(Sanitized)RESEARCH DEVELOPMENT

Document Type: 
Document Number (FOIA) /ESDN (CREST): 
CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6
Release Decision: 
RIPPUB
Original Classification: 
K
Document Page Count: 
45
Document Creation Date: 
December 28, 2016
Document Release Date: 
October 27, 2004
Sequence Number: 
31
Case Number: 
Publication Date: 
January 1, 1966
Content Type: 
REPORT
File: 
AttachmentSize
PDF icon CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6.pdf2.33 MB
Body: 
Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000990940031-6 Declass Review by NGA. STAT Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 STAT Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 PYRGft ApPFGyed DESIGN DECISIONS Precision optical viewers provide high resolution over a large subject area. But they are costly, complex and inflexible when compared to this high-magnification electronic viewer. It offers rapid, remote-control display and also permits image enhancement. Zoom Viewer Display from electronic film viewer shown with both negative and dif- ferentiator controls energized. Fig. 1. The Electro-Zoom viewer provides continuously variable, in-focus system magnification of a 70-mm transparency between 7 and 150 times. Further step magnification up to 380 times is possible when viewed on a 24-in. monitor. The limit of resolution is better than 50 optical lines per millimeter at magnification factors of 150 or more. A flying-spot scanner is used as the image transducer., This relatively old and simple technique converts a three dimensional scene-X, Y, f (intensity)-into a two-dimensional (l, t) time sequential video signal. A lens focuses the raster on the trans- parency with the photo-multiplier directly behind the film holder. Jacob L. Breitbord John Main Itek Corp. Lexington, Mass. A NEW and inexpensive electronic film viewer simplifies interpretation of aerial and space-satellite reconnaissance photo- graphs. The system offers many advantages over more expensive and complex optical viewers. Among them are : Large screen display. Multiple viewing at remote locations. High magnification with minimum trans- mission of energy through the film (par- ticularly important in satellite photog- raphy). Ability to repeatedly view first-gener- ation film without danger of loss or phys- ical damage. Full choice of positive or negative view- ing. Real-time video processing. Availability of image enhancement tech- niques. Except for the scanner tube and power supplies, the system consists entirely of com- mercially available television components. It uses a magnification system that combines optical minification and shrinking raster techniques to provide apparent resolution of up to 50 optical lines per millimeter at mag- nification of up to 380 diameters. By packing the relatively few active lines generated by commercial television into a small dimension, and by selecting scanner systems, optics and film with the required transfer characteristics, it becomes possible to increase apparent, or usable, resolution by a big factor. Looked at in another way, the total num- ber of active lines remains constant, but the number of lines in a small increment on the transparency can be increased by an order of magnitude. Electronic `Zoom' Control Allows Variable Magnification The viewer provides a relatively smooth electronic magnification without defocusing. The technique used is termed "electronic Reprinted from ELECTRONIC DESIGN February 15, 1963 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 CPYRGH T zoom" because the enlargement of the image resembles that obtained with zoom lens sys- tems. The active vertical scanner lines are placed closer together until they overlap and are no longer resolvable on the face of the scanner tube, thus filling in the ,dead space. Writing speed in the horizontal direction decreases as the raster is shrunk, and bandwidth require- ments are correspondingly reduced. A smooth, approximately 4-time magnifi- W 0 60 Z 0 40 Fig. 2. The original scene is displayed undistorted when the same sweep-wave forms, identically timed, cause the electron beams of the scanner tube and the display tube to be slaved together. The video sig- nal coincidentally modulates the intensity of the dis- play tube. Here is the system square-wave response. Fig. 3. The scanner tube used in the Itek Electro-Zoom Viewer is a Westinghouse 5CE P16 cathode-ray tube. Spectral response of the phosphor is shown here. A P16 (short persistence) phosphor is used on the scanner tube-face. The light output from this phosphor decays to 10 per cent of its initial brightness within 0.15 psec. Using a shrinking raster technique and normalizing, a line width of less than 0.00175 in. is measured for the tube. The beam is electromagnetically deflected and electrostatically focused. The face plate is optically flat and non-browning. Final anode voltage is 20 Kv. cation range is obtained in this manner. Within this range, the smaller the line width produced on the scanner tube, the larger the magnification, and the greater the resolution available to the viewer. The zoom control shrinks the raster linear- ly in, both horizontal and vertical directions by decreasing the drive to the respective de- flection yokes. A 3 x 4 aspect ratio is held within 10 per cent at maximum raster size, and to better than 3 per cent at minimum raster size. Horizontal linearity is very good. The saw-tooth of current through the yoke, and the resultant linearity on the scanner tube-face as displayed by a vertical bar pat- tern, is well within 5 per cent. In the vertical, the sawtooth of current through the yoke is linear well within a 5 per cent tolerance. The shrunken raster can be positioned over the face of the scanner tube to approximately ?0.5 in. in the horizontal, and ?0.7 in. in the vertical. Several fixed stages of magnification, in addition to the continuously variable zoom control, are provided, for a total electronic magnification of 12. Photomultiplier Is Used As Light Transducer An RCA 6199 multiplier phototube is used as the light transducer. This is a head-on, 10- stage photomultiplier having an S-11 spec- tral response (Fig. 3). In a laboratory mock- up, it was shielded from stray magnetic fields by two layers of shielding material-Mu- metal and Conetic. The shielding was tested by holding a permanent magnet with a strength of 500 Gauss in contact with the shield close to the photocathode. Motion of the magnet had no visual effect on the video output. The shield is grounded for additional elec- trostatic shielding; and sufficient space and insulation are provided between the shield and the glass bulb to prevent internal dis- charge, which creates large noise spikes. The dark-current signal (no incident light on the photocathode) of the combination photomultiplier and preamplifier is less than 0.3 my at the cathode-follower output. Light- shielding structures minimize stray light from external sources as well as scattered light from the scanner tube; this keeps noise to an acceptable level. In order to decrease noise in the signal by Fig. 4. Display with normal roster on scanner tube. Reprinted from ELECTRONIC DESIGN February 15, 1963 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 T Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 a factor of four dynode supply voltage was decreased from 1100 to 900 volts. Beam cur- rent of the scanner tube was then increased by a factor of 15 to compensate for the loss in sensitivity. A Conrac CGB-24 monitor (24-in.-diagon- al) serves as a primary display; but the sys- tem is designed to incorporate many different models of standard television monitors. Dis- plays 10 to 24 in. on the diagonal are avail- able for direct viewing. Projection-type TV viewers can also be used in the system. Linearity, contrast and brightness of the CGB-24 are more than adequate for system Fig. 5. Display with zoom control energized, resulting in a shrunken raster on the scanner tube. Fig. 6. Incorporation of video processing increases flexibility for the photointerpreter. A two-position switch shifts a positive dispI y to a negative display. This provides video from thefplate of the output tube, which is 180 degrees out o phase with the polarity of the signal from the photoultiplier output (negative video). Here is the display with the negative control energized. Fig. 7. Display with both negative and zoom controls energized. Fig. 8. A simple differentiating network also is in- corporated to provide a three-dimensional, bas-relief display. A very short RC-time constant is inserted in the video line. This network attenuates the low fre- quencies drastically, but provides an increasing re- sponse to the high frequencies. As a result, only the sharp transitions appear on the face of the monitor tube. The nonlinear phase function of this simple dif- ferentiator creates the three-dimensional effect shown here. requirements. Bandwidth specifications of ?1 db at 10 me have been verified by labo- ratory tests. The Blonder-Tongue Labs, Inc., TVC-1B- CG is used as the synchronizing pulse (sync) generator. It is a.relatively simple and inex- pensive unit. The horizontal sync pulses are generated by a 15,750-cps oscillator, which can be phase-locked to a 60-cps power line. The vertical sync pulses are obtained by mul- tiplying the horizontal by a factor of two to 31,500 cps; then counting down by factors of 3, 5, 7, and 5, to 60 pulses per second. ^ ^ E:. Reprinted from ELECTRONIC DESIGN February 15, 1963 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 STAT Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 COPY NO. AT HUMAN FACTORS ASPECTS OF PHOTO INTERPRETATION by STAT December 15, 1965 - March 15, 1966 1 STAT Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 1. INTRODUCTION 2. PROJECT ORGANIZATION AND PERSONNEL 3. AREAS OF INVESTIGATION 3.1 The Visual System as a Servomechanism 3.2 Visual Acuity 3.2.1 Factors affecting visual acuity 3.2.1.1 Luminance 3.2.1.2 Contrast 3.2.1.3 Adaptation 3.2.1.4 Wavelength 3.2.1.5 Optical variables 3.2.1.6 Eye movements 3.3 Screen Dither 3.4 Error Keys and the Effect of Prior Information on Photo Interpretation 3.5 Display Format and Search Procedure 4. FUTURE TASKS REFERENCES' PREVIOUS PUBLICATIONS SENT TO PROGRAM MONITOR EXAMPLES OF SCREEN MOTION APPENDIX A - ARMED FORCES NRC COMMITTEE ON VISION MEMBERSHIP LIST Page No. