a
superficial resemblance to performances which require
right hemisphere function. The similarities include the
highly schematicized drawings of objects in a room or of
remote scenes. Verbal identification of these drawings
is often highly inaccurate, and the drawings themselves
are frequently left-right reversed relative to the target
configuration. Further, written material is generally not
cognized. These characteristics have been seen in left
brain-injured patients and in callosal sectioned patients.
More relevant, perhaps, than right hemisphere functioning
per se are the resemblances to a class of functioning known
as associative visual agnosia. Associative visual agnosia
involves the inability of a patient to name or otherwise
identify objects which he is capable of seeing. Such patients
who do not have more generalized intellectual impairment
are rare, and only a few have been described in the neuro-
logical literature. Several of these patients have demon-
strated the ability to copy with pencil and paper the pic-
ture or object which they failed to name. It is this
quality which impressed me as being similar to the remote
viewing performances of the SRI subjects.
In a recent review of such casesiO five patients were
found who had the ability to draw an object without being
Approved For Rele*jpfe/t 3/&S("I"FE00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaDR EIA iFjprE9I0999A000400030001-7
SSr
able to name it. 'These are the patients reported by Mack
et al. (1975)10, Albert et al. (1975)11, Davidenkov (1956)12,
Rubens and Benson (1971)13, and Lhermitte and Beauvois (1973)1'.
Drriwings and attempted namings of pictured material for one
of the patients in the studies is shown in Figure 21.
In attempting to name an object, these patients would gen-
erally produce inappropriate names which, nevertheless,
reflected some visual form characteristics of the object
in question. Their attempts seem forced and made in piece-
meal fashion to various characteristics of the picture
rather than to the picture as a whole. In similar fashion
Te:.uber's patient (1975) described the figure below as an
apple with a worm and wormholes in it.
The above description and many of those in the references
clearly illustrate that the patient sees the object and is
able to respond to at least some of its visual characteris-
tics. Most of the drawings in the references are sufficiently
complete so that an observer would be able to name the object
represented. Yet the author of the drawing cannot do this.
This type of defective performance was frequently seen in
t:he SRI subjects when they were producing drawings in the
remote viewing experiments. Two obvious differences exist,
however, between the patients with associative visual
agnosia and the SRI subjects. The SRI subjects are able
to name objects appropriately when pictures are presented
directly to the visual modality. The patients cannot do
this, and, in addition, these patients have a variety of
other visual disabilities. The latter difference is to be
expected since the patients have substantial brain injury.
The location of brain damage in associative visual agnosia
is fairly well established. Two disconnections appear
necessary in order to produce this symptom. One involves
destruction of the Left visual area as evidenced by the
right homomonous hemianopia invariably found in these
patients. The second involves isolation of the right
visual area from speech areas in the left hemisphere. This
can be the result of extensive destruction of left visual
association areas or of damage to the posterior portion
of the corpus cali_osum. The net. result of these injuries
is that objects can be seen because of visual input to the
right hemisphere visual area but that they cannot be named
because of isolation of this area from left hemisphere
language areas. Use of these objects and the drawing of
pictures of them can be accomplished because of intact
pathways within the right hemisphere.
Approved For Relealj fq"/yQ p1 EIY999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release V CTA 95IF BP99A000400030001-7
FIGURE 21 DRAWINGS AND INTERPRETATIONS BY ASSOCIATIVE VISUAL
AGNOSIA PATIENTS
Copies of line drawings. Patient was unable to identify any before copying. After
making copy, his identifications were top left, key - "I still don't know"; top
right, pig - "Could be a dog or any other animal"; bottom left, bird - "Could
be a beach stump"; bottom right, locomotive - "A wagon or a car of some kind.
The larger vehicle is being pulled by the smaller one."
Approved For ReleastjlObAS$4IFPtEcD99A000400030001-7
Approved For Releastfl NCT MFITH 999A000400030001-7
It remains to speculate on the meaning of the similarity
between the patients described above and the SRI subjects.
It should first be rioted that the similarities are more than
superficial in that the verbal descriptions attempted by
some of the SRI subjects bear a striking resemblance in
kind to those of the patient shown in Figure 21. It is as
if they are struggling with similar difficulties in verbal-
izing the image which they can readily draw. In this regard
the lateralization involved is consistent with other indica-
tions of right hemisphere function in the SRI subjects. A
highly speculative hypothesis is that during remote viewing
the subjects "see" a grossly degraded image which is not
distinct. enough to encode directly into a verbal label.
Hence the piecemeal verbalization similar to that found in
patients with associative visual agnosia.
In summary, it would appear that the neuropsychological
data are compatible with the hypotheses that (1) information received
in a putative remote viewing mode is processed piecemeal in pattern form
(consistent with a low bit rate process but not necessarily requiring
it) and (2) the errors arise in the processes of attempted integration
of the data into larger patterns directed toward verbal labeling.