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t +T STAT During the reporting period, efforts were directed toward extracting limited and specific areas of concentration from a vast quantity of publi- cations on the broad and general subject called "vision." This winnowing process still leaves numerous areas of interest, so a shotgun approach has been taken to discuss many of these points. It is hoped that through better understanding of the psychophysical process of the visual system of the human observer, the photo interpreter can be aided in his vital task of reconnaissance and intelligence. The project continues under the direction of During the month of November 1965, investigator succeeding became principal Since that time, the program facility on two occasions to discuss and define some fields of interest. These areas of investigation included the compilation of lists of institutions, organizations or industries involved in visual programs which might be applicable to the photo-interpretation problem, display screen format, visual acuity, stereoscopy, flicker, and monitor visited the As a first attempt to indicate organizations and individuals involved in vision research, a membership list of the Armed Forces National Research Coucil Committee on Vision is included as Appendix A. The specialties or fields of interest of many of the individuals, along with their professional addresses, are included. Because of the very nature of this committee and its association with the Armed Forces, it appears that this list could provide a useful starting point. It is by no means complete, and this will be evident in an extensive listing of bibliographies which will appear in a subsequent report. The areas of investigation covered below in this report include visual acuity (and factors which influence acuity), viewing screen "dither," error keys and the effects of prior knowledge, and display formats. The first section is a brief introduction relating the human eye and visual system to a servomechanism. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t I 3.1 The Visual System as a Servomechanism The analogy between the human visual system and a photographic or tele- vision camera has often been made. There are, of course, obvious similar- ities but the analogy soon breaksdown, More recently, D. H. Fender (Ref. 1) likens the eye and visual system to a servomechanism which appears to be a much more accurate description. The eye as a servomechanism acts as a device that controls variable physical quantities by comparing actual values with a desired reference value, using differences to adjust the variable. Continuing the analogy, Fender notes that the cone cells are most closely packed in the fovea - the region of sharpest vision. For close examination, the eyes move so that the image falls on the corresponding areas of the two foveas. Each of the three pair of rotating muscles receives signals proportional to the displacement of the image from the fovea. Another control system brings the eyes to the correct angle of convergence, while still another adjusts the focus by changing the shape (and therefore the focal length) of the lens. This adjustment in focus - accommodation - is not "calculated" from the angle of convergence but instead is achieved by a steady "hunting" mechanism - like focusing a projector lens by hand until accommodation has been steered to the sharpest focus. Convergence and accommodation mechanisms are separate but cross-linked. Information, derived by one is fed to the other, for example, information in sharpest focus is fed across to the convergence mechanism. Another feedback mechanism changes the diameter of the pupil and is linked to the accommodative system because an increase in focal length requires an en- larged pupil to keep the image brightness constant. 3.2 Visual Acuity Visual acuity is, strictly speaking, the reciprocal of the visual angle, a, subtended by the critical detail of a test object, where a is expressed in minutes of visual angle. As this spatial resolving capacity increases, that is the ability to discriminate fine detail increases, acuity, 1/a, increases and the visual angle, a, decreases. The terms acuity and resolution are generally used interchangeably and involve the discrimination of two objects versus one. Detection, on the other hand, involves discerning between one object versus none. A fourth term, recog- nition, is more closely related to resolution, and involves a more specific categorization than just detection. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Boynton and Bush (Ref. 2) state that detection occurs when an observer identifies an object of interest but can't categorize it further. Recog- nition occurs when an observer identifies an object as belonging to a par- ticular class of objects or as having particular attributes. This can be broken down into increasingly specific categories. Recognition implies prior experience since an observer cannot categorize a completely novel object. Boynton and Bush indicated that in their experiments they were unable to obtain evidence of detection without recognition for the types of targets used. However, in examining, say, an aerial photograph, resolution of two or more objects can occur without recognition of a specific category. The limiting angular resolution of a typical human eye is generally taken to be about 1 minute of arc. This amounts to a linear retinal image of about 5 microns which corresponds to roughly 10 lines/mm at a reasonably comfortable viewing distance of about 13 inches. This resolution value is by no means constant and is in fact a function of a number of variables. The eye is capable of very high vernier acuity, that is, resolution of the offset between two straight edges placed end-to-end. For this type of resolution test, the visual angle can be as small as about 7 seconds of arc before the offset can no longer be seen. This angle is about one-third the angular subtense of a single cone receptor. Wires having a subtense only one-fortieth of a single cone can be detected under ideal circumstances. 3. 2. 1. 1 Luminance As the luminance level of a target increases, visual acuity also in- creases. Starting with the absolute threshold, visual acuity increases and begins to level off when, at the scotopic-photopic (rod-cone) break, acuity increases again and more or less levels off at about normal room luminances. It has been noted that contrast discrimination also improves in much the same fashion as acuity with increasing luminance, and that the two may be related. While acuity is measured in terms of a spatial thres- hold, and contrast discrimination in terms of a sensitivity threshold, it appears that acuity is a special form of luminance discrimination (Ref. 3). At a given luminance level, the higher the contrast the higher the acuity. Conversely, as the contrast approaches zero, the separation between two objects must be increased to be resolved. The minimum contrast Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 I r t for detection between a target and its background is often taken to be about 2 percent, but again this is only a very rough rule of thumb. The luminance differences of a ground scene, photographed from high altitudes, are generally quite small because of the contrast reducing effects of the atmosphere. These effects can be minimized through the proper selection of filters to attenuate the bluish veiling glare of atmos- pherically scattered light, and by selection of film which can be processed to a relatively high gamma (contrast). If the spectral characteristics of a target complex and its background are sufficiently well known, or can be guessed at with reasonable accuracy, film-filter combinations can sometimes be chosen to provide the photo in- terpreter a photograph with maximum target contrast. Unfortunately, more often than not, target signatures are usually only very roughly known and, except for some experimental situations, the majority of reconnaissance missions using panchromatic film rely almost exclusively on minus-blue filters, and these for their reduction of the effects of haze. The function of edge gradients can be included in a section on contrast. A number of people (probably starting with Mach in the 19th century) have observed that perceived contrast is'formed over the boundary of an object,' (Refs. 4, 5, 6) that is, the spatial-luminance transition connecting adja- cent areas. If the gradient at the boundary of two different luminance areas is shallow enough, these differences may not be detectable even if the contrast is well above threshold. This phenomenon has led to the development of many optical and electro-optical systems in an effort to enhance these edge gradients in a photograph. The human eye apparently performs an edge-enhancement function. The retinal image of a sharply defined bipartite object field is a more or less Gaussian distribution of energy. This image spreading is caused by a number of things, including diffraction by the pupil, spherical and chromatic aberration, scattered light and eye movements. Still, the edge-gradient can be perceived as being very sharp. Mach first suggested that this per- ceptual effect could be described by a second derivative correction applied to the retinal image, and it is this type of function which is usually per- formed in optical and electro-optical image-enhancement equipment. If images are blurred and have significantly reduced edge-gradients, search time increases,the duration of the visual fixations increases, and the distance between fixations decreases (Ref. 5). This is an area where, under some circumstances, contrast and image recognition can be improved Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 dramatically. Yet, while there are numerous contrast-enhancement devices, most of them being experimental, it has been pointed out (Ref. 7) that optical enhancement should probably be left to the interpreter's discretion. 