C. Identification of tleurophysiolo ig cal Correlates That Relate to
Paranormal Activities
ThLs part of the program had as its goal the identification of
neurophysiological correlates of paranormal activity. The existence of
such correlates is hypothesized on the expectation that, in addition to
obtaining overt responses such as verbalizations or key presses from a
subject, it should be possible to obtain objective evidence of informa-
tion transfer by direct measurement of some physiological parameter of
a subject. Kamiya, Lindsley, Pribram, Silverman, Walter, and others
brought together to discuss physiological methods to detect ESP function-
ing, for example, have suggested that a whole range of electroencephalo-
gram (LEG) responses--such as evoked potentials (EPs), spontaneous EEG,
and the contingent negative variation (CNV)--might be sensitive indi-
cators of the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by usual sensory
processes.16
The purpose of this part of the study was twofold: (a) to obtain
information about the neurophysiological state associated with paranormal
Approved For ReleasUM 1A S'S?E0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Rele N /t3 0S -M1E o999A0004OOO30001-7
activity in general, and (b) to determine whether physiological
correlates could be used as an indicator of paranormal functioning,
hopefully to provide indicators that differentiate between correct
and incorrect responses to a paranormally applied stimulus so that
an independently-determined bias factor could be applied during the
generation of data by a subject.
Early experimentation of this type was carried out by Douglas
Dean at the Newark College of Engineering. In his search for physio-
logical correlates of information transfer, he used the plethysmograph
to measure changes in the blood volume in a finger, a sensitive indica-
tor of autonomic nervous system functioning.17 A plethysmographic
measurement was made on the finger of a subject during paranormal
communication experiments. A sender looked at randomly selected target
cards consisting of names known to the subject, together with names
unknown to him (selected at random from a telephone book). The names
of the known people were contributed by the subject and were to be of
emotional significance to him. Dean found significant changes in the
chart recording of finger blood volume when the remote sender was
looking at those names known to the subject as compared with those names
randomly chosen.
Two other early experiments using the physiological approach were
also published. The first work by Tart 18 and the later work by Lloyd 19
both follow a similar pattern. Basically, a subject is closeted in an
electrically shielded room while his EEG is recorded. Meanwhile, in
another laboratory, a second person is stimulated from time to time,
and the time for that stimulus is marked on the magnetic tape recording
of the subject's EEG. The subject does not know when the remote
stimulus periods occur.
At SRI three facilities are in use for the purpose described above.
One is a standard EEG facility under the direction of Dr. Charles
Rebert, Life Sciences Division. This facility consists of a visually
opaque, acoustically and electrically shielded, double-walled steel,
room, as shown in Figure 22, a Grass Model 5 polygraph, and an Ampex
Approved For Rele NC/L3jS?4-F1I f()0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releae~/0 1k:.SS-WIt~q-~999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releast2pN31A SI5 f P1E-D999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaU (bAUDP?91999A000400030001-7
SP-300 magnetic tape recorder. The second facility is a standard EEG
facility under the direction of Dr. Jerry Lukas, head of SRI's Sleep Studies
program. This facility consists of two sound-isolated rooms with
appropriate signal lead connections, an eight-channel polygraph for
recording visually, and a magnetic tape/computer processing/printer
readout that provides on-line processing of the polygraph data. In our
configuration we obtain a hardcopy printout of five-second averages of eight
channels of polygraph information 15 minutes following a. 15-minute run.
At present we monitor broad band alpha (7 to 14 Hz) and beta (14 to 34 Hz)
brainwave components from the left and right occipital regions, galvanic
skin response, and two channels of plethysmograph data (blood volume and
pulse height).
The third facility is a smaller, semiportable four-channel polygraph
with a GSR channel, reflected-light plethysmograph indicating blood
volume/pulse height, one channel of unfiltered EEG activity, and a
fourth EEG channel with zero-crossing digital filtering. The last
permits percent-time measurements in any band, with upper and lower band
edge settings in one-hertz increments.
Two lines of investigation were pursued in the SRI program. The
first was basic in nature, an effort to determine whether, in a re-
peatable experiment under laboratory conditions, the remote viewing of
a specific stimulus (strobe light in another laboratory) would provide
any evidence of EEG correlates. The second involved mid-experiment
monitoring of a number of physiological parameters during routine
experimentation in remote viewing.
1. Remote Strobe Experiment
The following is a description of the first line of experi-
mentation, the remote viewing of a strobe light stimulus. With regard
to choice of stimulus, it was noted that in previous work others had
attempted, without success, to detect evoked potential changes in a
subject's EEG in response to a single flash stimulus observed by
another subject.20 In a discussion of that experiment, Kamiya suggested
that because of the unknown temporal characteristics of the information
channel, it might be more appropriate to use repetitive bursts of light
Approved For ReleaU e11 iINF S:SJ 999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release/~'31t0.A J$bD99A000400030001-7
to increase the probahlility of detecting information transfer. 21 There-
fore, in our study we chose to use a stroboscopic flash train of ten seconds
duration as the remote stimulus.
In the design of the study, we assumed that the application of
the remote stimulus would result in responses similar to those obtained
under conditions of direct stimulation. For example, when an individual
is stimulated with a low-frequency (< 30 Hz) flashing light, the EEG
typically shows a decrease in the amplitude of the resting rhythm and a
driving of the brain waves at the frequency of the flashes.22 We hypo-
thesized that if we stimulated one subject in this manner (a putative sender)
the EEG of another subject in a remote room with no flash present (a
receiver) might show changes in narrow band alpha (9 to 11 Hz) activity
and possibly an EEG driving similar to that of the sender, either by
coupling to the sender's EEG,23 or by coupling directly to the stimulus.
We informed c=ur subject (S4) that at certain times a light
was to be flashed in a sender's eyes in a distant room, and if the subject
perceived that event, consciously or unconsciously, it might be evident
from changes in his EEG output. The instructions to the subject are
in accordance with requirements governing activities with human subjects
(see Appendix B). The receiver was seated in the visually opaque,
acoustically and electrically shielded double-walled steel room shown in
Figure 22. The sender was seated in room about seven meters from the
receiver.
A Grass PS-2 photostimulator placed about one meter in front
of the sender was used to present flash trains of ten seconds duration.
The receiver's EEG activity from the occipital region (Oz), referenced
to linked mastoids, was amplified with a Grass 5P-1 preamplifier and
associated driver amplifier with a bandpass of 1 to 120 Hz. The EEG
data were recorded on magnetic tape with an Ampex SP 300 recorder.
On each trial-, a tone burst of fixed frequency was presented
to both sender and receiver and was followed in one second by either a
ten second train of flashes or a null flash interval presented to the sender.
Thirty-six such trials were given in an experimental session, consisting
Approved For ReleaJJMC(GL/A5SIf I ED 999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaI f ?/A JPf LDD999A000400030001-7
of 12 null trials--no flashes following the tone--12 trials of flashes
at 6 fps and 12 trials of flashes at 16 fps, all randomly intermixed,
determined by entries from a table of random numbers. Each of the trials
generated an 11 second EEG epoch. The last 4 seconds of the epoch was
selected for analysis to minimize the desynchronising action of the warning
cue. This 4 second segment was subjected to Fourier analysis on a LINC
8 computer.
Spectrum analyses gave no evidence of EEG driving in any
receiver, although in control runs the receivers did exhibit driving
when physically stimulated with the flashes.
Data from seven sets of 36 trials each were collected from
the subject on three separate days. This comprises all the data collected
with this subject under the test conditions described above. The alpha
band was identified from average spectra, then scores of average power
and peak power were obtained from individual trials and subjected to
statistical analysis.
Figure 23 shows an overlay of the three averaged spectra from
one of the subject's 36-trial runs, displaying differences in alph.t
activity for the three stimulus conditions.
Mean values for the average power and peak power for each of
the seven experimental sets are given in Table 20. The power measures
were less in the 16 fps case than in the 0 fps in all seven peak-power
measures and in six out of seven average-power measures. Note also the
reduced effect in the case in which the subject was informed that no sender
was present (Run 3). It seems that overall alpha production was
reduced for this run in conjunction with the subject's expressed appre-
hension about conducting the experiment without a sender. This is in
contrast to the case (Run 7) in which the subject was not informed.
Siegel's two-tailed t approximation to the nonparametric
randomization test24 was applied to the data from all sets, which in-
cluded the two sessions in which the sender was removed. Average power
on trials associated with the occurrence of 16 fps was significantly less
(-24%) than when there were no flashes (t = 2.09, d.f. = 118, P < 0.04).
Approved For ReleaU 100UA D~Fo 999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release r2QA~11 ~ItfBR7t~Q999A000400030001-7
. .
5 Hz 10 Hz 15 Hz
THREE CASES -- 0, 6 and 16 Hz flashes (12 trial averages)
FIGURE 23 OCCIPITAL EEG FREQUENCY SPECTRA, 0 TO 20 Hz, OF SUBJECT S4
ACTING AS RECEIVER, SHOWING AMPLITUDE CHANGES IN THE 9 TO
11-Hz BAND AS A FUNCTION OF STROBE FREQUENCY
Approved For Releast Rfp 9 1A SSFII E. 99A000400030001-7
Approved For Release 2003/03/10 : CIA-RDP79-00999A000400030001-7
TABLE 20
EEG Data for Subject S4 Showing Average Power and Peak Power in the
9- to 11-Hz Band, as a Function of Flash Frequency and Sender.
(Each Table Entry is an Average Over 12 Trials)
M
v
Flash
equency
Sender
0
Average Power
6 16
Peak Power
0 6 16
J.L.
94.8
84.1
76.8
357.7
329.2
289.6
R.T.
41.3
45.5
37.0
160.7
161.0
125.0
No Sender
25.1
35.7
28.2
87.5
95.7
81
7
(Subject
.
informed)
J.L.
54.2
55.3
44.8
191.4
170.5
149.3
J.L.
56.8
50.9
32.8
240.6
178.0
104.6
R.T.
39.8
24.9
30.3
145.2
74.2
122.1
No Sender
86.0
53.0
52.1
318.1
180.6
202
3
(Subject not
.
informed)
Averages
56.8
49.9
43.1
214.5
169.8
153.5
-12%
-24%
-21%
-28%
(P 3:1, re-
spectively, paralleling our results.28
d. Discussion of Physical Perturbation Effects
One significance of the perturbation of remote sensitive
equipment lies in the indication that the remote-sensing channel may
*
Superconducting Technology Cryogenic Magnetometer.
113
Approved For ReleU jLAC6614fiJ&D0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releas9 / 5f 1EI999A000400030001-7
possess a bilateral aspect. That is, there is the possibility that an
information-bearing signal can be coupled from an individual to a re-
mote location as well as in reverse, thus implying that the information
channel under consideration may sustain information transfer in either
direction.