3.2.1.3 Adaptation Acuity is highest where the fovea is adapted to the level of luminance of the target. In general, acuity is optimized when the surrounding room luminance is the same as that of the target (Refs. 8, 9). There is some degradation when the surround is darker, and even more degradation when the surround is brighter. As a practical matter for viewing images on a front or rear-projection screen, room illumination should be somewhat subdued to prevent stray light from reflecting from the screen, thereby reducing con- trast. 3.2.1.4 Wavelength The visual system, when daylight adapted, has maximum sensitivity to green light at a wavelength of about 555 m4a. The dark adapted eye is most sensitive to blue-green light near 510 m?. This change in sensitivity between the photopic and scotopic modes is known as the Purkinje shift. The optical system of the eye suffers from chromatic aberration, yet black-and-white objects do not in general appear to be fringed with color. This may be, partially explained by the sensitivity of the eye which tends to ignore the fringes which actually do exist in the retinal image. There is not a great deal of difference in visual acuity over a broad band of wavelengths, providing the luminance at each wavelength is optimized. Generally, in viewing images in filtered light, there is so little available blue light energy, and the sensitivity to blue is so low, that visual acuity is degraded. Where enough blue light, or red light, for that matter, is available, acuity is about the same as it is for yellow-green light. As mentioned in the section on contrast, the retinal image is affected by diffraction, spherical and chromatic aberration, astigmatism, stray light, hyperopia and myopia (far-sightedness and near-sightedness). If the pupil is very small, that is, about 2 mm, diffraction effects in the normal eye are the limiting factors for acuity. When the pupil is wide open at 7 or 8 mm, the aberration effects contribute most to image degradation. Optimum acuity occurs when the pupil is about 4 mm in diameter, and gets worse on each side of this value. The pupil seems to be set, at a given luminance value, for just the right aperture to give maximum acuity. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 I There have been numerous attempts to correlate the apparent sharpening of a diffuse image falling on a relatively coarse mosaic of cones with the observation that the eyes are constantly in motion. There are low ampli- tude motions which range from about 30 cps to 7Q cps. On top of this are slow motions of irregular frequency and extent, coupled with slow drifts and so-called saccadic jerks at irregular intervals. It can be shown, as in the section below on "screen dither,"that an image on a coarse mosaic can indeed be sharpened if the mosaic is moved about. However, in the case of eye movements. there appears to be increasing evidence that visual acuity is as good as it is in spite of eye movements, not because of them. The eye movements tend to keep the image from fading, for the neural system throughout the body is most sensitive to changes or differences. it is. by now, fairly well known that an image, which has been pro- jected onto a coarse surface or viewing screen such as ground glass, can be sharpened by moving the screen about in the plane of the image. The coarser the screen, the more dram,4ic the improvement. The image is inte- grated in time and undergoes a sort of statistical smoothing function which sharpens the image. An example of the effects of screen motion is shown in Figure 1. The top picture is simply an aerial image as seen through a.microscope and photographed by a camera attached to the eyepiece. The second photograph was obtained by projecting the resolution target onto a metal capstan roller and photographing the light scattered from the roller. This photo shows the grainy structure of the roller, and the reduced resolution. The bottom photo is similar except that in this case the roller is spinning during photography. The difference is quite evident. There are screens, such as the Polacoat LS60G material, available which are capable of resolving upwards of 60 lines/mm. With this type of screen, there is little improvement in image quality of high-contrast targets when the screen is dithered, but there is significant low contrast improve- Since areas of interest in an aerial photo are so often low contrast, ment. screen dither may be of considerable value in improving the perception of this kind of detail. McLachlan and Adams, in a letter to the editor of the Journal of the Optical Society of America, have also illustrated the effects of moving Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t I 1 I screens. A copy of this publication is included in this report. Also in- cluded is a copy of a patent issued in 1965 to Q. Parenti describing a mechanism to move a viewing screen for increased resolution, and an abstract of a paper by Carpenter on "Granularity of Rear-Projection Screens." 3.4 Error Keys and the Effect of Prior Information on Photo Interpretation Varous researchers (Refs. 10, 11, 12) have pointed out the importance of a photo-interpreter's prior knowledge in determining the probability of detecting or recognizing a particular target. This probability depends, in part, on his expectancies, that is, if he has been told to expect a certain target. If an interpreter has been furnished with additional intelligence he is much more likely to find a particular target. He is also more likely to "invent" targets, that is, report targets which are not actually there at all. It has also been shown that there are significant effects due to the interference of erroneous information. An interpreter who has been given false information is often seriously hampered in his search and de- tection capability. Martinek and Sadacca (Ref. 13)designed "error keys" and "rights keys" to aid in image identification. The error key was designed to help inter- preters avoid common misidentifications. This key resulted in a substan- tial decrease in the numer of errors, with an attendent increase in accuracy, but no difference in the number of correct identifications. The rights key was produced by presenting photographs of the same quality and scale, taken over the same type of terrain, as the photos to be interpreted. This key, it was reported, had no significant effect on any aspect of performance measured. 3.5 Display Format and Search Procedure Work performed by Reilly and Teichner (Ref. 14) indicated that a square field of view is generally superior to round ones for target detection. This indeed is fortunate because aerial photography formats are almost exclusively square or rectangular. Screens in viewers are often made about 20" by 30" or 30" by 30", which provides a single interpreter a fairly com- fortable viewing field. If a screen is made too small, concentration of area in the center increases, durations of fixation increase, interfixation distances decrease, and overall search efficiency decreases (Ref. 15). Even with so-called "optimum" screen sizes, however, an interpreter tends to make a quick scan throughout the field and then spend an increasingly greater amount of time concentrating on the center. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 t 1 1 1 While this concentration on the central part of the screen may be partly due to a natural tendency to look more or less straight ahead at a viewing screen, the fact that the image quality is usually better at the center of the format may also play a role. The image quality at the edge of an aerial photograph is almost invariably worse than at the center because of aberrations in the camera lens. These same afflictions affect the viewer projection lens with a further loss in image quality. Finally, screen illumination in the corners is often somewhat lower when viewed from the center. With these factors affecting the image quality, particularly toward the edges, it is not particularly surprising that an observer's attention more or less naturally drifts to a region where he can"see" better. Fry and Townsend (Ref. 16) found that machine-generated search patterns, using a ring or outline square, which-give a complete and uniform coverage are useful primarily when the targets are difficult to find. They also reported that, under good visibility, free search is much preferred. Apparently, under these conditions, peripheral vision plays a significant role and that, on the average, free search represents a faster way of finding a target. The artifical search patterns may serve as a useful training aid. however. Future areas of investigation will include stereoscopy, flicker, color vision, and fatigue. During the next reporting period, the principal investigator will attend the SPIE conference in New York on The Human in the Photo-Optical System." Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 t 1 1 1 1 1. Fender, Derek H.: Control Mechanisms of the Eye. Scientific American, vol. 211, no. 1, July 1964, pp 24-33. 