The above concept has a rigorous basis in quantum theory
in the so-called "observer problem," the effect of an observer on experi-
mental measurement. In quantum theory it is recognized that although
the evoi_ution of a physical system proceeds deterministically on the basis
of" Schrodinger's equation (or its equivalent), the result of a calcula-
t.E_on is not in general the prediction of a well-defined value for some
experimental variable. Rather, it is the prediction of a range of
possibi.ii.ties with a certain distribution of probabilities. In a given
neasuremenr, however, some particular value for a variable is actually
obtained, which implies that an additional event--so-called state vector
collapse--must take place during the measurement process itself and in
manner that is unpreaictable except probabilistically. Analysis of
the significance of this latter process leads inescapably to the conclusion
that to the degree that consciousness is involved in observation and measure-
ment (and it always is), to that degree consciousness must also be seen
to interact with the physical environment and to participate in the collapse
ni the state vector. Efforts to extract quantum theory from this conclu-
;i.on by. for example, an infinite regression of measuring apparatus,
have proved unsuccessful. These conclusions, arrived at by theorists
such as Wigner,.9 imply the possibility of nontrivial coupling between
consciousness and quantum states of the physical environment at an
extremeiy fundamental level. Such a realization has led to theories
(it paranormal phenomena modeled on the basis of this so-called "ob-
;server problem" in quantum theory. =b
The phenomena implied by the observer problem are generally
unobservable on the gross macroscopic scale for statistical reasons.
This is codified in the thermodynamic concept that for an isolated
system. entropy (disorder) on the average increases, effectively masking
the microscopic observer effects. It is just this requirement of
Approved For Relea ftG iAS SIEI Ea999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReIe# 'LAS Folf tE7D0999A000400030001-7
isolation, however, that can be expected to be weakened under conditions
of efforts at paranormal perturbation via the remote-sensing channel,
and it can be argued that observer effects would be maximally operative
in just those situations where the intrusion'of consciousness as an
ordering phenomenon could result in a significant local reversal of
entropy increase.
These considerations lead to the following series of
conceptualizations or hypotheses around which future experiments can
be designed.
(1) Researchers in the area of willed pertur-
bation effects appear to be plagued by results
whose amplitudes have a signal-to-noise ratio
near unity, regardless of the process or mechanism
involved. This may indicate that, rather than
simple perversity, what is being articulated by
the experimental results is a coherence pheno-
mena involving partial mobilization of system noise,
as if the components of the noise spectrum had been
brought into phase coherence, and thus the mag-
nitude constraint. The subject would thus appear
to act as a local negentropic (that is, entropy-
decreasing) source. If true, it may be more
advantageous as a practical matter to work with
extremely noisy systems, rather than with highly
constrained or organized systems, so as to maxi-
mize possible effects due to the introduction
of order.
(2) Willed perturbation effects often appear to
be more the result of coincidence that the effect
of a well-defined cause. Again, rather than being
the result of the perversity of nature, the ob-
served goal-oriented synchronicity may indicate
that physical systems are more easily manipulated
Approved For ReIeUNI?3IQAS $II\F1JED00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release_ 0 /999A000400030001-7
at. the Y obal level of boundary conditions and
!?lurscraiuts rather than at the level of mechan-
isi. 'thus, the apparency that a given result
may be explained away by a coincidental but
"natural" event needs to be explored more fully.
tinexpeccea- but natural causes may be the effect
.3i a :at?r i_es of causal links, outside the defined
t,xl)erl_r~eatal boundaries but representing an un-
ioresee=r lK.ne of least resistance. At worst, such
b?a=1sa i inks may in fact be unobservable in the
erase of the hidden variables concept in quan-
s.:um theory, but nevertheless act as instruments
of the W it L.
(_3) Wilted perturbation effects appear to be
ni r?lrti Wa iiy spontaneous; i.e., it is difficult
cr evoke such effects "on cue," with the result
hat the phenomenon is often considered to not be
under aooc control, and therefore not amenable
-o controlled experimentation. This difficulty
is so pronounced that it is likely that we are
:~bservin- some macroscopic analog of a quantum
r.rrd u., an event similarly unpredictable in
Lir:.fe ex eft as a probability function. it the
analogy is correct, experimentation in this area
rimpiv heeds to be treated in the manner or, for
x mne. weak photon experiments.
(4) 1'o:.-3 bly related to Item (3), the more closely
Arne actcmvts to observe willed perturbation effects,
the less likely one is to see them, a factor con-
>idered -)v many to support hypotheses of poor
4,bservaL.fraud, and the like. To a sophis-
a:-icatec observer, however, simple dismissal does
Approved For Releas1200 fPP9D999A000400030001-7
"- TMMA *IF *a
Approved For Rele NC/e31&OCSi fg171Y0999A000400030001-7 #AF
not stand up under scrutiny. Invoking again
the idea of a microscopic analog of a quantum
transition, we may, as observers of delicate
phenomena, be witnesses to observer effects
generally associated with the uncertainty prin-
ciple. Paradoxically, from the subject's view-
point, the production of the phenomena may also
be an observer effect, perturbing as it does the
expected behavior of a piece of instrumentation.
In this model the scrutiny of psychokinetic pheno-
mena under laboratory conditions could in prin-
ciple be considered to be a collective phenomena
involving interfering observer effects in a manner
known to occur at the microscopic quantum level.
(5) Finally, it may be useful as a guiding prin-
ciple to continually recognize that all of the
phenomena we deal with in macroscopic psycho-
energetics are totally permissible at the micro-
scopic level within the framework of physics as
presently understood. It is simply that time
reversibility, tunneling through barriers, simul-
taneous multiple-state occupation, and so on are
generally unobservable as gross macroscopic phen-
omena for statistical reasons only, as codified
in the concept of increasing disorder (entropy).
Therefore, it may be appropriate to consider an
individual with psychokinetic abilities primarily
as a source of ordering phenomena of sufficient
magnitude so as to restructure the otherwise
random statistics of the macroscopic environment.