2. Boynton, R. M., and Bush, W. R.: Laboratory Studies Pertaining to Visual Air Reconnaissance. WADC Tech. Rpt. 55-304, Sept. 1955. Boynton, R. M.: Spatial Vision. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 13, 1962. 4. Lamar, E. S., Hecht, S., Schlaer, S., and Hendley, C. D.: Size, Shape, and Contrast in Detection of Targets by Daytime Vision. JOSA, vol. 37, 1947, pp 531-545. 5. Brainard, R. W., and Ornstein, G. N.: Image Quality Enhancement. North American Aviation, Inc., AMRL-TR-65-28, Behavioral Sciences Lab., Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio, April 1965. 6. Perrin, F. H.: Methods of Appraising Photographic Systems, Part I. J. SMPTE, 69, 1960, pp 151-156. 7. Blackwell, R. H., Ohmart, J. G., and Brainard, R. W.: Experimental Evaluation of Optical Enhancement of Literal Visual Display. ASD Tech. Rpt. 61-568, Oct. 1961. 8. Westheimer, G.: Visual Acuity. Annual Review of Psychology, vol. 16, 1964. 9. Lythogoe, R. J.: The Measurement of Visual Acuity. Spec. Rpt. Ser. No. 173, Med. Res. Council, London, 1932. 10. Sadacca, R.: New Techniques in Image Interpretation Systems. Pre- sented at the Seventh Annual Army Human Factors Engineering Con- ference, 1960. 11. Sadacca, R., Castelmovo, A., Ranes, J.: The Impact of Intelligence Information Furnished Interpreters. HFRB Tech. Res. Note No. 117, June 1961. 12. Klingberg, C. L., Elworth, C. L.. and Kraft, C. L.: Identification of Oblique Forms. RADC-TDR-64-144, The Boeing Co., Seattle, Washington. RADC Display Techniques Branch, Aug. 1964. 13. Martinek, H. and Sadacca, R.: Error Keys as Reference Aids in Image Interpretation. Tech. Res. Note 153, USAPRO, June 1965. 14. Reilly, R. E. and Teichner, W. H.: Effects of Shape and Degree of Structure of the Visual Field on Target Detection and Location. JOSA, vol. 52, 1962, pp 214-218. 15. Enoch, J. N.: Effect of the Size of a Complex Visual Display Upon Visual Search. JOSA, vol. 48, 1958, p 836 (Abstract). 16. Fry, G. A. and Townsend, C. A.: The Effects of Controlling the Search Pattern of a Photo Interpreter. RADC 'tech, Rpt., RADC TN-59-533, Sept. 1959. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 STAT 1 t t I 2. Sykos, M.: Safety Considerations of Lasers, Univ. of Calif., Lawrence Radiation Lab., Livermore, Calif., Jan. 14, 1963. 3. Straub, H. W.: Protection of the Human Eye from Laser Radiation, Harry Diamond Lab., USAMC, Wash., D.C., July 10, 1963. STAT Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 - (I0-RnP7RRna77nannnannndnn31-6 N 0"111-im 11 0 10 9 1 FIG. I - ILLUSTRATION OF GRAIN REDUCTION EFFECTS BY MOVING VIEWING SCREEN Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 f i Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 lengths associated with the three color components. Then v a,(x,y) Ur(l) represents the complex envelope at the space-time point (x,y,l) due to that portion of the beam which has traversed the object, while E exp[-27ri(l+x)6v,/c]U,(t) ,-t represents the corresponding complex envelope due to the refer- ence beam. The resultant complex wave amplitude V (x,y,t) at (x,y,t) in the recording plane is therefore a V (x,y,l)- E {a,(x,y)+exp[-27ri(l+x)Bv,/c]I XU,(t) exp(-2rriv,l), (1) where v1, v2, va are the midfrequencies corresponding to the three primary colors. Let us now make the usual assumptions that the optical field is stationary, so that all the ensemble averages are time independent, and that there is no second-order coherence between the three color components, so that the average (Ur*(i)L~,?(t))=J b,,,?. (2) Then the mean light intensity I (x,y) recorded on the photographic plate at the point (x,y) is I (x,y) _ (Vr (x,y,l) V (x,y,t)) = E J,I Ia,(x,y)12+1+a: (x,y) exp[-2ai(l+x)Bv,/c] +a,(x,y) exp[21ri(1+x)0v,/c3). (3) After development of the photographic plate this record constitutes the hologram. Let us suppose that the amplitude transmission of the plate at the point (x,y) after development and reversal is proportional to I(x,y). If the plate is illuminated normally by a 3-color, polar- ized, plane light beam of the same kind as before, of complex wave amplitude a V'(t) U.'(t) exp(-2lrivd), (4) .-1 then the waves emerging at the point (x,y) at time t will have a complex amplitude V"(x,y,t) of the form V"(x,y,t)= E 7, K.J,[1a,(x,y)12+1]U.'(1) exp(-21riv.I) 3 a + K.J,a, (x,y)U,'(t)exp(-2tri[v,(l+x)0/c+v.l]) The complex constants K1, K2, K3 represent transmission coef- ficients of the hologram for the three primary colors, and probably do not differ too greatly in absolute value. As is well-known from the analysis of Leith and Upatnieks,2,3 the terms of the first double summation in Eq. (5) represent plane waves travelling longitudinally, while those of the second double summation represent various reconstructions with phase reversal of the original wavefront, but at an angle to the longitudinal. The terms of the third double summation in Eq. (5) represent the genuine reconstruction of the original wavefront at other angles to the longitudinal, and give rise to the virtual image. In the pres- ent case 9 such terms are to be considered. The terms obtained by putting rms correspond to waves of the three primary colors travelling at an angle a to the longitudinal, which are correctly recording plane Flo. 1. Optical arrangement for recording holograms. modulated by the appropriate amplitude transmission function of the original object. Provided IK11, 1K21, and IK3I do not differ too much, these three waves allow the virtual image of the object to be seen in true color from a direction 0. The terms obtained by putting rv-1s in the last double summa- tion of Eq. (5) represent light waves of some color modulated by the amplitude transmission function of the object corresponding to a different color. If these waves were superposed on the previ- ously mentioned ones they would clearly distort the color reproduc- tion process. Fortunately, however, as can he seen from Eq. (5), these waves do not propagate at an angle 0 to the longitudinal, but at angles (vr/p.)B (with r;6s). Thus, if 0 is 30? and if v1, v2, va are 4.6, 5.5, 6.7X 1014 cps, respectively, the angles of propagation are approximately 21?, 25?, 36?, 44?. Hence, provided the view of the virtual image is restricted so as to exclude these directions, there will be no distortion of the color reproduction. In practice the aperture of observation is restricted in any case, and the foregoing restriction is not likely to be serious. It seems then that the hologram technique of Leith and Upat- nieks should be capable of reproducing images in color, substan- tially without modification. I am indebted to R. L. Lamberts of the Eastman Kodak Com- pany for a number of discussions of the problems of imaging by the method of wavefront reconstruction. *Work supported in part by the I1. S. Air Force Office of Scientific Re' search. D. Gabor, Proc. Roy. Soc. (London) 197A, 454 (1949). t E. Leith and J. Upatnieks, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 52, 1123 (1962). 3 L. Leith and J. Upatnieks, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53, 1377 (1963). 4 H. Leith and J. Upatnieks, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 54, 1295 (1964). G. W. Stroke and D. G. Falconer, Phys. Letters 13, 306 (1964). Reduced Graininess of Moving Screens DAN MCLACELAN, JR.,* AND HERBERT D. ADAMS University of Denver, Denver, Colorado (Revision received 22 July 1965) IT is well-known that graininess on a screen is equivalent to a background noise which deters the observer from extracting a clear impression of an image that is intended to be projected upon the screen. For example, a beaded screen such as is used for the home projection of color slides gives sharper images when the beads are as small as is practical; and a ground glass on a reflex camera is more effective when the surface is prepared in an expert way. In a manner not to be discussed here, the clarity of an image on a grainy screen is a function of the number, site, and distribu- tion of the grains or scattering points over the surface of ascreen. On the assumption that the clarity of images on a screen is de- termined largely by the effective density of scattering points, some experiments were performed to show that the effective number of points can be increased by changing the positions of the points Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 CPYRGH T i IYRGHT Ftc. 1. Arrangement of anpnratus. Turin a lime exposure. This was Clone by moving the screen on t hich a sl al ionar' \ inr.tge was cast while it was bci ug lthol oc raphed. .\ 1 S. National Bureau of Standards lest filet teas placed at Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Then two motors were attached to the ground glass at GS; one moved the screen ' in. horizontally one revolution per minute and the other moved the glass - in. vertically 12 rpm. The total length of path traversed by each point on the screen was about four inches. The resulting picture is shown in Fig. 3. This experiment suggests that the effectiveness of a viewing screen can be improved by motion. This might he particularly useful for fluorescent screens as used for viewing in the electron microscope or for direct viewing in medical x-ray fluoroscopy. Of course, for direct viewing the motion would have to be produced by high-frequency vibration to smooth out the effect for the human eye. * Present address: The Ohio State University, Department of Miner- alogy, 116 West 19th Avenue, Columbus, Ohio 432111. Erratum t 55, 203 (1965) TRABKA, E. A. "Wiener Spectrum of Scans Obtained from an Isotropic Two-Dimensional Random Field." The lower limit of integration in Eq. (3) should be w instead of 0. Ref. 3 should be J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 16, 151 (1945). Book Reviews I nlargeon nt of image on gtound girt of I3urean of Standard, te-t film. I I' in I ig. I ;uul projected at a reduction of 1%15.4 on a ground Mass scrim at (;S. To sec host badly the ground glass resolved :lit, pattern, it t\as magnilicd 9.95X onto photographic 111111 at 1 II. 