2. Disscussion of Possible "Mechanisms"in Remote Viewing
With regard to the wider problem of the remote-viewing channel
itself, beyond the specific aspects of equipment perturbation via
117
Approved For Rele.UNC/.3S54-II?D0999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasiA3M
Ct1A:S604-0D999A000400030001-7
,144
this channel there is considerable current interest in quantum theory
in the implications brought on by the observation- 0,31 of nonlocal
correlation or "quantum interconnectedness" (to use Bohm's term `) of
distant parts of quantum systems of macroscopic dimensions. Bell's
theorem 3 emphasizes that no theory of reality compatible with quantum
theory can require spatially separated events to be independent,
but must permit interconnectedness of distant events in a manner that
is "contrary" to "common.sense" 351,36 This prediction has been
experimentally tested and confirmed in the recent experiments of, for
example, Freedman and ulauser. 0,31 F.H. Walker and 0. Costa de Beauregard,
independently proposing theories of paranormal functioning based on
quantum concepts, argue that observer effects open the door to the
possibility of nontrivial, coupling between consciousness and the environ-
ment, and that the nonlocality principle permits such coupling to
transcend spatial and temporal barriers. 26,37
IM alternative hypothesis (that is, alternative to the
specifically quantum hypothesis) has been put forward by I.M. Kogan,
Chairman of the Bioinformation Section of the Moscow Board of the
Popov Society, USSR. He is a Soviet engineer who until 1969 published
"!xrensively in the open literature on the theory of paranormal communi-
cation. t8-41 His hypothesis is that information transfer under con-
ditions of sensory shielding is mediated by extremely-low-frequency (ELF)
electromagnetic waves in the 300- to 1000-km region, a proposal which does
not seem to he ruled out by any obvious physical or biological facts.
Experimental support for the hypothesis is claimed on the basis of:
slower than inverse-square attenuation, compatible with source-per-
cipient distances lying in the induction field range as opposed to the
radiation field range; observed low bit rates (0.005 to 0.1 bit/s)
compatible with the information-carrying capacity of ELF waves; apparent
ineffectiveness of ordinary electromagnetic shielding as an attenuator;
and standard antenna calculations entailing biologically generated
currents yielding results compatible with observed signal-to-noise
ratios.
Approved For ReleaCoIS/AIEISQ999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaU CL/A 9?0999A000400030001-7
M. Persinger, Psychophysiology Laboratory, Laurentian University,
Toronto, Canada, has narrowed the ELF hypothesis to the suggestion
that the 7.8-Hz "Shumann waves", and their harmonics propagating along
the earth-ionosphere waveguide duct, may be responsible. Such an
hypothesis is compatible with driving by brain-wave currents, and leads
to certain hypotheses, such as asymmetry between east-west and west-east
propagation, preferred experimental times (midnight to 4:a.m.),and expected
negative correlation between success and the U index (a measure of geo-
magnetic disturbance throughout the world). Persinger claims initial
support for these factors on.the basis`. of a literature search.42,43
On the negative side with regard to a straightforward ELF
interpretation as a blanket hypothesis are: (a) apparent real-time
descriptions of remote activities in sufficient detail to require a
channel capacity in all probability greater than that allowed by a
conventional modulation of an ELF signal: (b) lack of a proposed
mechanism for coding and decoding the information onto the proposed
ELF carrier; and (c) apparent precognition data. The hypothesis must
nonetheless remain open at this stage of research, since it is con-
ceivable that counterindication (a) may eventually be circumvented on
the basis that the apparent high bit rate results from a mixture of low-
bit-rate input and high-bit-rate "filling in the blanks" from imagina-
tion; counterindication (b) is common to a number of normal perceptual
tasks and may therefore simply reflect a lack of sophistication on our
part with regard to perceptual functioning; 44
and counterindication
(c) may be accommodated by an ELF hypothesis if advanced waves as
well as retarded waves are admitted .27,45
Experimentation to determine
whether the ELF hypothesis is viable can be carried out by the use of
ELF sources as targets, by the study of parametric dependence on pro--
pagational directions and diurnal timing, and by the exploration of
interference effects caused by creation of a high-intensity ELF environ-
ment during experimentation, all of which are under consideration as part
of a proposed follow-up program in our laboratory.
119
Approved For Rele M 51IR4 "V00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Rele ( 3/ASCMMED0999A000400030001-7
The move arguments are not intended to indicate that we
understand the precise nature of the information channel coupling remote
events and human perception. Rather, we intend only to show that modern
theory is not without resources that can be brought to hear on the pro-
blems at hand, and it is our expectation that these problems will,
with further work, yield to analysis and specification.
Approved For Rele CI S5JI-FIIFED0999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleiNCv S5d-Fd1ED0999A000400030001-7
3. Communication Theory Approach to Channel Utilization
Independent of the mechanisms that may be involved in remote
sensing, observation of the phenomenon implies the existence of an infor-
mation channel in the information-theoretic sense. Since such channels
are amenable to analysis on the basis of communication theory techniques,
channel characteristics, such as bit rate, can be determined independent
of a well-defined underlying theory in the sense that thermodynamic con-
cepts can be applied to the analysis of systems independent of underlying
mechanisms. Therefore, the collection of data under specified conditions
permits headway to be made despite the formidable work that needs to be
done to clarify the underlying bases of the phenomena.