'I'll(- result:utt picture is shown in 1'ig. 2. t 1r1G. 3. Enlargement of iutage on moving ground glass of Bureau of Standards test film. Optical Transforms C. A. TAYLOR AND 1-I. 1.3PSON. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, New York, 1964. Pp. 182. Price $7.50. The application of. optical transforms to x-ray diffraction problems is presented, by use of the close analogy between the diffraction of x rays and the diffraction of light. This book de- scribes a new research tool for x-ray crystallographers, while illus- trating Fourier-transform ideas in general through the visual medium of optical transforms. Two-dimensional models of crystal structures can be made from holes punched in opaque cards (masks). The diffraction pattern of an arrangement of holes representing a single molecule is called the optical transform. Optical equipment used in diffraction experiments is described, together with the physical and photographic methods of mask preparation. The authors discuss the transformation in both di- rections between real and reciprocal space. The physical principles of symmetry are illustrated in two dimensions by means of optical diffraction. Fourier synthesis is described for reconstruction of the images from the scattered light waves. The book is profusely illustrated with figures and photographs which show in detail the use of optical transforms. Fifty-four plates are collected at the center of the book; the complexity and beauty of these photographs clearly demonstrates the es- sentially physical basis of x-ray crystallography. These photo- graphs should also prove useful to persons engaged in teaching. J. L. DONOVAN Research Laboratories Eastman Kodak Company Rochester, New York 14650 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 r t Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 June 1, 1965 C. PARENTI 3,186,299 DEVICE TO INCREASE THE RESOLVING POWLI1 IN PROJECTIONS ON A TRANSLUCENT SCREEN, PARTICULARLY FIT FOR PiiOTOGRAMMETRIC APPLIANCES Oiled March 21, 1962 Fig.2 INVENTOR. C-tno Pareri t BY Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 BEST COPY Available Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 t United States Patent Office ': H36,299 DE'-VICE TO INC>t> . '_if THE REcIlLVING 1'3WER iN PRO sEC it-IONS ON Ah TRANSLUCENT SCREEN, I'ARTICU1_.'ARLY El i< FOR P110TOGRAMMETRIC r;k'I'LI:'s.i :IS Ciao Parc" i, tdome, holy, assignor to Ottico Meccanica _ zfi.tua Sa,c. p. Az., Rcme, Italy 1;:..! i01ar. 21, k962, Ser. do. 181,247 Claims prioki y, applic.:uun Italy, Ir. '22, 1961, 5,018/61 1 Claim. (Cl. 88-28.93) illere are known in photokl'anllitetl:e and auroplroto- granurictrle itistr'untentai[aus devices fur dlasecioc pro- jection of images on plates or films on a translucent screen, for the purpose of enlarging, them properly and, observing them through transparency, to perform on Them ip iaggs of measurements which in this way may reach a high =,-de precision. It is t icrefore necessary to obtain a considerable defi- nition of the projected hoot,., joined to an almost uni- foiin distribution of the luminous intensity of the screen. it is knovin that the definition of the projected images, is tile greater, the mote minute the roughness is of the polish constituting the tr:dnslucent surface on which the image (screcu) is formed; on the ether hand, however, the possibility of having the optimum of the above-men- ticncd uniformity of luminous intensify is the gic?: ter, the greater within a certain limit, is the roughness of this surface (diffusing capacity of the screen). The purpose of this invention is to ensure a good defi- nition of the projected images, also to maintain a con- siderable uniformity of the distribution of the intensity of the illumination of the screen. The description of the invention may more easily be followed in reference to the added illustrating design which represents, by way of a not limited example, it preferred performance. In the illustration: FIG. I represents a screen mounted on a frame; FIG. 2 is it section of the same according to the hatched plan II-11 of FIG. 1. Referring to the figures a screen is represented with such roughness as to ensure a uniform distribution of the luminosity of the image. Said screen is mounted on frame 2 on the lower end of which are made two holes 3, while at the upper end two pivots 4 are fitted free to turn in th,-ir proper seats. On a supporting plate S the motor 6 is fitted of which the turnh g shaft is made conjoint, by means of a joint 7, with shaf . 8 turning in the bearings 9. in the positions shown in the drawing two endless screws 10 are keyed on shaft 8 in play with as many helicuidal wheels 11. The pivots 4 fitted on frame 2 are clutched in seats 12 eccentrically arranged in the helicoi- dal wheels 11. Two pivots 13 are fixed in the holes 3 and made con- 3,186,299 Patented June 1, 1965 2 joint wish plate 5. The arrangement of pivots 13 and the size of the holes 3, taking into account the eccen- tricity of pivots 4 with respect to the helicoidal wheels 11, are such as to allow the frame 2 and therefore the 5 formation plan of the projected image, a uniform circu- lar movement of video the trajectory, by virtue of the flanges of pivots 13, cvt;st:ultly will be on the hirer locat- ed by the above-mentioned collection plan of the pro- jected image. to A cover 14, whilst it protects the mechanical parts in movenicl)t, prevents the observer from seeing this, so that the projected image on screen 1 appears to him clear and perfectly defined. In fact, owning to the movement of screen 1, the effect of the roughness of the translucent 15 screen, will be of less influence to the clearness of the image acid therefore the image will turn out to admit the relief also in the smallest details. Moreover, the con- siderable grade of roughness which .it was possible to adopt for iite translucent surface of the screen, will per- 20 Writ to observe the projected insane, endowed by lumi- nous intensity almost uniform from the centre to the riuu'gins of the drawing. The variations of a constructive character which might he applied to the described device will fall into the field 25 of protection of the invention every time that the same inventiv, conception here exposed would be carried out to reach equal or similar results. What 1 claim is: - An o,tical device, comprising a single translucent 80 screen having a rough surface and adapted to receive a projecteu image, a frame carrying said screen and hav- ing two upper corner portions and two lower corner portions, two symmetrically disposed pivots mounted in said upper corner portions, a supporting plate, a motor 35 carriedcy said plate, an elongated shaft driven by said motor, i,vo endless screws keyed upon said shaft, two helicoidal wheels meshing with said screws, each of said pivots being eccentrically mounted in a separate helicoi- dal wheel and being; rotatable therewith, said two lower 40 corner portions having circular symmetrically disposed uniform holes, and two pivots mounted in said plate, the two last-mentioned pivots engaging the side walls of said holes and having diameters which are smaller than those. of said holes. 45 References Cited by the Examiner UNITED STATES PATENTS 50 1,969,909 8/34 Sinijian -------------- 88-28.9 2,525,596 10/50 Finn ---------------- 88-28:93 2,780,136 2/57 Erban --------------- 88-28.93 JULIA E. COINER, Primary Examiner. NORTON ANSHER, Examiner. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 CPYRGH T JOURNAL OF THE OPTICAL SOCIETY OF AMERICA Vol. 53, No. 4, April 1963, p 522 WA20. Granularity of Rear Projection Screens. Vance J. Carpenter, Bausch & Lomb, Inc., 635 St. Paul Street, Rochester 2, New York. In the conventional use of a rear-projection screen, as exemplified by a contour projector, the observer sees a granular structure which is colored and which moves with the observer's eye. This effect has been found to be dependent upon the numerical aperture of the projection system, and it disappears when the N.A. is large. Results of measurements showing this relationship will be given. A qualitative theory of the cause of this phenomena will be proposed, and a means of eliminating it will be suggested. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30: CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 ARMED FORCES NRC COMMITTEE ON VISION Mempership List Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Membership List January 1, 1966 XECUTIVE COUNCIL Chairman: John L. Brown, PhD Visual acuity, spectral sensitivity, Graduate School electro-physiology, effects of spect- Kansas State University rally select adaptation Manhattan, Kansas '.t. Col. James F. Culver School of Aerospace Medicine Brooks AFB, Texas 78235 Richard Feinberg, PhD Aging studies utilizing fundus photog- AM-12,.Clinical Research Branch raphy, critical flicker fusion, Federal Aviation Agency pupillography, brightness contrast, Washington, D. C. refraction, biomicroscopy, tonometry, etc. Glen Hawkes, PhD Chief, Basic Sciences Research Branch U. S..Army Medical R & D Command Office of Surgeon General Washington, D. C. 20315 Visual acuity and protection in space Elwin Marg, PhD Eye motility, electro-physiology of the School of Optometry visual system, color vision, tonometry, University of California accommodation Berkeley, California Walton L. Jones, MD, Code RBH NASA Headquarters Washington, D. C. T. G. Martens, MD Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Extra-ocular muscle imbalance James W. Miller, PhD Office of Naval Research Code 454 Washington, D. C. (Contract Monitor) John H. Taylor, PhD Psychophysical methods, target detection Visibility Laboratory and recognition, form discrimination, Scripps Institution of oceanography spatial summation, visual search University of California peripheral thresholds, small subtense 4aJolla, California color vision, vision underwater Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 EX- OFFICIO Executive Secretary, Division of Behavioral Sciences: Peter Hammond, PhD National Academy of Sciences - National Research Council 2101 Constitution Avenue Washington, D. C. Staff Advisor: Conrad Mueller, PhD Department of Psychology University of Indiana Bloomington, Indiana Executive Secretary: Milton A. Whitcomb, PhD National Research Council 2101 Constitution Avenue Washington, D. C. NRC MEMBERS Mathew Alpern, PhD Psycho-physiology of contrast in human 3536 Kresge Medical Research Bldg. eye, eiectro-physiology of retina, University of Michigan pupil accommodation convergence Ann Arbor, Michigan Howard D. Baker, PhD Department of Psychology Florida State University Tallahssee, Florida Visual adaptation Horac-.. !iarlow, PhD Schoci o1c Optometry University of California Berkeley, California William Bevan, PhD Visual form perception, scaling of visual Vice President for Academic Affairs -dimensions, color vision Johns Hopkins University Baltimore, Maryland H. Richard Blackwell, PhD Physiological optics, visual psychophysics, institute for Research in Vision retinal disease, illumination, visibility 1314 Kinnear Road Columbus, Ohio Paul Boeder, MD Department of Ophthalmology University Hospitals University of Iowa Iowa City, Iowa Ocular mechanics Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 t 1 r Goodwin Breinin, MD New York University School of Medicine 550 First Avenue New York, New York Charles J. Campbell, MD Eye Institute 635 West 165th Street New York, New York Carter Collins Institute of Visual Sciences Presbyterian Medical Center 2340 Clay Street San Francisco, California 94115 Tom N. Cornsweet, PhD Department of Psychology University of California Berkeley, California Thomas G. Dickinson, MD 301 Doctor's Garden Building 1880, Arlington Street Sarasota, Florida Ocular motility, electrophysiology, physiology, ophthalmology Visual biophysics Photochemistry of color vision, retinal spatial interaction Visual problems, high altitude flight, visual standards for flight personnel John Dowling, PhD Woods Research Building Johns Hopkins Hospital Baltimore, Maryland 21205 Seibert Q. Duntley, ScD Visibility Laboratory Scripps Institution of Oceanography University of California San Diego, California Jay M. Enoch, PhD Cepartment of Ophthalmology School of Medicine Washin?r'on University 660 S. Euclid St. Louis, Missouri All phases of visual search detection Visual search, retinal optics and response characteristics Thecdore W. Forbes, PhD Deoartment of Psychology Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan Glenn A. Fry, PhD School of Optometry Ohio State University Columbus, Ohio Head and eye movements in driving, highway sign legibility and effectiveness, per- ceptual problems in auto driving safety Color vision (chromatic adaptation and color blindness) visual performance studies, accommodation-convergence relations Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 J. W. Gebhard, PhD Applied Physics Laboratory Johns Hopkins University 8621 Georgia Avenue Silver Spring, Maryland William Greenspon, OD 815 Heatherwood Road Bluefield, West Virginia Sylvester K. Guth, EE Radiant Energy Effects Laboratory Lamp Division General Electric Company Nela Park Cleveland, Ohio Rate discrimination for intermittent photic stimulation Visibility, visual performance, color, physiological effects of light and lighting Rita M. Halsey, PhD Department 591 Engineering Labs., IBM Neighborhood, Road Kingston, New York E. Rae Harcum, PhD Department of Psychology College of William and Mary Williamsburg, Virginia James Harris Visibility Laboratory Scripps Institution of Oceanography University of California LaJolla, California Gordon G. Heath, PhD Division of Optometry indiana University Bloomington, Indiana Eric G. Heinemann, PhD Department of Psychology Brooklyn College Brooklyn, New York Visual d!splays, color Pattern and form detection and recognition Detection theory and analytic formulations applied to visual processes, problems of visual search, and pre-display processing to improve observer performance Color blindness, night vision, accommoda- tion--convergence relationships and neural control, electrophysiology of visual pathways Spatial interaction, adaptation, space perception I Richard M. He;d, PhD Department of Psychology Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139 Henry Hofstetter, PhD Division of Optometry 'ndiana University Bloomington, Indiana Occupational vision, accommodation and convergence relationships Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t I E. Porter Horne, PhD Box 12353 University Station Gainesville, Florida Flicker-fusion--frequency and apparent motion as related to perceptual processes static and dynamic r t t Dorothea Jameson Hurvich, PhD Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Robert Jampel, MD College of Physicians > Surgeons Columbia University 630 West 168th Street New York, New York Arthur Jampolsky, MD Cirector, Eye Research Institute Presbyterian Medical Center San Francisco, California Donald H. Kelly, PhD ITEK Corporation, Vidya Division 1450 Page Mill Road Palo Alto, California Robert Kling, MD Room 116 Georgetown University Hospital 3800 Reservoir Road Washington, D. C. John Krauskopf, PhD 7218 Beacon Terrace Bethesda, Maryland Herschel Leibowitz, PhD Department of Psychology Pennsylvania State University University Park, Pennsylvania Color vision, psycho-physics, psycho- physiology Neuro-ophthalmology, physiology of eye movements Electromyography of ocular muscles, ocular motor anomalies Perception, psychophysiology, displays John Levinson, PhD Bell Telephone Laboratories, Inc. Room 2D-514 Murray Hill, New Jersey Arthur Linksz, MD 6 East 76th Street New York, New York Leo E. Lipetz, PhD Institute for Research in Vision 1314 Kinnear Road Columbus, Ohio "Retinal response to light, temporal and spatial Color vision, space perception, sensory disturbances in strabismus Transmission of information by visual system, mechanisms of light adaptation, correlation of structure and function, and retinal light scatter Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 I I Alfred Lit, PhD Department of Psychology Southern Illinois University Carbondale, Illinois Irene E. Loewenfeld, PhD Department of Ophthalmology College of Physicians & Surgeons Columbia University 635 West 165th Street New York, New York Depth discrimination, visual latent period, applications of visual psychophysiology and perception to human engineering problems The pupil (autonomic innervation), rela- tions between pupillary activity and vision George Long, PhD MOL Subdivision Douglas Aircraft Company Huntington Beach, California Norman H. Mackworth, PhD Harvard School of Public Health Center for Aerospace Health & Safety 665 Huntington Avenue Boston, Massachusetts Recording of eye movements during visual performance tasks Leonard Matin, PhD Department of Psychology Columbia University Schermerhorn Hall New York, New York Ailene Morris, PhD Eye Research Institute San Francisco Institute of Medical Sci. 2340 Clay Street San Francisco, California Visual search, psychophysics of visibility engineering, visibility calculation, form and pattern detection and recognition, visual standards vs. visual efficiency und- er various atmospheric conditions Jacob Nachmias, PhD Department of Psychology College Hall University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19104 Thomas Nelsor, PhD University of Alberta Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Gunter K. von Noorden, MD The Wilmer Institute The Johns Hopkins Hospital Ba:timore, Maryland Kenneth N. Ogle, PhD S3ction of Biophysics Mayo Clinic Rochester, Minnesota Visual acuity, eye movements, space perception theory of psychophysics Flicker, brightness, form and orientations, and traffic marking devices Ocular motility disturbances, amblyopia, mechanisms of eye movements, visual physiology Visibility of out of focus images. Steroscopic depth from delayed Images in the two eyes Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 t s 1 1 1 George N. Ornstein, PhD North American Aviation, Inc. 4300 East Fifth Avenue Columbus, Ohio Albert Potts, PhD Department of Ophthalmology University of Chicago Chicago, Illinois Harris Ripps, PhD Department of Ophthalmology New Yori: ,,iiversity Medical Center 550 First Avenue New York, New York Heinrich Rose, MD 1651 Tulane Drive Mountain View, California Thorne Shipley, PhD Binocular vision, critical flicker fre- Department of Ophthalmology quency, color vision, neurophysiology of University of Miami School of Medicine the visual system 1638, N. W. Tenth Avenue Miami, Florida Ezra V. Saul, PhD Institute for Applied Experimental Psychology Tufts University Medford, Massachusetts Robert Sleight, PhD Sign legibility, form perception, image Applied Psychology Corporation interpretation 4113 Lee Highway Arlington, Virginia Olin W. Smith, PhD Development problems of depth, distance, Department of Psychology size, motion perception and measures of Cornell University visual acuity Ithaca, New York Stanley W. Smith, PhD Institute for Research in Vision Ohio State University Research Center 1314 Kinnear Road Columbus, Ohio 43212 Harry G. Sperling, PhD Color vision theory, colorimetry, photos Minneapolis Honeywell Regulator Company metry--temporal aspects of vist!al res- Military Products Group ponse 2700 Ridgway Road Minneapolis, Minnesota Mathematical models of visual perceptual processes, image enhancement techniques, visual performance evaluation in military target identification systems Electroretinography, biochemistry and toxicology of the eye, electronic process- ing of ophthalmoscopic images Photo-labile pigments of the retina Visual orientation : space, depth per- ception, eye protec :on against radiation, vision in approach End landing, night vision Documentation of the vision literature and pictorial communication Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t t I Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Lawrence Stark, MD Neurological mechanisms underlying eyf Bioengineering Department movement control systems: version, ver- University of Illinois gence, lens and pupil. Bioengineering, Chicago, Illinois 60680 cybernetics Gerald Westheirner, PhD School of Optometry University of California Berkeley, California Oculomotor systerns--retinal image U. S. ARMY A I ter,,... i.a to LAi--, i ve Cc::,: Col. Sidney L. Marvin, MC Chief, Behavioral Sciences Branch U. S. Army Medical R & D Command Office of the Surgeon General Washington, D. C. 20315 Electrical recording Lt. Co1..Robert W. Bailey, MSC Dark adaptation, color vision, visual Commanding Officer problems associated with aviation U. S. Army Aeromedical Research Unit Fort Rucker, Alabama 36362 Philip J. Bersh, PhD Image interpretation, visual displays, Combat Systems Research Laboratory stereopsis Room 1406 U. S. Army Personnel Research Laboratory Washington, D. C. Lt. Col. Roswell G. Daniels, MC All aspects having military or industrial Occupational Health Branch application John C. Armington, PhD Dept.. of Sensory Psychology, NP Div. WRAIR, Walter Reed Army Medical Center Washington, D. C. P1eventive Medicine Division Office of the Surgeon General Department of the Army Washington, D. C. E. Ralph Dusek, PhD Visual search from low performance Scientific Adviser for aircraft--ground to air and air to Military Performance ground Headquarters U. S. Army Research Institute of Environmental Medicine Natick, Massachusetts David L. Easley, PhD U. S. Army Armor Human Research Unit Fort Knox, Kentucky Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 t t I 11 f III Lt. Col. Richard Froemming, MC Chief, Occupational Health Branch Professional Services Office of the Surgeon General Department of the Army Washington, D. C. 20315 Lt. Col. Billy C. Greene, MSC Visual standards, occupational vision Office of the Surgeon General programming, eye/vision protective Department of the Army devices (eye armor) Washington, D. C. 20315 George S. Har':ar , Psychology Division U. S. Army Medical Research Laboratory Fort Knox, Kentucky Stern sc oip c vision :space percept' Col. Kenneth E. Hudson, MC Hq. VII Corps APO 107, New York, New York Arthur Jones, PhD Experimental Psychology Division Army Medical Research Laboratory Fort Knox, Kentucky Contact lenses of military personnel Leon T. Katchmar, PhD Systems Research Lab. U. S. Army Ordnance Human Engineering Labs. Aberdeen Proving Ground, Maryland John Kobrick, PhD Headquarters U. S. Army Research Institute of Environmental Medicine Natick, Massachusetts Effects of environmental exposure vari- ables upon visual performance; human factors applications of vision principles directed toward equipment design for Army use, particularly QMC Lt. Col. Charles W. Kraul, MC Personnel and Training Directorate Army Materiel Command Department of the Army Washington, D. C. 20315 Lt. Col. Robert W. Neidlinger, MC Box 209 Letterman General Hospital Presidio of San Francisco, California Occupational vision programs Lt. Col. Wayne R. Otto, MC Vision as associated with aviation, Chief, Aviation Operations Branch physical standards for flyers, visual Directorate of Plans, requirements for flying Supply and Operations Office of the Surgeon General, D/A Main Navy Building Washington, D. C. 20315 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t t Col. Jack W. Passmore, MC Chief, Ophthalmology Service Walter Reed General Hospital Walter Reed Army Medical Center Washington, D. C. Lt. Col. Richard Phillips U. S. Army Environmental Hygiene Agency Edgewood Arsenal, Maryland Edward H. Polley, PhD Chief, Neurology Branch Dire.. e of Medi ca 1 Res {:. ch U. S. - my Chemical R & 0 Labs. Edgewood Arsenal, Maryland 2 1010 Michael H. Siegel, PhD Color discriminati,:n, psychophysical U. S. Army Chemical Center methodology Edgewood Arsenal Maryland Francis H. Thomas, PhD Training of visual search techniques or U. S. Army Aviation Human Research Unit unaided eye Fort Recker, Alabama j. E. Uhlaner, PhD Director, Research Laboratories Personnel Research Office Department of the Army Washington, D. C. Display panel--image interpretation .iohn D. Weisz, PhD Human Engineering Lab. - Bldg. 2427 Aberdeen Provinq Ground, Maryland Robert S. Wiseman, PhD Recognition and detection capabilities, Warfare Vision Branch improvement of night vision U. S. Army Engineer R # D Labs. Fort Belvoir, Virginia 22060 Joseph Leidner, PhD Relation of age function to vision U. Army Personnel Research office ,.standards Office of the Chief, R & D Department of the Army Washington, D. C. Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t t 1 t 1 t I U. S. AIR FORCE Carl P. Crocetti, PhD Human Engineering Laboratory, RCSH Rome Air Development Center Griffiss AFB, New York Capt. Herbert D. Fenske, USAF, MC JSAF Hospital Andrews Andrews AFB, Maryland 20331 Capt. Glenn P. Johnston, USAF, MC Wilford Nall USAF Hospital Lackland AFB, Texas 78236 Maj. Paul W. Lappin Eye protection against radiation, visual Radiation Shielding Branch (MRBBR) problems in space Biophysics Laboratory 6570th Aerospace Medical Research Labs. Wright-Patterson AFB, Ohio ,Maj. Donald G. Pitts, USAF, MSC Visibility and search, accommodative- Physical Physiological Optics Section convergence relationships, ocular trans- Ophthalmology Department mission and retinal burns, all aspects of USAF School of Aerospace Medicine vision in space flight Brooks AF6, Texas Brig. Gen. Benjamin A. Strickland, or. USAF, MC Director Profess i .,na l Services Office of the Surgeon General, USAF Washington, D. C. 20333 J. S. NAVY John A. Bartelt Aircraft exterior lighting, aircraft Code RAAE-531 searchlights Electrical Br. - Airborne Equipment Div. Bureau of Weapons Department of the Navy Washington, D. C. Capt. Sidney D. Bond, Jr., MC, USN Use of infra-red photography in evaluat- U. S. Naval School of Aviation Medicine ing strabismus U. S. Naval Aviation Medical Center Pensacola, Florida CGpt. Roland A. Bosee, MSC, USN Visual orientation, eye protection, night Bureau of Naval Weapons (RA-15) vision, air crew station lighting Department of the Navy Washington, D. C. 20360 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 t Gloria Chisum, PhD Vision Branch U. S. Naval Air Development Center. Johnsville, Pennsylvania 18974 Amos R. David Photopic and scotopic aspects in relation U. S. Navy Department to U. S. Navy ship design and operation Bureau of Ships - Code L09.3C Standardization Societies Liaison Sec. Washington, D. C. 20360 Victor Fields, PhD Deputy Head, Psychologica ;:.search ?r Personnel Research Divisic- Bureau of Naval Personnel Department of the Navy Washington, D. C. Personnel selection and nerfnrmance Pua)'!a- Timothy Kenney Safety Engineer Bureau of Ships Department of the Navy Washington, D. C. Cdr. Paul R. Kent, MSC, USN Vision Branch, Research Division Naval Submarine Medical Center U. S. Naval Submarine Base New London/Groton, Connecticut JoAnn S. Kinney, PhD Color vision, night vision, temporal Box 600 factors USN Submarine Medical Center USN Submarine Base Groton, Connecticut 06342 Clinton H. Maag, PhD Deputy Life Sciences Officer Life Sciences Department, Box 31 U. S. Naval Missile Center Point Mugu, California Earl Miller, PhD Interaction between the visual and vesti- Head, Physiological Optics Branch bular systems, space perception, evaluation Medical Sciences Division of visual tests, vision as influenced by U. S. Naval School of Aviation Medicine, unusual conditions in orbital and space U. S. Naval Aviation Medical Center flight Pensacola, Florida 32512 'Thomas I. Monahan, PhD Head, Physics Branch (Code 9410) U. S. Nava) Applied Science Laboratory Naval Base, Brooklyn, New York 11251 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 t t 1 I Helen Paulson, PhD U. S. Naval Submarine Medical Center `J. S. Naval Submarine Base, New London Groton, Connecticut Ob342 Capt. Neal D. Sanborn, MC, USN Head, Aviation Physical Qualifications Or. Aviation Medical Operations Division Bureau of Medicine and Surgery--511 Navy Department Washington, D. C. 20390 Richard Tousey, PhD Code 7140 U. S. Naval Research Laboratory Washington, D. C. Richard Trumbull, PhD Office of Naval Research Code 450 Washington, D.. C. Capt. Ralph L. Vasa (MSC) USN Code 3121 Bureau of Medicine and Surgery Washington, D. C. Visual standards, protective devices, visual performance Capt. H. G. Wagner, MC, USN Director Aerospace Crew Equipment Laboratory Naval Air Engineering Center Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 19112 Carroll White, PhD Code 3380 U. S. Naval Electronics Laboratory San Diego, California Temporal factors in vision, eye movement studies, visually evoked cortical potentials and the electroretinogram Myron L. Wolbarsht, PhD Naval Medical Research institute Bethesda, Maryland 20014 Color vision and organization of the retina, comparative physiology and bio- physics of photoreceptors and associated neural elements, electro-physiology of visual system FEDERAL AVIATION AGENCY Roland H. S. Bedell, MD Georgetown Clinical Research Inst. FAA, AM-130 3800 Reservoir Road Washington, D. C. Ophthalmology as applied to aviation- glaucoma, vascular changes in eye, visual standards 13 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 .William Collins Color vision, color blindness, psycho- civil Aeromedical Research Institute physics and interaction of the visual :ederal Aviation