One useful application of the communication channel concept was
the utilization of such a channel for error-free transmission of informa-
tion by the use of redundancy coding. The experiment was carried out by
Dr. Milan Ryzl, a chemist with the Institute of Biology of the Czecho-
slovakian Academy of Science. He reasoned that a paranormal channel
exhibits the attributes of a communication channel perturbed by noise,
and that redundancy coding could be used to combat the effects of the
noisy channel in a straightforward application of communication theory.8
Ryzl had an assistant randomly select five groups of three decimal digits
each. These 15 digits were then encoded into binary form and translated
into a sequence of green and white cards sealed in opaque envelopes.
With the use of a subject who has produced highly significant results
with many contemporary researchers, 46-51 he was able, by means of redun-
dant calling and an elaborate majority vote protocol, to correctly
identify all 15 numbers, a result significant at p = 10-15. The experi-
ment required 19,350 calls, averaging nine seconds per call. The hit
rate for individual calls was 61.9 percent, 11,978 hits and 7,372 misses. 4`i'
Note added in proof. It has been brought to our attention that a similar
procedure was used to transmit without error the word "peace," in inter-
national Morse Code; J.C. Carpenter "Toward the Effective Utilization of
Enhanced Weak-Signal ESP Effects," presented at the annual meeting of
the American Association for the Advancement of Science, New York,
Jan. 27, 1975.
121
Approved For Rele.9d AD'3'YAP& Al-URTY00999A000400030001-7
Approved For Releas / 999A000400030001-7
discussed in the section on the random target generator, the
bit rate is calculated from
R = H(x) - (t,, (x)
where H(x) is the uncertainty of the source message containing symbols
with a priori probability p.
it (x) Pilog.,Pi
and HV (x) is the conditional entropy based on the a posteriori probabil-
ities that a received signal was actually transmitted,
P(i,j)Iog2P1(j)
For the above run, with D. = 1/2, pj(j) = 0.619, and an average time of
nine seconds per choice. we have a source uncertainty H(x) = 1 bit and a
calculated bit rate
0.041 ?-lts/symbol
or
?i/'t' = 0.0(146 bits/second.
Since the 15-digit number (;49.8 bits) was actually transmitted at the
rate of 2.9 x 10-4 bits per second, an increase in bit rate by a factor
of about 20 could be expected on the basis of a coding scheme more optimum
than that used in the experiments. The actual bit rate is roughly the
;ame as that observed in our random target generator experiment discussed
earlier.
An excellent reaundancy coding technique for a communication
channel is the sequential sampling procedure used earlier in Section II-B
for the sorting of SW from non-SW cards. In this application of the se-
quential sampling procedure, one would first express the message to be
sent as a series of binary digits, encoded, for example, as shown in
']'able 22. The sequential method then gives a rule of procedure for making
one of three possible decisions following the.receipt of each bit:
accept 1 as the bit being transmitted; reject 1 as the bit being transmitted
Approved For Relea li' /& :59Wt'~D999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleJNI"SQA-FIJFED0999A000400030001-7
Five-Bit Code for Alphanumeric Characters
E 00000 Y 01000
T 11111 G,J 10111
N 00001 W 01001
R 11110 V 10110
I 00010 B 01010
0 11101 0 10101
A 00011 1 01011
S,X,Z 11100 2 10100
D 00100 3 01100
H 11011 4 10011
L 00101 5 01101
C,K.Q 11010 6 10010
F 00110 7 01110
P 11001 8 10001
U 00111 9 01111
M 11000 10000
Note: Alphabet characters listed in order of decreasing frequency in
English text. See, for example, A. Sinkov, Elementary Cryptanalysis
--A Mathematical Approach.52 (The low frequency letters, X,,Z,K,Q,
and J have been grouped with similar characters to provide space
for numerics in a five-bit code.) In consideration of the uneven
distribution of letter frequencies in English text, this code is
chosen such that 0 and 1 have equal probability.
(i.e., accept 0); or continue transmission of the bit under consideration.
As discussed earlier, use of the sequential sampling procedure
requires the specification of parameters that are determined on the basis
of the following considerations. Assume that a message bit (0 or 1) is
being transmitted. In the absence of a priori knowledge, we may assume
equal probability (p = 0.5) for the two possibilities (0,1) if an encoding
procedure like that of Table 22 is used. Therefore, from the standpoint
of the receiver, the probability of correctly identifying the bit being
transmitted is p = 0.5 because of chance alone. An operative remote
Approved For Relek) 20D;tM~iE k 11 -00999A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaseJt 3?tA9SII DP99A000400030001-7
sensing channel could then be expected to alter the probability of cor-
r re.ct identification to a value p = 0.5 + q., where the parameter ~ satis-
f es 0 1I,I < 0.5. (The quantity may be positive or negative, depending
on whetner the paranormal channel results in so-called psi-hitting or psi-
missing.) Good psi functioning on a repetitive task is observed to result
i_ w= 0.1.2, as reported by Ryzl.4 Therefore, to indicate the design
procedure, let us assume a baseline psi parameter 0, = 0.1 and design a
communication system on tnis basis.
he question to be addressed is whether, upon repeated trans-
mission, a given message bit is labeled a "1" at a low rate p commensurate
with the hypothesis H that the bit in question is a "0", or at a higher ill rate PI commensurate with the hypothesis H1 that the bit in question is
indeed a "I". The decision making process requires the specification of
tour parameters:
f : The probability of labeling incorrectly a "0" message
!,it. as The probability of labeling correctly a
""0"' as "0 ' is p = 0.5 + 'U = 0.6. Therefore, the
probabii i ty of labeling incorrectly a "0" as a "I" is
p,: The probability of labeling correctly a '1T' message bit
.z a "I". . -> iven by p 1 = 0.5 + t~ = 0.6.
m . : The probability of rejecting a correct identification
for a "t:" ((Type I error). We shall take a = 0.01.