Agency and vestibular systems Aeronautical Center P. 0. Box 1082 Oklahoma City, Oklahoma VATIONAL AERONAUTICS AND SPACE ADMINISTRATION Stanley Deutsch, PhD Visual skills in aircraft and space Chief, Man-Systems integration systems, visual performance measurement, Biotechnology and Human Research psycho-physical methods Office of Advanced Research & Technology "-Iqs. NASA 4ashington, D. C. 20546 (Alternate to Dr. Jones) William H. Allen Mission Analysis Division NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field,.California Visual problems of operational space flight, protective equipment John Billingham, PhD NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field, California Siegfried Gerathewohl, PhD Manned Space Division office of Space Science & Applications NASA Headquarters Washington, D. C. John D. Hilchey, PhD Marshall Space Flight Center Huntsville, Alabama 35812 Robert Jones, PhD Manned Spacecraft Center Houston, Texas 77058 R. Mark Patton, PhD NASA Ames Research Center Moffett Field, California 94035 Arthur W. Vogeley Langley Research Center Hampton, Virginia 23365 Roger L. Winblade Code REC NASA Headquarters Washington, D. C. 14 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78B04770A000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 I I. Wdg/Cdr. William Allan Crawford Royal Air Force Staff British Embassy Washington, D. C. 20008 Mr. C. L. Crouch Illuminating Engineering Society United Engineering Center 345 East 47th Street New York, New York Dr. Charles A. Douglas Photometry & Colorimetry Section National Bureau of Standards Washington, D. C. Dr. D. F. Downing Defence Research Staff British Embassy 3100 Massachusetts Avenue, N. W. Washington, D. C. Dr'. Richard E. Hoover, MD Veterans Administration 14 W. Mt. Vernon Place Baltimore, Maryland Dr. James H. Allen Department of Ophthalmology Tulane Medical School 1430 Tulane Avenue New Orleans, Louisiana Dr. Stanley S. Ballard Department of Physics University of Florida Gainesville, Florida Dr. Robert M. Boynton Department of Psychology University of Rochester Rochester, New York Dr. Hermann M. Burian University Hospitals Iowa City, Iowa Dr. Victor A. Byrnes 234 Beach Drive, N. E. St. Petersburg, Florida LI Al SON Dr. J. Clement McCulloch 830 Medical Arts Building 170 St. George Street Toronto 5, Ontario, Canada Lt. Cdr. John M. O'Connell, Jr., USCG Testing and Development Division Headquarters U. S. Coast Guard 1300 E Street, N. W. Washington, D. C. 5g. Cdr. H. D. Oliver Medical Liaison Officer Canadian Defence Liaison Staff Washington, D. C. 20008 Dr. Ludwig von Sallman National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Blindness National Institutes of Health Bethesda, Maryland Dr. Mary Warga Optical Society of America, 1155 - 16th Street, N. W. Washington, D. C. Prof. Alphonse Chapanis Department of Psychology Johns Hopkins University Baltimore, Maryland Dr. Frederick Crescitelli Department of Zoology University of California Los Angeles, California Dr. Mason N. Crook Institute for Applied Experimental Psychology Tufts University Medford, Massachusetts Dr. Russell DeValois Department of Psychology University of Indiana Bloomington, Indiana Dr. Stanley Diamond 490 Post Street San Francisco, California 94102 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 1 )r. Forrest L. Dimmick i Mine Street New Brunswick, New Jersey 08901 Dr. Glen Finch AFOSR ndependence & Sixth Street, S. W. Washington, D. C. Dr. Gerald Fonda 551 Millburn Avenue Short Hills, New Jersey 07078 Jr. Charles S. Gersoni A PA 1200 - 17th Street, N. W. Washington, D. C. 20036 Dr. James J. Gibson Department of Psychology Morrill Hall Cornell University Ithaca, New York Jr. Walter Gogel Department of Psychology University of California Santa Barbara, California Dr. Clarence H. Graham Department of Psychology Columbia University New York, New York Dr. Randall M. Hanes Applied Physics Laboratory Johns Hopkins University 8621 Georgia Avenue Silver Spring, Maryland Dr. William M. Hart Director, Eye Research Foundation 3710 Old Georgetown Road Bethesda, Maryland 20014 Dr. Albert E. Hickey, Jr. President, ENTELEK, Inc. 65 State Street Newburyport, Massachusetts Prof. Charles W. Hill Delta Regional Primate Research Center Covington, Louisiana 70433 Or. J. Harry Hi l l NASA Electronics Research Center qnt r,-.n M7ac~r ti..~^atS Mr. John M. Hood, Jr. U. S. Navy Electronics Laboratory Code 2122 San Diego, California Dr. Leo H. Hurvich Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Maj. Gen. Aubrey L. Jennings, USAF, (MC), (Ret.) 3733 N. Oakland Street Arlington, Virginia Dr. E. Parker Johnson Dean of the Faculty Colby College Waterville, Maine Dr. Deane B. Judd Photometry and Colorimetry Section National Bureau of Standards Washington, 0. C. Dr. John L. Kennedy Department of Psychology -Princeton University Princeton, New Jersey Dr. N. C. Kephart Department of Psychology Dr. H. Keffer Hartline Purdue University Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research Lafayette, Indiana 66th Street and York Avenue New York, New York Dr. Harry Helson Department of Psychology Kansas State University Manhattan, Kansas Dr. Heinrich Kluver Division of Biological Sciences University of Chicago Culver Hall Chicago, Illinois Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 I Dr. Henry A. Knoll tiophysics Department F.esearch and Development Division Bausch and Lomb, Inc. Rochester, New York Dr. Hedwig S. Kuhn 7142 Hohman Avenue Hammond, Indiana Dr. Elek Ludvigh Kresge Eye institute 690 Mullet Street t,etroit, Michigan Dr. Edward F. MacNichol, Jr. Department of Biophysics Johns Hopkins University Baltimore, Maryland Dr. Walter R. Miles Box 100, Medical Research Laboratory USN Submarine Base Groton, Connecticut Dr. Brian O'Brien P. 0. Box 117 Pomfret, Connecticut Dr. James O'Rourke Department of ophthalmology 3800 Reservoir Road Georgetown Hospital Washington, D. C. Dr. Floyd Ratliff Kockefeller Institute for Med. Research 66th Street and York Avenue New York, New York Dr. Lorrin A. Riggs Department of Psychology Brown University Providence, Rhode Island Dr. Charles W. Shilling Biological Sciences Communications Proj. The George Washington University 2000 P Street, N. W. Washington, D. C. Dr. Louise L. Sloan Laboratory of Physiological Optics Wilmer Institute The Johns Hopkins Medical School Baltimore, Maryland Approved Dr. F. Dow Smith Director, Optics Division ITEK Corporation 10 Maguire Road Lexington, Massachusetts Dr. Philip 1. Sperling Special Operations Research Office American University 5010 Wisconsin Avenue, N. W. Washington, D. C. Prof. Everett M. Strong Department of Electrical Engineering 110 Phillips Hail Cornell University Ithaca, New York Dr. Kenneth Swan University of Oregon Medical School Department of Ophthalmology Portland, Oregon Dr. Wilson P. Tanner, Jr. Department of Electrical Engineering Cooley Electronics Laboratory University of Michigan Ann Arbor, Michigan Dr. James M. Vanderplas Department of Psychology Washington University St. Louis, Missouri Dr. George Wald Biological Labs. A-302 Harvard University Cambridge, Massachusetts Dr. Ralph Wick American Academy of optometry 810 Mt. View Rapid City, South Dakota Dr. Ernst Wolf Retina Foundation 20 Stanford Street Boston, Massachusetts Dr. Benjamin J. Wolpaw 2323 Prospect Avenue Cleveland, Ohio Dr. Joseph W. Wulfeck Dunlap and Associates, Inc. 1454 Cloverfield Boulevard Santa Monica, California 17 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 INTERNATIONAL CORRESPONDENTS 1 Dr. M. Aguilar Instituto de Optica ;errano, 119 Madrid, Spain Prof. A. Arnulf Institut d'Optique 3 Boulevard Pasteur Paris XV, France )r. Edgar Auer ba.:i1 Hebrew University Hadassah Medical School P. 0. Box 1 172 Jerusalem, Israel ')r. E. Baumgardt O;recteur de Recherche College de France 11,. Place Marcelin-Berthelot Paris-.V, France Prof. Dr. M. A. Bouman Director, Institute for Perception National Defense Research Organization TNO Kampweg 5 Soesterberg, Netherlands Dr. B. H. Crawford National Physical Laboratory Teddington, Middlesex, England Prof. R. W. Ditchburn, F.R.S. J. J. Thomson Physics Laboratory Whiteknights Park University of Reading Reading, Berkshire, England Prof. Gosta Ekman The Psychological Lab. Teknologgatan 8 Stockholm VA, Sweden Dr. Adriana Fiorentini Instituto Nazionale di Ottica Via S. Leonardo 79 Arcetri, Florence, Italy Prof. R. G. Hopkinson Bartlett School of Architecture University College London Gower Street London, W.C.I. England Prof. Yves le Grand Musee d'Histoire Naturelle 57 Rue Cuvier Paris 5. France Prof. Erik P. G. Ingelstam Head, Institute for Optical Research Royal Swedish Institute of Technology Stockholm 70, Sweden Dr. KU iLi Motokawa Professor, Department of Physiology Tohoku University School of Medicine Kitayobancno Sendai, Japan Prof. R. W. Pic:kford Department of Psychology Glasgow University Glasgow, Scotland Dr. M. H. Pirenne Department of Physiology Oxford University Oxford, England Dr. M. Richter Unter den Eichen 87 Berlin-Dahlem (Western sector) Germany Prof. Dr. H. Schober Institute for Medical Optics University of Munich Arnulfstrasse 205 Munich , 9, Germany Dr. J. F. Schouten -Inst. voor Perceptie Onderzoek Insulindelaan 2 Eindhoven, The Netherlands Dr. W. M. Vaidya Optics Div., N.P.L. Hillside Road New Delhi 12, India Dr. G. Verriest 79 Coupure Ghent, Belgium Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6 Dr. Pieter L. Walraven Dep. Director Institute for Perception National Defense Research Organization TNO Kampweg 5 Soesterberg, Netherlands Dr. R. A. Weale Department of Physiological Optics Medical Research Council Institute of Ophthalmology Judd Street London, W.C. 1. England Wg/Cdr. T. C. D. Whiteside, RAF institute of Aviation Medicine Farnborough, Hampshire, England 19 Approved For Release 2004/11/30 : CIA-RDP78BO477OA000900040031-6