The probability of accepting an incorrect identification
r a (Type II error). We shall take 6 = 0.01.
With the parameters thus specified, the sequential sampling pro-
c-c=~dure provides for construction of a decision graph as shown in Figure
Thee equations for the upper and lower limit lines are, respectively,
+ sn
- 10 0-M Approved For Releases /OL/A: 4ED 999A000400030001-7 ftff 41A,
Approved For Release UNI'.3Yt0 St"E 9A000400030001-7
z
w 50
Q0
z
cn w
Ha 40 ~-
c3 30
LL Q
Ow
Cr 2 20
M
DECISION 1
Accept "1" as
the Bit Being
Transmitted
2 w DECISION 2
Accept "0" as
x 10 the Bit Being
w
> F- Transmitted
F- z
< 0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80
FIGURE 27 ENHANCEMENT OF SIGNAL-TO-NOISE RATIO BY
SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING PROCEDURE (p0 = 0.4,
p, = 0.6, a = 0.01, a = 0.01)
Approved For ReleasUNCLLA f 99A000400030001-7
Approved For Releas fqf43iI1 A. ISY7 L D999A000400030001-7
1 1--n
log- --
A cumulative record of receiver-generated responses to the target bit is
cuimmpileci until either the upper or lower limit line is reached, at which
point a decision is made to accept 0 or 1 as the bit being transmitted.
Channel reliability (probability of correctly determining
message being transmitted) as a function of operative psi parameter i is
plotted in Figure 28. As observed, the sequential sampling procedure
an result in 90 percent or greater reliability with psi parameters of
the order of a few percent. Figure 29 indicates the average number of
trials required to reach a decision on a given message bit. The average
number of- trials falls oft rapidly as a function of increasing psi param-
eLers y .
?molementation of the sequential sampling procedure requires
the transmission of a message coded in binary digits. Therefore, the
target space must consist of dichotomous elements such as the white and
green cards used in the experiments by Ryzl.
ii coueration. a sequence corresponding to the target bit (0
or 1) is sent and the cumulative entries are made (Figure 27) until a
decision is reached to accept either a 1 or U as the bit being transmitted.
At a prearranged time, the next sequence is begun and continues as above
until the entire message has been received. A useful alternative.
which rei_ieves the percipient of the burden of being aware of his self-
contradiction from trial to trial, consists of cycling through the entire
un ssage repetitively, entering each response on its associated graph
until a decision has been reached on all message bits.
I-om the results obtained in such experiments, the channel bit
rate can be ascertained tor the system configuration under consideration.
Approved For Release ? ~`.-L'A. 11 1~ LF 99A000400030001-7
Approved For Release/~'31~0A 4c D99A000400030001-7
>- cm
c 0.6
M
-0 y
O OJ
Q.rpi
0.4
E
J
M E 0.2
I I I I I I 11
+0.4 +0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4
0 (psi parameter)
FIGURE 28 RELIABILITY CURVE FOR SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING
PROCEDURE (po = 0.4, p, = 0.6, a = 0.01, 0 = 0.01)
Approved For Releas1Na :LlA sl5-&RK-a99A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleasF1(,~0l/p3/10~ ~Vrffu"AO00400030001-7
d-,...i
+0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4
j (psi parameter)
t GUHF 29 AVERAGE SAMPLE NUMBER
FOR SEQUENTIAL SAMPLING
PROCEDURE (p0 0.4, p1 = 0.6,
=e=O101,f3=0.01)
Approved For Release NCL0 SRfKE(D9A000400030001-7
Approved For Relea e" W1 . J-I!TPOO999AOOO4OOO3OOOl-7
Furthermore, bit rates for other degrees of reliability (i.e., for other
p0'pl' a, and S) can be estimated by construction of other decision curves
over the same data base and thus provide a measure of the bit rate per
degree of reliability.
In summary, the procedures described here can provide a speci-
fication of the characteristics of a remote sensing channel under well-
defined conditions. These procedures also provide for a determination
of the feasibility of such a channel for particular applications.
4. Soviet Efforts
This discussion would be incomplete if we did not mention certain
aspects of the current state of research in the USSR. Since the 1930s
in the laboratory of L. Vasiliev (Leningrad Institute for Brain Research),
there has been an interest in the use of paranormal communication as a
method of influencing the behavior of a person at a distance. In Vasiliev's
book Experiments in Mental Suggestion, 53 he makes it clear that the bulk
of his laboratory's experiments were aimed at long-distance communication
and what we would today call behavior modification; for example, putting
people to sleep at a distance through hypnosis.
The behavior modification type of experiment has been carried
out in recent times by I.M. Kogan. He was concerned with three principal
kinds of experiments: mental suggestion without hypnosis over short
distances, in which the percipient attempts to identify an object;
mental awakening over short distances, in which a subject is awakened
from a hypnotic sleep at the "beamed" suggestion from the hypnotist;
and long-range (intercity) paranormal communication. 39 ,Kogan's main
interest has been to quantify the channel capacity of the paranormal
channel. He finds that the bit rate decreases from 0.1 bits per second
for laboratory experiments to 0.005 bits per second for his 1000-km
intercity experiments.
As indicated earlier, in the USSR serious consideration is given
to the hypothesis that paranormal communication is mediated by extremely-
low-frequency (ELF) electromagnetic propagation. In general, the entire
field of paranormal research in the USSR is part of a larger one concerned
with the interaction between electromagnetic fields and living organisms. 54,55
Approved For Relea e' c/0 ll1 LPOO999AOOO4OOO3OOO1 -7
Approved For Releasp~,2 @ 1f1, JIt1 P i E-D999A000400030001-7
At the first lnternat:ional Congress on Parapsychology and Psychotronics
in Prague, Czechoslovakia, in 1973, for example, Kholodov spoke at length
about the susceptibility of living systems to extremely low-level ac and
do fields. He described conditioning effects on the behavior of fish from
the application of 1.0 to 100 }iW of RF to their tank.'h The USSR take
these data seriously in that the Soviet safety requirements for
steady- microwave exposure set limits at 10 uW/cm whereas the United
States has set a steady-state limit of 10 mW/cm`."' Kholodov spoke also
about the nonthermal effects of microwaves on animals' central nervous
systems. His experiments were very carefully carried out and are char-
ucteristic of a new dimension in paranormal. research both in the USSR
~~nd elsewhere.
the increasing importance of this area in Soviet research was
indicated recently when the Soviet Psychological Association issued an
unprecedented position paper calling on the Soviet Academy of Sciences
to step up efforts in this area. 'S The Association recommended that the
cew.y formed Psychological Institute within the Soviet Academy of Sciences
and the Psychological Institute of the Academy of Pedagogical Sciences
review the area and consider the creation of a new laboratory within one
of the institutes to study persons with unusual abilities. They also
recommended a comprehensive evaluation of experiments and theory by the
Academy of Sciences' Institute of Biophysics and Institute for the
'rnblems of Information Transmission.
Cone los ions
"It is the province of natural science to investigate nature,
impartially and without prejudice." 59 Nowhere in scientific inquiry has
this dictum met as great a challenge as in the area of so-called para-
oorma perception, the detection of remote stimuli not mediated by the
usual sensory processes. Such phenomena, although under scientific con-
sideration for over a century, have historically been fraught with unre-
liability and controversy, and validation of the phenomena by accepted
scientific methodology has been slow in coming. Even so, a recent survey
conducted by the British oublication New Scientist revealed that 67 percent
of nearly 1500 responding readers (the majority of whom are working
Approved For Release 1 S If ~UP99A000400030001-7
Approved For ReleaU UCLA WFHD0999AO00400030001-7
scientists and technologists) considered paranormal perception to be an
established fact or a likely possibility, and 88 percent held the investi-
gation of paranormal perception to be a legitimate scientific under-
taking- 60
A review of the literature reveals that although well-conducted
experiments by reputable researchers yielding reproducible results were
begun over a century ago (e.g., Sir William Crookes' study of D.D. Home,
1860s),61,62 many consider the study of these phenomena as only recently
emerging from the realm of quasi-science. One reason for this is that,
despite experimental results, no satisfactory theoretical construct had
been advanced to correlate data or to predict new experimental outcomes.
Consequently, the area in question remained for a long time in the
recipe state reminiscent of electrodynamics before the unification brought
about by the work of Ampere, Faraday, and Maxwell. Since the early work,
however, we have seen the development of quantum theory, information
theory, and neurophysiological research, and these disciplines provide
powerful conceptual tools that appear to bear directly on the issue. In
fact, several leading physicists are now of the opinion that, contrary
to "common sense" notions, these phenomena are not at all inconsistent
with the framework of modern physics: the often-held view that observa-
tions of this type are a priori incompatible with known laws is erroneous,
such a concept being based on the naive realism prevalent before the
development of quantum theory. In the emerging view it is accepted that
research in this area can be conducted so as to uncover not just a catalog
of interesting events, but rather patterns of cause-effect relationships
of the type that lend themselves to analysis and hypothesis in the forms
with which we are familiar in the physical sciences.
Accordingly, we consider it important to continue data collection
and to encourage others to do likewise; investigations such as those
reported here need replication and extension under as wide a variety of
rigorously controlled conditions as possible.
Approved For ReleaU NQLAS. FRU 9d0999A000400030001-7
Approved For Release RW-MTl&. I 999A000400030001-7
As a result of exploratory research on human perception carried out
hi SRI's Electronics and Bioengineering Laboratory, we initiated an inves-
?. gati_on of a perceptua channel whereby individuals can access by means
ti mental imagery and describe randomly-chosen remote sites located
:e.veral ;piles or more awav. In this final report, we document the
t:t=udv at SRI or this human information-accessing capability that we call
"cernot.e viewing," the characteristics of which appear to fall outside the
range or well-understood perceptual or information-processing abilities.
t3his phenomenon is one of a, broad class of abilities of certain individuals
to access by means of menra1 nrocesses and describe information sources
'docked from ordinary perception and generally accepted as secure against
:_;uch access. tndividuais exhibiting this faculty include not only SRI
subjects, but visiting staff members of the sponsoring organization who
:iarciciuated as subiects so as to critique the protocol.
the program was divided into two categories of approximately equal
;iori..--app1ied researr?ht and basic research. The applied research effort
explored the otierationai utility of the above perceptual abilities. The
zisi.c rP.